• Subject List
  • Take a Tour
  • For Authors
  • Subscriber Services
  • Publications
  • African American Studies
  • African Studies
  • American Literature
  • Anthropology
  • Architecture Planning and Preservation
  • Art History
  • Atlantic History
  • Biblical Studies
  • British and Irish Literature
  • Childhood Studies
  • Chinese Studies
  • Cinema and Media Studies
  • Communication
  • Criminology
  • Environmental Science
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • International Law
  • International Relations
  • Islamic Studies
  • Jewish Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Latino Studies
  • Linguistics
  • Literary and Critical Theory
  • Medieval Studies
  • Military History
  • Political Science
  • Public Health
  • Renaissance and Reformation
  • Social Work
  • Urban Studies
  • Victorian Literature
  • Browse All Subjects

How to Subscribe

  • Free Trials

In This Article Expand or collapse the "in this article" section Problem Solving and Decision Making

Introduction.

  • General Approaches to Problem Solving
  • Representational Accounts
  • Problem Space and Search
  • Working Memory and Problem Solving
  • Domain-Specific Problem Solving
  • The Rational Approach
  • Prospect Theory
  • Dual-Process Theory
  • Cognitive Heuristics and Biases

Related Articles Expand or collapse the "related articles" section about

About related articles close popup.

Lorem Ipsum Sit Dolor Amet

Vestibulum ante ipsum primis in faucibus orci luctus et ultrices posuere cubilia Curae; Aliquam ligula odio, euismod ut aliquam et, vestibulum nec risus. Nulla viverra, arcu et iaculis consequat, justo diam ornare tellus, semper ultrices tellus nunc eu tellus.

  • Artificial Intelligence, Machine Learning, and Psychology
  • Counterfactual Reasoning
  • Critical Thinking
  • Heuristics and Biases
  • Protocol Analysis
  • Psychology and Law

Other Subject Areas

Forthcoming articles expand or collapse the "forthcoming articles" section.

  • Data Visualization
  • Executive Functions in Childhood
  • Remote Work
  • Find more forthcoming articles...
  • Export Citations
  • Share This Facebook LinkedIn Twitter

Problem Solving and Decision Making by Emily G. Nielsen , John Paul Minda LAST REVIEWED: 26 June 2019 LAST MODIFIED: 26 June 2019 DOI: 10.1093/obo/9780199828340-0246

Problem solving and decision making are both examples of complex, higher-order thinking. Both involve the assessment of the environment, the involvement of working memory or short-term memory, reliance on long term memory, effects of knowledge, and the application of heuristics to complete a behavior. A problem can be defined as an impasse or gap between a current state and a desired goal state. Problem solving is the set of cognitive operations that a person engages in to change the current state, to go beyond the impasse, and achieve a desired outcome. Problem solving involves the mental representation of the problem state and the manipulation of this representation in order to move closer to the goal. Problems can vary in complexity, abstraction, and how well defined (or not) the initial state and the goal state are. Research has generally approached problem solving by examining the behaviors and cognitive processes involved, and some work has examined problem solving using computational processes as well. Decision making is the process of selecting and choosing one action or behavior out of several alternatives. Like problem solving, decision making involves the coordination of memories and executive resources. Research on decision making has paid particular attention to the cognitive biases that account for suboptimal decisions and decisions that deviate from rationality. The current bibliography first outlines some general resources on the psychology of problem solving and decision making before examining each of these topics in detail. Specifically, this review covers cognitive, neuroscientific, and computational approaches to problem solving, as well as decision making models and cognitive heuristics and biases.

General Overviews

Current research in the area of problem solving and decision making is published in both general and specialized scientific journals. Theoretical and scholarly work is often summarized and developed in full-length books and chapter. These may focus on the subfields of problem solving and decision making or the larger field of thinking and higher-order cognition.

back to top

Users without a subscription are not able to see the full content on this page. Please subscribe or login .

Oxford Bibliographies Online is available by subscription and perpetual access to institutions. For more information or to contact an Oxford Sales Representative click here .

  • About Psychology »
  • Meet the Editorial Board »
  • Abnormal Psychology
  • Academic Assessment
  • Acculturation and Health
  • Action Regulation Theory
  • Action Research
  • Addictive Behavior
  • Adolescence
  • Adoption, Social, Psychological, and Evolutionary Perspect...
  • Advanced Theory of Mind
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Affirmative Action
  • Ageism at Work
  • Allport, Gordon
  • Alzheimer’s Disease
  • Ambulatory Assessment in Behavioral Science
  • Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA)
  • Animal Behavior
  • Animal Learning
  • Anxiety Disorders
  • Art and Aesthetics, Psychology of
  • Assessment and Clinical Applications of Individual Differe...
  • Attachment in Social and Emotional Development across the ...
  • Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in Adults
  • Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in Childre...
  • Attitudinal Ambivalence
  • Attraction in Close Relationships
  • Attribution Theory
  • Authoritarian Personality
  • Bayesian Statistical Methods in Psychology
  • Behavior Therapy, Rational Emotive
  • Behavioral Economics
  • Behavioral Genetics
  • Belief Perseverance
  • Bereavement and Grief
  • Biological Psychology
  • Birth Order
  • Body Image in Men and Women
  • Bystander Effect
  • Categorical Data Analysis in Psychology
  • Childhood and Adolescence, Peer Victimization and Bullying...
  • Clark, Mamie Phipps
  • Clinical Neuropsychology
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Consistency Theories
  • Cognitive Dissonance Theory
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Communication, Nonverbal Cues and
  • Comparative Psychology
  • Competence to Stand Trial: Restoration Services
  • Competency to Stand Trial
  • Computational Psychology
  • Conflict Management in the Workplace
  • Conformity, Compliance, and Obedience
  • Consciousness
  • Coping Processes
  • Correspondence Analysis in Psychology
  • Counseling Psychology
  • Creativity at Work
  • Cross-Cultural Psychology
  • Cultural Psychology
  • Daily Life, Research Methods for Studying
  • Data Science Methods for Psychology
  • Data Sharing in Psychology
  • Death and Dying
  • Deceiving and Detecting Deceit
  • Defensive Processes
  • Depressive Disorders
  • Development, Prenatal
  • Developmental Psychology (Cognitive)
  • Developmental Psychology (Social)
  • Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM...
  • Discrimination
  • Dissociative Disorders
  • Drugs and Behavior
  • Eating Disorders
  • Ecological Psychology
  • Educational Settings, Assessment of Thinking in
  • Effect Size
  • Embodiment and Embodied Cognition
  • Emerging Adulthood
  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Empathy and Altruism
  • Employee Stress and Well-Being
  • Environmental Neuroscience and Environmental Psychology
  • Ethics in Psychological Practice
  • Event Perception
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Expansive Posture
  • Experimental Existential Psychology
  • Exploratory Data Analysis
  • Eyewitness Testimony
  • Eysenck, Hans
  • Factor Analysis
  • Festinger, Leon
  • Five-Factor Model of Personality
  • Flynn Effect, The
  • Forensic Psychology
  • Forgiveness
  • Friendships, Children's
  • Fundamental Attribution Error/Correspondence Bias
  • Gambler's Fallacy
  • Game Theory and Psychology
  • Geropsychology, Clinical
  • Global Mental Health
  • Habit Formation and Behavior Change
  • Health Psychology
  • Health Psychology Research and Practice, Measurement in
  • Heider, Fritz
  • History of Psychology
  • Human Factors
  • Humanistic Psychology
  • Implicit Association Test (IAT)
  • Industrial and Organizational Psychology
  • Inferential Statistics in Psychology
  • Insanity Defense, The
  • Intelligence
  • Intelligence, Crystallized and Fluid
  • Intercultural Psychology
  • Intergroup Conflict
  • International Classification of Diseases and Related Healt...
  • International Psychology
  • Interviewing in Forensic Settings
  • Intimate Partner Violence, Psychological Perspectives on
  • Introversion–Extraversion
  • Item Response Theory
  • Law, Psychology and
  • Lazarus, Richard
  • Learned Helplessness
  • Learning Theory
  • Learning versus Performance
  • LGBTQ+ Romantic Relationships
  • Lie Detection in a Forensic Context
  • Life-Span Development
  • Locus of Control
  • Loneliness and Health
  • Mathematical Psychology
  • Meaning in Life
  • Mechanisms and Processes of Peer Contagion
  • Media Violence, Psychological Perspectives on
  • Mediation Analysis
  • Memories, Autobiographical
  • Memories, Flashbulb
  • Memories, Repressed and Recovered
  • Memory, False
  • Memory, Human
  • Memory, Implicit versus Explicit
  • Memory in Educational Settings
  • Memory, Semantic
  • Meta-Analysis
  • Metacognition
  • Metaphor, Psychological Perspectives on
  • Microaggressions
  • Military Psychology
  • Mindfulness
  • Mindfulness and Education
  • Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI)
  • Money, Psychology of
  • Moral Conviction
  • Moral Development
  • Moral Psychology
  • Moral Reasoning
  • Nature versus Nurture Debate in Psychology
  • Neuroscience of Associative Learning
  • Nonergodicity in Psychology and Neuroscience
  • Nonparametric Statistical Analysis in Psychology
  • Observational (Non-Randomized) Studies
  • Obsessive-Complusive Disorder (OCD)
  • Occupational Health Psychology
  • Olfaction, Human
  • Operant Conditioning
  • Optimism and Pessimism
  • Organizational Justice
  • Parenting Stress
  • Parenting Styles
  • Parents' Beliefs about Children
  • Path Models
  • Peace Psychology
  • Perception, Person
  • Performance Appraisal
  • Personality and Health
  • Personality Disorders
  • Personality Psychology
  • Person-Centered and Experiential Psychotherapies: From Car...
  • Phenomenological Psychology
  • Placebo Effects in Psychology
  • Play Behavior
  • Positive Psychological Capital (PsyCap)
  • Positive Psychology
  • Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)
  • Prejudice and Stereotyping
  • Pretrial Publicity
  • Prisoner's Dilemma
  • Problem Solving and Decision Making
  • Procrastination
  • Prosocial Behavior
  • Prosocial Spending and Well-Being
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Psychological Literacy
  • Psychological Perspectives on Food and Eating
  • Psychology, Political
  • Psychoneuroimmunology
  • Psychophysics, Visual
  • Psychotherapy
  • Psychotic Disorders
  • Publication Bias in Psychology
  • Reasoning, Counterfactual
  • Rehabilitation Psychology
  • Relationships
  • Reliability–Contemporary Psychometric Conceptions
  • Religion, Psychology and
  • Replication Initiatives in Psychology
  • Research Methods
  • Risk Taking
  • Role of the Expert Witness in Forensic Psychology, The
  • Sample Size Planning for Statistical Power and Accurate Es...
  • Schizophrenic Disorders
  • School Psychology
  • School Psychology, Counseling Services in
  • Self, Gender and
  • Self, Psychology of the
  • Self-Construal
  • Self-Control
  • Self-Deception
  • Self-Determination Theory
  • Self-Efficacy
  • Self-Esteem
  • Self-Monitoring
  • Self-Regulation in Educational Settings
  • Self-Report Tests, Measures, and Inventories in Clinical P...
  • Sensation Seeking
  • Sex and Gender
  • Sexual Minority Parenting
  • Sexual Orientation
  • Signal Detection Theory and its Applications
  • Simpson's Paradox in Psychology
  • Single People
  • Single-Case Experimental Designs
  • Skinner, B.F.
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Small Groups
  • Social Class and Social Status
  • Social Cognition
  • Social Neuroscience
  • Social Support
  • Social Touch and Massage Therapy Research
  • Somatoform Disorders
  • Spatial Attention
  • Sports Psychology
  • Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE): Icon and Controversy
  • Stereotype Threat
  • Stereotypes
  • Stress and Coping, Psychology of
  • Student Success in College
  • Subjective Wellbeing Homeostasis
  • Taste, Psychological Perspectives on
  • Teaching of Psychology
  • Terror Management Theory
  • Testing and Assessment
  • The Concept of Validity in Psychological Assessment
  • The Neuroscience of Emotion Regulation
  • The Reasoned Action Approach and the Theories of Reasoned ...
  • The Weapon Focus Effect in Eyewitness Memory
  • Theory of Mind
  • Therapy, Cognitive-Behavioral
  • Thinking Skills in Educational Settings
  • Time Perception
  • Trait Perspective
  • Trauma Psychology
  • Twin Studies
  • Type A Behavior Pattern (Coronary Prone Personality)
  • Unconscious Processes
  • Video Games and Violent Content
  • Virtues and Character Strengths
  • Women and Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math (STEM...
  • Women, Psychology of
  • Work Well-Being
  • Workforce Training Evaluation
  • Wundt, Wilhelm
  • Privacy Policy
  • Cookie Policy
  • Legal Notice
  • Accessibility

Powered by:

  • [185.148.24.167]
  • 185.148.24.167
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

What Is Cognitive Psychology?

The Science of How We Think

Topics in Cognitive Psychology

  • Current Research
  • Cognitive Approach in Practice

Careers in Cognitive Psychology

How cognitive psychology differs from other branches of psychology, frequently asked questions.

Cognitive psychology is the study of internal mental processes—all of the workings inside your brain, including perception, thinking, memory, attention, language, problem-solving, and learning. Learning about how people think and process information helps researchers and psychologists understand the human brain and assist people with psychological difficulties.

This article discusses what cognitive psychology is—its history, current trends, practical applications, and career paths.

Findings from cognitive psychology help us understand how people think, including how they acquire and store memories. By knowing more about how these processes work, psychologists can develop new ways of helping people with cognitive problems.

Cognitive psychologists explore a wide variety of topics related to thinking processes. Some of these include: 

  • Attention --our ability to process information in the environment while tuning out irrelevant details
  • Choice-based behavior --actions driven by a choice among other possibilities
  • Decision-making
  • Information processing
  • Language acquisition --how we learn to read, write, and express ourselves
  • Problem-solving
  • Speech perception -how we process what others are saying
  • Visual perception --how we see the physical world around us

History of Cognitive Psychology

Although it is a relatively young branch of psychology , it has quickly grown to become one of the most popular subfields. Cognitive psychology grew into prominence between the 1950s and 1970s.

Prior to this time, behaviorism was the dominant perspective in psychology. This theory holds that we learn all our behaviors from interacting with our environment. It focuses strictly on observable behavior, not thought and emotion. Then, researchers became more interested in the internal processes that affect behavior instead of just the behavior itself. 

This shift is often referred to as the cognitive revolution in psychology. During this time, a great deal of research on topics including memory, attention, and language acquisition began to emerge. 

In 1967, the psychologist Ulric Neisser introduced the term cognitive psychology, which he defined as the study of the processes behind the perception, transformation, storage, and recovery of information.

Cognitive psychology became more prominent after the 1950s as a result of the cognitive revolution.

Current Research in Cognitive Psychology

The field of cognitive psychology is both broad and diverse. It touches on many aspects of daily life. There are numerous practical applications for this research, such as providing help coping with memory disorders, making better decisions , recovering from brain injury, treating learning disorders, and structuring educational curricula to enhance learning.

Current research on cognitive psychology helps play a role in how professionals approach the treatment of mental illness, traumatic brain injury, and degenerative brain diseases.

Thanks to the work of cognitive psychologists, we can better pinpoint ways to measure human intellectual abilities, develop new strategies to combat memory problems, and decode the workings of the human brain—all of which ultimately have a powerful impact on how we treat cognitive disorders.

The field of cognitive psychology is a rapidly growing area that continues to add to our understanding of the many influences that mental processes have on our health and daily lives.

From understanding how cognitive processes change as a child develops to looking at how the brain transforms sensory inputs into perceptions, cognitive psychology has helped us gain a deeper and richer understanding of the many mental events that contribute to our daily existence and overall well-being.

The Cognitive Approach in Practice

In addition to adding to our understanding of how the human mind works, the field of cognitive psychology has also had an impact on approaches to mental health. Before the 1970s, many mental health treatments were focused more on psychoanalytic , behavioral , and humanistic approaches.

The so-called "cognitive revolution" put a greater emphasis on understanding the way people process information and how thinking patterns might contribute to psychological distress. Thanks to research in this area, new approaches to treatment were developed to help treat depression, anxiety, phobias, and other psychological disorders .

Cognitive behavioral therapy and rational emotive behavior therapy are two methods in which clients and therapists focus on the underlying cognitions, or thoughts, that contribute to psychological distress.

What Is Cognitive Behavioral Therapy?

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is an approach that helps clients identify irrational beliefs and other cognitive distortions that are in conflict with reality and then aid them in replacing such thoughts with more realistic, healthy beliefs.

If you are experiencing symptoms of a psychological disorder that would benefit from the use of cognitive approaches, you might see a psychologist who has specific training in these cognitive treatment methods.

These professionals frequently go by titles other than cognitive psychologists, such as psychiatrists, clinical psychologists , or counseling psychologists , but many of the strategies they use are rooted in the cognitive tradition.

Many cognitive psychologists specialize in research with universities or government agencies. Others take a clinical focus and work directly with people who are experiencing challenges related to mental processes. They work in hospitals, mental health clinics, and private practices.

Research psychologists in this area often concentrate on a particular topic, such as memory. Others work directly on health concerns related to cognition, such as degenerative brain disorders and brain injuries.

Treatments rooted in cognitive research focus on helping people replace negative thought patterns with more positive, realistic ones. With the help of cognitive psychologists, people are often able to find ways to cope and even overcome such difficulties.

Reasons to Consult a Cognitive Psychologist

  • Alzheimer's disease, dementia, or memory loss
  • Brain trauma treatment
  • Cognitive therapy for a mental health condition
  • Interventions for learning disabilities
  • Perceptual or sensory issues
  • Therapy for a speech or language disorder

Whereas behavioral and some other realms of psychology focus on actions--which are external and observable--cognitive psychology is instead concerned with the thought processes behind the behavior. Cognitive psychologists see the mind as if it were a computer, taking in and processing information, and seek to understand the various factors involved.

A Word From Verywell

Cognitive psychology plays an important role in understanding the processes of memory, attention, and learning. It can also provide insights into cognitive conditions that may affect how people function.

Being diagnosed with a brain or cognitive health problem can be daunting, but it is important to remember that you are not alone. Together with a healthcare provider, you can come up with an effective treatment plan to help address brain health and cognitive problems.

Your treatment may involve consulting with a cognitive psychologist who has a background in the specific area of concern that you are facing, or you may be referred to another mental health professional that has training and experience with your particular condition.

Ulric Neisser is considered the founder of cognitive psychology. He was the first to introduce the term and to define the field of cognitive psychology. His primary interests were in the areas of perception and memory, but he suggested that all aspects of human thought and behavior were relevant to the study of cognition.

A cognitive map refers to a mental representation of an environment. Such maps can be formed through observation as well as through trial and error. These cognitive maps allow people to orient themselves in their environment.

While they share some similarities, there are some important differences between cognitive neuroscience and cognitive psychology. While cognitive psychology focuses on thinking processes, cognitive neuroscience is focused on finding connections between thinking and specific brain activity. Cognitive neuroscience also looks at the underlying biology that influences how information is processed.

Cognitive psychology is a form of experimental psychology. Cognitive psychologists use experimental methods to study the internal mental processes that play a role in behavior.

Sternberg RJ, Sternberg K. Cognitive Psychology . Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. 

Krapfl JE. Behaviorism and society . Behav Anal. 2016;39(1):123-9. doi:10.1007/s40614-016-0063-8

Cutting JE. Ulric Neisser (1928-2012) . Am Psychol . 2012;67(6):492. doi:10.1037/a0029351

Ruggiero GM, Spada MM, Caselli G, Sassaroli S. A historical and theoretical review of cognitive behavioral therapies: from structural self-knowledge to functional processes .  J Ration Emot Cogn Behav Ther . 2018;36(4):378-403. doi:10.1007/s10942-018-0292-8

Parvin P. Ulric Neisser, cognitive psychology pioneer, dies . Emory News Center.

APA Dictionary of Psychology. Cognitive map . American Psychological Association.

Forstmann BU, Wagenmakers EJ, Eichele T, Brown S, Serences JT. Reciprocal relations between cognitive neuroscience and formal cognitive models: opposites attract? . Trends Cogn Sci . 2011;15(6):272-279. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.002

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

7.3 Problem Solving

Learning objectives.

  • Describe problem solving strategies
  • Define algorithm and heuristic
  • Explain some common roadblocks to effective problem solving

People face problems every day—usually, multiple problems throughout the day. Sometimes these problems are straightforward: To double a recipe for pizza dough, for example, all that is required is that each ingredient in the recipe be doubled. Sometimes, however, the problems we encounter are more complex. For example, say you have a work deadline, and you must mail a printed copy of a report to your supervisor by the end of the business day. The report is time-sensitive and must be sent overnight. You finished the report last night, but your printer will not work today. What should you do? First, you need to identify the problem and then apply a strategy for solving the problem.

Problem-Solving Strategies

When you are presented with a problem—whether it is a complex mathematical problem or a broken printer, how do you solve it? Before finding a solution to the problem, the problem must first be clearly identified. After that, one of many problem solving strategies can be applied, hopefully resulting in a solution.

A problem-solving strategy is a plan of action used to find a solution. Different strategies have different action plans associated with them ( Table 7.2 ). For example, a well-known strategy is trial and error . The old adage, “If at first you don’t succeed, try, try again” describes trial and error. In terms of your broken printer, you could try checking the ink levels, and if that doesn’t work, you could check to make sure the paper tray isn’t jammed. Or maybe the printer isn’t actually connected to your laptop. When using trial and error, you would continue to try different solutions until you solved your problem. Although trial and error is not typically one of the most time-efficient strategies, it is a commonly used one.

Method Description Example
Trial and error Continue trying different solutions until problem is solved Restarting phone, turning off WiFi, turning off bluetooth in order to determine why your phone is malfunctioning
Algorithm Step-by-step problem-solving formula Instruction manual for installing new software on your computer
Heuristic General problem-solving framework Working backwards; breaking a task into steps

Another type of strategy is an algorithm. An algorithm is a problem-solving formula that provides you with step-by-step instructions used to achieve a desired outcome (Kahneman, 2011). You can think of an algorithm as a recipe with highly detailed instructions that produce the same result every time they are performed. Algorithms are used frequently in our everyday lives, especially in computer science. When you run a search on the Internet, search engines like Google use algorithms to decide which entries will appear first in your list of results. Facebook also uses algorithms to decide which posts to display on your newsfeed. Can you identify other situations in which algorithms are used?

A heuristic is another type of problem solving strategy. While an algorithm must be followed exactly to produce a correct result, a heuristic is a general problem-solving framework (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). You can think of these as mental shortcuts that are used to solve problems. A “rule of thumb” is an example of a heuristic. Such a rule saves the person time and energy when making a decision, but despite its time-saving characteristics, it is not always the best method for making a rational decision. Different types of heuristics are used in different types of situations, but the impulse to use a heuristic occurs when one of five conditions is met (Pratkanis, 1989):

  • When one is faced with too much information
  • When the time to make a decision is limited
  • When the decision to be made is unimportant
  • When there is access to very little information to use in making the decision
  • When an appropriate heuristic happens to come to mind in the same moment

Working backwards is a useful heuristic in which you begin solving the problem by focusing on the end result. Consider this example: You live in Washington, D.C. and have been invited to a wedding at 4 PM on Saturday in Philadelphia. Knowing that Interstate 95 tends to back up any day of the week, you need to plan your route and time your departure accordingly. If you want to be at the wedding service by 3:30 PM, and it takes 2.5 hours to get to Philadelphia without traffic, what time should you leave your house? You use the working backwards heuristic to plan the events of your day on a regular basis, probably without even thinking about it.

Another useful heuristic is the practice of accomplishing a large goal or task by breaking it into a series of smaller steps. Students often use this common method to complete a large research project or long essay for school. For example, students typically brainstorm, develop a thesis or main topic, research the chosen topic, organize their information into an outline, write a rough draft, revise and edit the rough draft, develop a final draft, organize the references list, and proofread their work before turning in the project. The large task becomes less overwhelming when it is broken down into a series of small steps.

Everyday Connection

Solving puzzles.

Problem-solving abilities can improve with practice. Many people challenge themselves every day with puzzles and other mental exercises to sharpen their problem-solving skills. Sudoku puzzles appear daily in most newspapers. Typically, a sudoku puzzle is a 9×9 grid. The simple sudoku below ( Figure 7.8 ) is a 4×4 grid. To solve the puzzle, fill in the empty boxes with a single digit: 1, 2, 3, or 4. Here are the rules: The numbers must total 10 in each bolded box, each row, and each column; however, each digit can only appear once in a bolded box, row, and column. Time yourself as you solve this puzzle and compare your time with a classmate.

Here is another popular type of puzzle ( Figure 7.9 ) that challenges your spatial reasoning skills. Connect all nine dots with four connecting straight lines without lifting your pencil from the paper:

Take a look at the “Puzzling Scales” logic puzzle below ( Figure 7.10 ). Sam Loyd, a well-known puzzle master, created and refined countless puzzles throughout his lifetime (Cyclopedia of Puzzles, n.d.).

Pitfalls to Problem Solving

Not all problems are successfully solved, however. What challenges stop us from successfully solving a problem? Albert Einstein once said, “Insanity is doing the same thing over and over again and expecting a different result.” Imagine a person in a room that has four doorways. One doorway that has always been open in the past is now locked. The person, accustomed to exiting the room by that particular doorway, keeps trying to get out through the same doorway even though the other three doorways are open. The person is stuck—but she just needs to go to another doorway, instead of trying to get out through the locked doorway. A mental set is where you persist in approaching a problem in a way that has worked in the past but is clearly not working now.

Functional fixedness is a type of mental set where you cannot perceive an object being used for something other than what it was designed for. During the Apollo 13 mission to the moon, NASA engineers at Mission Control had to overcome functional fixedness to save the lives of the astronauts aboard the spacecraft. An explosion in a module of the spacecraft damaged multiple systems. The astronauts were in danger of being poisoned by rising levels of carbon dioxide because of problems with the carbon dioxide filters. The engineers found a way for the astronauts to use spare plastic bags, tape, and air hoses to create a makeshift air filter, which saved the lives of the astronauts.

Link to Learning

Check out this Apollo 13 scene where the group of NASA engineers are given the task of overcoming functional fixedness.

Researchers have investigated whether functional fixedness is affected by culture. In one experiment, individuals from the Shuar group in Ecuador were asked to use an object for a purpose other than that for which the object was originally intended. For example, the participants were told a story about a bear and a rabbit that were separated by a river and asked to select among various objects, including a spoon, a cup, erasers, and so on, to help the animals. The spoon was the only object long enough to span the imaginary river, but if the spoon was presented in a way that reflected its normal usage, it took participants longer to choose the spoon to solve the problem. (German & Barrett, 2005). The researchers wanted to know if exposure to highly specialized tools, as occurs with individuals in industrialized nations, affects their ability to transcend functional fixedness. It was determined that functional fixedness is experienced in both industrialized and nonindustrialized cultures (German & Barrett, 2005).

In order to make good decisions, we use our knowledge and our reasoning. Often, this knowledge and reasoning is sound and solid. Sometimes, however, we are swayed by biases or by others manipulating a situation. For example, let’s say you and three friends wanted to rent a house and had a combined target budget of $1,600. The realtor shows you only very run-down houses for $1,600 and then shows you a very nice house for $2,000. Might you ask each person to pay more in rent to get the $2,000 home? Why would the realtor show you the run-down houses and the nice house? The realtor may be challenging your anchoring bias. An anchoring bias occurs when you focus on one piece of information when making a decision or solving a problem. In this case, you’re so focused on the amount of money you are willing to spend that you may not recognize what kinds of houses are available at that price point.

The confirmation bias is the tendency to focus on information that confirms your existing beliefs. For example, if you think that your professor is not very nice, you notice all of the instances of rude behavior exhibited by the professor while ignoring the countless pleasant interactions he is involved in on a daily basis. Hindsight bias leads you to believe that the event you just experienced was predictable, even though it really wasn’t. In other words, you knew all along that things would turn out the way they did. Representative bias describes a faulty way of thinking, in which you unintentionally stereotype someone or something; for example, you may assume that your professors spend their free time reading books and engaging in intellectual conversation, because the idea of them spending their time playing volleyball or visiting an amusement park does not fit in with your stereotypes of professors.

Finally, the availability heuristic is a heuristic in which you make a decision based on an example, information, or recent experience that is that readily available to you, even though it may not be the best example to inform your decision . Biases tend to “preserve that which is already established—to maintain our preexisting knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, and hypotheses” (Aronson, 1995; Kahneman, 2011). These biases are summarized in Table 7.3 .

Bias Description
Anchoring Tendency to focus on one particular piece of information when making decisions or problem-solving
Confirmation Focuses on information that confirms existing beliefs
Hindsight Belief that the event just experienced was predictable
Representative Unintentional stereotyping of someone or something
Availability Decision is based upon either an available precedent or an example that may be faulty

Please visit this site to see a clever music video that a high school teacher made to explain these and other cognitive biases to his AP psychology students.

Were you able to determine how many marbles are needed to balance the scales in Figure 7.10 ? You need nine. Were you able to solve the problems in Figure 7.8 and Figure 7.9 ? Here are the answers ( Figure 7.11 ).

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology/pages/1-introduction
  • Authors: Rose M. Spielman, Kathryn Dumper, William Jenkins, Arlene Lacombe, Marilyn Lovett, Marion Perlmutter
  • Publisher/website: OpenStax
  • Book title: Psychology
  • Publication date: Dec 8, 2014
  • Location: Houston, Texas
  • Book URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology/pages/1-introduction
  • Section URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology/pages/7-3-problem-solving

© Feb 9, 2022 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.

Problem-Solving

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online: 01 January 2020
  • pp 4042–4045
  • Cite this reference work entry

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  • Jon C. Willford 3  

49 Accesses

Problem solving refers to the process of identifying a gap between a desired goal state and a present state, and proposing and performing a set of operations or solutions in order to move toward the goal state. Generally, the solution to the problem is not immediately known and the process can involve multiple solutions and attempts to reach the intended goal. Problem solving involves a set of cognitive processes associated with problem definition, information gathering, analyzing, planning, and execution. An individual’s capacity to problem solve is influenced by a number of factors including cognitive ability, disposition, knowledge, and background.

Introduction

Problem solving involves a set of complex cognitive processes that require thinking and reasoning. A problem occurs when there is a goal that needs to be reached and there is not a clear path to achieving the goal (Mayer 2013 ). Problems can range in terms of type, complexity, strategy use, domain, and other...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22 (2), 227–257.

Google Scholar  

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Elliott, T. R., Herrick, S. M., MacNair, R. R., & Harkins, S. W. (1994). Personality correlates of self-appraised problem solving ability: Problem orientation and trait affectivity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63 (3), 489–505.

Article   Google Scholar  

Greiff, S., & Neubert, J. C. (2014). On the relation of complex problem solving, personality, fluid intelligence, and academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 36 , 37–48.

Mayer, R. E. (2013). Problem solving. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Neubert, J. C., Mainert, J., Kretzschmar, A., & Greiff, S. (2015). The assessment of 21st century skills in industrial and organizational psychology: Complex and collaborative problem solving. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 8 (02), 238–268.

Pretz, J. E., Naples, A. J., & Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Recognizing, defining, and representing problems. In J. E. Davidson & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of problem solving (pp. 3–30). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA

Jon C. Willford

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jon C. Willford .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Oakland University, Rochester, MI, USA

Virgil Zeigler-Hill

Todd K. Shackelford

Section Editor information

University of North Carolina-Charlotte, Charlotte, NC, USA

Charlie Reeve

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Cite this entry.

Willford, J.C. (2020). Problem-Solving. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K. (eds) Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_993

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_993

Published : 22 April 2020

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-24610-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-24612-3

eBook Packages : Behavioral Science and Psychology Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Share this entry

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

The PMC website is updating on October 15, 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection

Logo of pheelsevier

Problem solving through values: A challenge for thinking and capability development

  • • This paper introduces the 4W framework of consistent problem solving through values.
  • • The 4W suggests when, how and why the explication of values helps to solve a problem.
  • • The 4W is significant to teach students to cope with problems having crucial consequences.
  • • The paper considers challenges using such framework of thinking in different fields of education.

The paper aims to introduce the conceptual framework of problem solving through values. The framework consists of problem analysis, selection of value(s) as a background for the solution, the search for alternative ways of the solution, and the rationale for the solution. This framework reveals when, how, and why is important to think about values when solving problems. A consistent process fosters cohesive and creative value-based thinking during problem solving rather than teaching specific values. Therefore, the framework discloses the possibility for enabling the development of value-grounded problem solving capability.The application of this framework highlights the importance of responsibility for the chosen values that are the basis for the alternatives which determine actions. The 4W framework is meaningful for the people’s lives and their professional work. It is particularly important in the process of future professionals’ education. Critical issues concerning the development of problem solving through values are discussed when considering and examining options for the implementation of the 4W framework in educational institutions.

1. Introduction

The core competencies necessary for future professionals include problem solving based on complexity and collaborative approaches ( OECD, 2018 ). Currently, the emphasis is put on the development of technical, technological skills as well as system thinking and other cognitive abilities (e.g., Barber, 2018 ; Blanco, Schirmbeck, & Costa, 2018 ). Hence, education prepares learners with high qualifications yet lacking in moral values ( Nadda, 2017 ). Educational researchers (e.g., Barnett, 2007 ; Harland & Pickering, 2010 ) stress that such skills and abilities ( the how? ), as well as knowledge ( the what? ), are insufficient to educate a person for society and the world. The philosophy of education underlines both the epistemological and ontological dimensions of learning. Barnett (2007) points out that the ontological dimension has to be above the epistemological one. The ontological dimension encompasses the issues related to values that education should foster ( Harland & Pickering, 2010 ). In addition, values are closely related to the enablement of learners in educational environments ( Jucevičienė et al., 2010 ). For these reasons, ‘ the why ?’ based on values is required in the learning process. The question arises as to what values and how it makes sense to educate them. Value-based education seeks to address these issues and concentrates on values transfer due to their integration into the curriculum. Yazdani and Akbarilakeh (2017) discussed that value-based education could only convey factual knowledge of values and ethics. However, such education does not guarantee the internalization of values. Nevertheless, value-based education indicates problem solving as one of the possibilities to develop values.

Values guide and affect personal behavior encompassing the ethical aspects of solutions ( Roccas, Sagiv, & Navon, 2017 ; Schwartz, 1992 , 2012 ; Verplanken & Holland, 2002 ). Therefore, they represent the essential foundation for solving a problem. Growing evidence indicates the creative potential of values ( Dollinger, Burke, & Gump, 2007 ; Kasof, Chen, Himsel, & Greenberger, 2007 ; Lebedeva et al., 2019) and emphasizes their significance for problem solving. Meanwhile, research in problem solving pays little attention to values. Most of the problem solving models (e.g., Newell & Simon, 1972 ; Jonassen, 1997 ) utilize a rational economic approach. Principally, the research on the mechanisms of problem solving have been conducted under laboratory conditions performing simple tasks ( Csapó & Funke, 2017 ). Moreover, some of the decision-making models share the same steps as problem solving (c.f., Donovan, Guss, & Naslund, 2015 ). This explains why these terms are sometimes used interchangeably ( Huitt, 1992 ). Indeed, decision-making is a part of problem solving, which emerges while choosing between alternatives. Yet, values, moral, and ethical issues are more common in decision-making research (e.g., Keeney, 1994 ; Verplanken & Holland, 2002 ; Hall & Davis, 2007 ; Sheehan & Schmidt, 2015 ). Though, research by Shepherd, Patzelt, and Baron (2013) , Baron, Zhao, and Miao (2015) has affirmed that contemporary business decision makers rather often leave aside ethical issues and moral values. Thus, ‘ethical disengagement fallacy’ ( Sternberg, 2017, p.7 ) occurs as people think that ethics is more relevant to others. In the face of such disengagement, ethical issues lose their prominence.

The analysis of the literature revealed a wide field of problem solving research presenting a range of more theoretical insights rather empirical evidence. Despite this, to date, a comprehensive model that reveals how to solve problems emphasizing thinking about values is lacking. This underlines the relevance of the chosen topic, i.e. a challenge for thinking and for the development of capabilities addressing problems through values. To address this gap, the following issues need to be investigated: When, how, and why a problem solver should take into account values during problem solving? What challenges may occur for using such framework of thinking in different fields of education? Aiming this, the authors of the paper substantiated the conceptual framework of problem solving grounded in consistent thinking about values. The substantiation consists of several parts. First, different approaches to solving problems were examined. Second, searching to reveal the possibilities of values integration into problem solving, value-based approaches significant for problem solving were critically analyzed. Third, drawing on the effect of values when solving a problem and their creative potential, the authors of this paper claim that the identification of values and their choice for a solution need to be specified in the process of problem solving. As a synthesis of conclusions coming from the literature review and conceptual extensions regarding values, the authors of the paper created the coherent framework of problem solving through values (so called 4W).

The novelty of the 4W framework is exposed by several contributions. First, the clear design of overall problem solving process with attention on integrated thinking about values is used. Unlike in most models of problem solving, the first stage encompass the identification of a problem, an analysis of a context and the perspectives that influence the whole process, i.e. ‘What?’. The stage ‘What is the basis for a solution?’ focus on values identification and their choice. The stage ‘Ways how?’ encourages to create alternatives considering values. The stage ‘Why?’ represent justification of a chosen alternative according particular issues. Above-mentioned stages including specific steps are not found in any other model of problem solving. Second, even two key stages nurture thinking about values. The specificity of the 4W framework allows expecting its successful practical application. It may help to solve a problem more informed revealing when and how the explication of values helps to reach the desired value-based solution. The particular significance is that the 4W framework can be used to develop capabilities to solve problems through values. The challenges to use the 4W framework in education are discussed.

2. Methodology

To create the 4W framework, the integrative literature review was chosen. According to Snyder (2019) , this review is ‘useful when the purpose of the review is not to cover all articles ever published on the topic but rather to combine perspectives to create new theoretical models’ (p.334). The scope of this review focused on research disclosing problem solving process that paid attention on values. The following databases were used for relevant information search: EBSCO/Hostdatabases (ERIC, Education Source), Emerald, Google Scholar. The first step of this search was conducted using integrated keywords problem solving model , problem solving process, problem solving steps . These keywords were combined with the Boolean operator AND with the second keywords values approach, value-based . The inclusion criteria were used to identify research that: presents theoretical backgrounds and/or empirical evidences; performed within the last 5 years; within an educational context; availability of full text. The sources appropriate for this review was very limited in scope (N = 2).

We implemented the second search only with the same set of the integrated keywords. The inclusion criteria were the same except the date; this criterion was extended up to 10 years. This search presented 85 different sources. After reading the summaries, introductions and conclusions of the sources found, the sources that do not explicitly provide the process/models/steps of problem solving for teaching/learning purposes and eliminates values were excluded. Aiming to see a more accurate picture of the chosen topic, we selected secondary sources from these initial sources.

Several important issues were determined as well. First, most researchers ground their studies on existing problem solving models, however, not based on values. Second, some of them conducted empirical research in order to identify the process of studies participants’ problem solving. Therefore, we included sources without date restrictions trying to identify the principal sources that reveal the process/models/steps of problem solving. Third, decision-making is a part of problem solving process. Accordingly, we performed a search with the additional keywords decision-making AND values approach, value-based decision-making . We used such inclusion criteria: presents theoretical background and/or empirical evidence; no date restriction; within an educational context; availability of full text. These all searches resulted in a total of 16 (9 theoretical and 7 empirical) sources for inclusion. They were the main sources that contributed most fruitfully for the background. We used other sources for the justification the wholeness of the 4W framework. We present the principal results of the conducted literature review in the part ‘The background of the conceptual framework’.

3. The background of the conceptual framework

3.1. different approaches of how to solve a problem.

Researchers from different fields focus on problem solving. As a result, there still seems to be a lack of a conventional definition of problem solving. Regardless of some differences, there is an agreement that problem solving is a cognitive process and one of the meaningful and significant ways of learning ( Funke, 2014 ; Jonassen, 1997 ; Mayer & Wittrock, 2006 ). Differing in approaches to solving a problem, researchers ( Collins, Sibthorp, & Gookin, 2016 ; Jonassen, 1997 ; Litzinger et al., 2010 ; Mayer & Wittrock, 2006 ; O’Loughlin & McFadzean, 1999 ; ect.) present a variety of models that differ in the number of distinct steps. What is similar in these models is that they stress the procedural process of problem solving with the focus on the development of specific skills and competences.

For the sake of this paper, we have focused on those models of problem solving that clarify the process and draw attention to values, specifically, on Huitt (1992) , Basadur, Ellspermann, and Evans (1994) , and Morton (1997) . Integrating the creative approach to problem solving, Newell and Simon (1972) presents six phases: phase 1 - identifying the problem, phase 2 - understanding the problem, phase 3 - posing solutions, phase 4 - choosing solutions, phase 5 - implementing solutions, and phase 6 - final analysis. The weakness of this model is that these phases do not necessarily follow one another, and several can coincide. However, coping with simultaneously occurring phases could be a challenge, especially if these are, for instance, phases five and six. Certainly, it may be necessary to return to the previous phases for further analysis. According to Basadur et al. (1994) , problem solving consists of problem generation, problem formulation, problem solving, and solution implementation stages. Huitt (1992) distinguishes four stages in problem solving: input, processing, output, and review. Both Huitt (1992) and Basadur et al. (1994) four-stage models emphasize a sequential process of problem solving. Thus, problem solving includes four stages that are used in education. For example, problem-based learning employs such stages as introduction of the problem, problem analysis and learning issues, discovery and reporting, solution presentation and evaluation ( Chua, Tan, & Liu, 2016 ). Even PISA 2012 framework for problem solving composes four stages: exploring and understanding, representing and formulating, planning and executing, monitoring and reflecting ( OECD, 2013 ).

Drawing on various approaches to problem solving, it is possible to notice that although each stage is named differently, it is possible to reveal some general steps. These steps reflect the essential idea of problem solving: a search for the solution from the initial state to the desirable state. The identification of a problem and its contextual elements, the generation of alternatives to a problem solution, the evaluation of these alternatives according to specific criteria, the choice of an alternative for a solution, the implementation, and monitoring of the solution are the main proceeding steps in problem solving.

3.2. Value-based approaches relevant for problem solving

Huitt (1992) suggests that important values are among the criteria for the evaluation of alternatives and the effectiveness of a chosen solution. Basadur et al. (1994) point out to visible values in the problem formulation. Morton (1997) underlines that interests, investigation, prevention, and values of all types, which may influence the process, inspire every phase of problem solving. However, the aforementioned authors do not go deeper and do not seek to disclose the significance of values for problem solving.

Decision-making research shows more possibilities for problem solving and values integration. Sheehan and Schmidt (2015) model of ethical decision-making includes moral sensitivity, moral judgment, moral motivation, and moral action where values are presented in the component of moral motivation. Another useful approach concerned with values comes from decision-making in management. It is the concept of Value-Focused Thinking (VFT) proposed by Keeney (1994) . The author argues that the goals often are merely means of achieving results in traditional models of problem solving. Such models frequently do not help to identify logical links between the problem solving goals, values, and alternatives. Thus, according to Keeney (1994) , the decision-making starts with values as they are stated in the goals and objectives of decision-makers. VFT emphasizes the core values of decision-makers that are in a specific context as well as how to find a way to achieve them by using means-ends analysis. The weakness of VFT is its restriction to this means-ends analysis. According to Shin, Jonassen, and McGee (2003) , in searching for a solution, such analysis is weak as the problem solver focuses simply on removing inadequacies between the current state and the goal state. The strengths of this approach underline that values are included in the decision before alternatives are created. Besides, values help to find creative and meaningful alternatives and to assess them. Further, they include the forthcoming consequences of the decision. As VFT emphasizes the significant function of values and clarifies the possibilities of their integration into problem solving, we adapt this approach in the current paper.

3.3. The effect of values when solving a problem

In a broader sense, values provide a direction to a person’s life. Whereas the importance of values is relatively stable over time and across situations, Roccas et al. (2017) argue that values differ in their importance to a person. Verplanken and Holland (2002) investigated the relationship between values and choices or behavior. The research revealed that the activation of a value and the centrality of a value to the self, are the essential elements for value-guided behavior. The activation of values could happen in such cases: when values are the primary focus of attention; if the situation or the information a person is confronted with implies values; when the self is activated. The centrality of a particular value is ‘the degree to which an individual has incorporated this value as part of the self’ ( Verplanken & Holland, 2002, p.436 ). Thus, the perceived importance of values and attention to them determine value-guided behavior.

According to Argandoña (2003) , values can change due to external (changing values in the people around, in society, changes in situations, etc.) and internal (internalization by learning) factors affecting the person. The research by Hall and Davis (2007) indicates that the decision-makers’ applied value profile temporarily changed as they analyzed the issue from multiple perspectives and revealed the existence of a broader set of values. The study by Kirkman (2017) reveal that participants noticed the relevance of moral values to situations they encountered in various contexts.

Values are tightly related to personal integrity and identity and guide an individual’s perception, judgment, and behavior ( Halstead, 1996 ; Schwartz, 1992 ). Sheehan and Schmidt (2015) found that values influenced ethical decision-making of accounting study programme students when they uncovered their own values and grounded in them their individual codes of conduct for future jobs. Hence, the effect of values discloses by observing the problem solver’s decision-making. The latter observations could explain the abundance of ethics-laden research in decision-making rather than in problem solving.

Contemporary researchers emphasize the creative potential of values. Dollinger et al. (2007) , Kasof et al. (2007) , Lebedeva, Schwartz, Plucker, & Van De Vijver, 2019 present to some extent similar findings as they all used Schwartz Value Survey (respectively: Schwartz, 1992 ; ( Schwartz, 1994 ), Schwartz, 2012 ). These studies disclosed that such values as self-direction, stimulation and universalism foster creativity. Kasof et al. (2007) focused their research on identified motivation. Stressing that identified motivation is the only fully autonomous type of external motivation, authors define it as ‘the desire to commence an activity as a means to some end that one greatly values’ (p.106). While identified motivation toward specific values (italic in original) fosters the search for outcomes that express those specific values, this research demonstrated that it could also inhibit creative behavior. Thus, inhibition is necessary, especially in the case where reckless creativity could have painful consequences, for example, when an architect creates a beautiful staircase without a handrail. Consequently, creativity needs to be balanced.

Ultimately, values affect human beings’ lives as they express the motivational goals ( Schwartz, 1992 ). These motivational goals are the comprehensive criteria for a person’s choices when solving problems. Whereas some problem solving models only mention values as possible evaluation criteria, but they do not give any significant suggestions when and how the problem solver could think about the values coming to the understanding that his/her values direct the decision how to solve the problem. The authors of this paper claim that the identification of personal values and their choice for a solution need to be specified in the process of problem solving. This position is clearly reflected in humanistic philosophy and psychology ( Maslow, 2011 ; Rogers, 1995 ) that emphasize personal responsibility for discovering personal values through critical questioning, honest self-esteem, self-discovery, and open-mindedness in the constant pursuit of the truth in the path of individual life. However, fundamental (of humankind) and societal values should be taken into account. McLaughlin (1997) argues that a clear boundary between societal and personal values is difficult to set as they are intertwined due to their existence in complex cultural, social, and political contexts at a particular time. A person is related to time and context when choosing values. As a result, a person assumes existing values as implicit knowledge without as much as a consideration. This is particularly evident in the current consumer society.

Moreover, McLaughlin (1997) stresses that if a particular action should be tolerated and legitimated by society, it does not mean that this action is ultimately morally acceptable in all respects. Education has possibilities to reveal this. One such possibility is to turn to the capability approach ( Sen, 1990 ), which emphasizes what people are effectively able to do and to be. Capability, according to Sen (1990) , reflects a person’s freedom to choose between various ways of living, i.e., the focus is on the development of a person’s capability to choose the life he/she has a reason to value. According to Webster (2017) , ‘in order for people to value certain aspects of life, they need to appreciate the reasons and purposes – the whys – for certain valuing’ (italic in original; p.75). As values reflect and foster these whys, education should supplement the development of capability with attention to values ( Saito, 2003 ). In order to attain this possibility, a person has to be aware of and be able to understand two facets of values. Argandoña (2003) defines them as rationality and virtuality . Rationality refers to values as the ideal of conduct and involves the development of a person’s understanding of what values and why he/she should choose them when solving a problem. Virtuality approaches values as virtues and includes learning to enable a person to live according to his/her values. However, according to McLaughlin (1997) , some people may have specific values that are deep or self-evidently essential. These values are based on fundamental beliefs about the nature and purpose of the human being. Other values can be more or less superficial as they are based on giving priority to one or the other. Thus, virtuality highlights the depth of life harmonized to fundamentally rather than superficially laden values. These approaches inform the rationale for the framework of problem solving through values.

4. The 4W framework of problem solving through values

Similar to the above-presented stages of the problem solving processes, the introduced framework by the authors of this paper revisits them (see Fig. 1 ). The framework is titled 4W as its four stages respond to such questions: Analyzing the Problem: W hat ? → Choice of the value(s): W hat is the background for the solution? → Search for the alternative w ays of the solution: How ? → The rationale for problem solution: W hy is this alternative significant ? The stages of this framework cover seven steps that reveal the logical sequence of problem solving through values.

Fig. 1

The 4 W framework: problem solving through values.

Though systematic problem solving models are criticized for being linear and inflexible (e.g., Treffinger & Isaksen, 2005 ), the authors of this paper assume a structural view of the problem solving process due to several reasons. First, the framework enables problem solvers to understand the thorough process of problem solving through values. Second, this framework reveals the depth of each stage and step. Third, problem solving through values encourages tackling problems that have crucial consequences. Only by understanding and mastering the coherence of how problems those require a value-based approach need to be addressed, a problem solver will be able to cope with them in the future. Finally, this framework aims at helping to recognize, to underline personal values, to solve problems through thinking about values, and to take responsibility for choices, even value-based. The feedback supports a direct interrelation between stages. It shapes a dynamic process of problem solving through values.

The first stage of problem solving through values - ‘ The analysis of the problem: What? ’- consists of three steps (see Fig. 1 ). The first step is ‘ Recognizing the problematic situation and naming the problem ’. This step is performed in the following sequence. First, the problem solver should perceive the problematic situation he/she faces in order to understand it. Dostál (2015) argues that the problematic situation has the potential to become the problem necessary to be addressed. Although each problem is limited by its context, not every problematic situation turns into a problem. This is related to the problem solver’s capability and the perception of reality: a person may not ‘see’ the problem if his/her capability to perceive it is not developed ( Dorst, 2006 ; Dostál, 2015 ). Second, after the problem solver recognizes the existence of the problematic situation, the problem solver has to identify the presence or absence of the problem itself, i.e. to name the problem. This is especially important in the case of the ill-structured problems since they cannot be directly visible to the problem solver ( Jonassen, 1997 ). Consequently, this step allows to determine whether the problem solver developed or has acquired the capability to perceive the problematic situation and the problem (naming the problem).

The second step is ‘ Analysing the context of the problem as a reason for its rise ’. At this step, the problem solver aims to analyse the context of the problem. The latter is one of the external issues, and it determines the solution ( Jonassen, 2011 ). However, if more attention is paid to the solution of the problem, it diverts attention from the context ( Fields, 2006 ). The problem solver has to take into account both the conveyed and implied contextual elements in the problematic situation ( Dostál, 2015 ). In other words, the problem solver has to examine it through his/her ‘contextual lenses’ ( Hester & MacG, 2017 , p.208). Thus, during this step the problem solver needs to identify the elements that shape the problem - reasons and circumstances that cause the problem, the factors that can be changed, and stakeholders that are involved in the problematic situation. Whereas the elements of the context mentioned above are within the problematic situation, the problem solver can control many of them. Such control can provide unique ways for a solution.

Although the problem solver tries to predict the undesirable results, some criteria remain underestimated. For that reason, it is necessary to highlight values underlying the various possible goals during the analysis ( Fields, 2006 ). According to Hester and MacG (2017) , values express one of the main features of the context and direct the attention of the problem solver to a given problematic situation. Hence, the problem solver should explore the value-based positions that emerge in the context of the problem.

The analysis of these contextual elements focus not only on a specific problematic situation but also on the problem that has emerged. This requires setting boundaries of attention for an in-depth understanding ( Fields, 2006 ; Hester & MacG, 2017 ). Such understanding influences several actions: (a) the recognition of inappropriate aspects of the problematic situation; (b) the emergence of paths in which identified aspects are expected to change. These actions ensure consistency and safeguard against distractions. Thus, the problem solver can now recognize and identify the factors that influence the problem although they are outside of the problematic situation. However, the problem solver possesses no control over them. With the help of such context analysis, the problem solver constructs a thorough understanding of the problem. Moreover, the problem solver becomes ready to look at the problem from different perspectives.

The third step is ‘ Perspectives emerging in the problem ’. Ims and Zsolnai (2009) argue that problem solving usually contains a ‘problematic search’. Such a search is a pragmatic activity as the problem itself induces it. Thus, the problem solver searches for a superficial solution. As a result, the focus is on control over the problem rather than a deeper understanding of the problem itself. The analysis of the problem, especially including value-based approaches, reveals the necessity to consider the problem from a variety of perspectives. Mitroff (2000) builds on Linstone (1989) ideas and claims that a sound foundation of both naming and solving any problem lays in such perspectives: the technical/scientific, the interpersonal/social, the existential, and the systemic (see Table 1 ).

The main characteristics of four perspectives for problem solving

Characteristic of perspectivesTechnical/scientific perspectiveInterpersonal/social perspectiveExistential perspectiveSystemic perspective
GoalProblem solving focuses on implementation and a productAction, stability, processLives and fates of individual human beings and their life-worldsProblem within the context of a larger whole; trying to establish the nature of different relationships
Mode of inquiryModelling, data, analysisConsensual and adversaryIntuition, learning, experienceEncompass all above mentioned; connecting to the whole
Ethical basisRationalityJustice, fairnessMoralityHolistic approach
Planning horizonLong-termIntermediateShort-term and long-termLong-term, focus on the consequences
CommunicationTechnical report, briefingLanguage differs for insiders, publicPersonality importantPersonality important as a part of a whole

Whereas all problems have significant aspects of each perspective, disregarding one or another may lead to the wrong way of solving the problem. While analysing all four perspectives is essential, this does not mean that they all are equally important. Therefore, it is necessary to justify why one or another perspective is more relevant and significant in a particular case. Such analysis, according to Linstone (1989) , ‘forces us to distinguish how we are looking from what we are looking at’ (p.312; italic in original). Hence, the problem solver broadens the understanding of various perspectives and develops the capability to see the bigger picture ( Hall & Davis, 2007 ).

The problem solver aims to identify and describe four perspectives that have emerged in the problem during this step. In order to identify perspectives, the problem solver search answers to the following questions. First, regarding the technical/scientific perspective: What technical/scientific reasons are brought out in the problem? How and to what extent do they influence a problem and its context? Second, regarding the interpersonal/social perspective: What is the impact of the problem on stakeholders? How does it influence their attitudes, living conditions, interests, needs? Third, regarding the existential perspective: How does the problem affect human feelings, experiences, perception, and/or discovery of meaning? Fourth, regarding the systemic perspective: What is the effect of the problem on the person → community → society → the world? Based on the analysis of this step, the problem solver obtains a comprehensive picture of the problem. The next stage is to choose the value(s) that will address the problem.

The second stage - ‘ The choice of value(s): What is the background for the solution?’ - includes the fourth and the fifth steps. The fourth step is ‘ The identification of value(s) as a base for the solution ’. During this step, the problem solver should activate his/her value(s) making it (them) explicit. In order to do this, the problem solver proceeds several sub-steps. First, the problem solver reflects taking into account the analysis done in previous steps. He/she raises up questions revealing values that lay in the background of this analysis: What values does this analyzed context allow me to notice? What values do different perspectives of the problem ‘offer’? Such questioning is important as values are deeply hidden ( Verplanken & Holland, 2002 ) and they form a bias, which restricts the development of the capability to see from various points of view ( Hall & Paradice, 2007 ). In the 4W framework, this bias is relatively eliminated due to the analysis of the context and exploration of the perspectives of a problem. As a result, the problem solver discovers distinct value-based positions and gets an opportunity to identify the ‘value uncaptured’ ( Yang, Evans, Vladimirova, & Rana, 2017, p.1796 ) within the problem analyzed. The problem solver observes that some values exist in the context (the second step) and the disclosed perspectives (the third step). Some of the identified values do not affect the current situation as they are not required, or their potential is not exploited. Thus, looking through various value-based lenses, the problem solver can identify and discover a congruence between the opportunities offered by the values in the problem’s context, disclosed perspectives and his/her value(s). Consequently, the problem solver decides what values he/she chooses as a basis for the desired solution. Since problems usually call for a list of values, it is important to find out their order of priority. Thus, the last sub-step requires the problem solver to choose between fundamentally and superficially laden values.

In some cases, the problem solver identifies that a set of values (more than one value) can lead to the desired solution. If a person chooses this multiple value-based position, two options emerge. The first option is concerned with the analysis of each value-based position separately (from the fifth to the seventh step). In the second option, a person has to uncover which of his/her chosen values are fundamentally laden and which are superficially chosen, considering the desired outcome in the current situation. Such clarification could act as a strategy where the path for the desired solution is possible going from superficially chosen value(s) to fundamentally laden one. When a basis for the solution is established, the problem solver formulates the goal for the desired solution.

The fifth step is ‘ The formulation of the goal for the solution ’. Problem solving highlights essential points that reveal the structure of a person’s goals; thus, a goal is the core element of problem solving ( Funke, 2014 ). Meantime, values reflect the motivational content of the goals ( Schwartz, 1992 ). The attention on the chosen value not only activates it, but also motivates the problem solver. The motivation directs the formulation of the goal. In such a way, values explicitly become a basis of the goal for the solution. Thus, this step involves the problem solver in formulating the goal for the solution as the desired outcome.

The way how to take into account value(s) when formulating the goal is the integration of value(s) chosen by the problem solver in the formulation of the goal ( Keeney, 1994 ). For this purpose the conjunction of a context for a solution (it is analyzed during the second step) and a direction of preference (the chosen value reveals it) serves for the formulation of the goal (that represents the desired solution). In other words, a value should be directly included into the formulation of the goal. The goal could lose value, if value is not included into the goal formulation and remains only in the context of the goal. Let’s take the actual example concerning COVID-19 situation. Naturally, many countries governments’ preference represents such value as human life (‘it is important of every individual’s life’). Thus, most likely the particular country government’s goal of solving the COVID situation could be to save the lifes of the country people. The named problem is a complex where the goal of its solution is also complex, although it sounds simple. However, if the goal as desired outcome is formulated without the chosen value, this value remains in the context and its meaning becomes tacit. In the case of above presented example - the goal could be formulated ‘to provide hospitals with the necessary equipment and facilities’. Such goal has the value ‘human’s life’ in the context, but eliminates the complexity of the problem that leads to a partial solution of the problem. Thus, this step from the problem solver requires caution when formulating the goal as the desired outcome. For this reason, maintaining value is very important when formulating the goal’s text. To avoid the loss of values and maintain their proposed direction, is necessary to take into account values again when creating alternatives.

The third stage - ‘ Search for the alternative ways for a solution: How? ’ - encompasses the sixth step, which is called ‘ Creation of value-based alternatives ’. Frequently problem solver invokes a traditional view of problem identification, generation of alternatives, and selection of criteria for evaluating findings. Keeney (1994) ; Ims and Zsolnai (2009) criticize this rational approach as it supports a search for a partial solution where an active search for alternatives is neglected. Moreover, a problematic situation, according to Perkins (2009) , can create the illusion of a fully framed problem with some apparent weighting and some variations of choices. In this case, essential and distinct alternatives to the solution frequently become unnoticeable. Therefore, Perkins (2009) suggest to replace the focus on the attempts to comprehend the problem itself. Thinking through the ‘value lenses’ offers such opportunities. The deep understanding of the problem leads to the search for the alternative ways of a solution.

Thus, the aim of this step is for the problem solver to reveal the possible alternative ways for searching a desired solution. Most people think they know how to create alternatives, but often without delving into the situation. First of all, the problem solver based on the reflection of (but not limited to) the analysis of the context and the perspectives of the problem generates a range of alternatives. Some of these alternatives represent anchored thinking as he/she accepts the assumptions implicit in generated alternatives and with too little focus on values.

The chosen value with the formulated goal indicates direction and encourages a broader and more creative search for a solution. Hence, the problem solver should consider some of the initial alternatives that could best support the achievement of the desired solution. Values are the principles for evaluating the desirability of any alternative or outcome ( Keeney, 1994 ). Thus, planned actions should reveal the desirable mode of conduct. After such consideration, he/she should draw up a plan setting out the actions required to implement each of considered alternatives.

Lastly, after a thorough examination of each considered alternative and a plan of its implementation, the problem solver chooses one of them. If the problem solver does not see an appropriate alternative, he/she develops new alternatives. However, the problem solver may notice (and usually does) that more than one alternative can help him/her to achieve the desired solution. In this case, he/she indicates which alternative is the main one and has to be implemented in the first place, and what other alternatives and in what sequence will contribute in searching for the desired solution.

The fourth stage - ‘ The rationale for the solution: Why ’ - leads to the seventh step: ‘ The justification of the chosen alternative ’. Keeney (1994) emphasizes the compatibility of alternatives in question with the values that guide the action. This underlines the importance of justifying the choices a person makes where the focus is on taking responsibility. According to Zsolnai (2008) , responsibility means a choice, i.e., the perceived responsibility essentially determines its choice. Responsible justification allows for discovering optimal balance when choosing between distinct value-based alternatives. It also refers to the alternative solution that best reflects responsibility in a particular value context, choice, and implementation.

At this stage, the problem solver revisits the chosen solution and revises it. The problem solver justifies his/her choice based on the following questions: Why did you choose this? Why is this alternative significant looking from the technical/scientific, the interpersonal/social, the existential, and the systemic perspectives? Could you take full responsibility for the implementation of this alternative? Why? How clearly do envisaged actions reflect the goal of the desired solution? Whatever interests and for what reasons do this alternative satisfies in principle? What else do you see in the chosen alternative?

As mentioned above, each person gives priority to one aspect or another. The problem solver has to provide solid arguments for the justification of the chosen alternative. The quality of arguments, according to Jonassen (2011) , should be judged based on the quality of the evidence supporting the chosen alternative and opposing arguments that can reject solutions. Besides, the pursuit of value-based goals reflects the interests of the individual or collective interests. Therefore, it becomes critical for the problem solver to justify the level of responsibility he/she takes in assessing the chosen alternative. Such a complex evaluation of the chosen alternative ensures the acceptance of an integral rather than unilateral solution, as ‘recognizing that, in the end, people benefit most when they act for the common good’ ( Sternberg, 2012, p.46 ).

5. Discussion

The constant emphasis on thinking about values as explicit reasoning in the 4W framework (especially from the choice of the value(s) to the rationale for problem solution) reflects the pursuit of virtues. Virtues form the features of the character that are related to the choice ( Argandoña, 2003 ; McLaughlin, 2005 ). Hence, the problem solver develops value-grounded problem solving capability as the virtuality instead of employing rationality for problem solving.

Argandoña (2003) suggests that, in order to make a sound valuation process of any action, extrinsic, transcendent, and intrinsic types of motives need to be considered. They cover the respective types of values. The 4W framework meets these requirements. An extrinsic motive as ‘attaining the anticipated or expected satisfaction’ ( Argandoña, 2003, p.17 ) is reflected in the formulation of the goal of the solution, the creation of alternatives and especially in the justification of the chosen alternative way when the problem solver revisits the external effect of his/her possible action. Transcendent motive as ‘generating certain effects in others’ ( Argandoña, 2003, p.17 ) is revealed within the analysis of the context, perspectives, and creating alternatives. When the learner considers the creation of alternatives and revisits the chosen alternative, he/she pays more attention to these motives. Two types of motives mentioned so far are closely related to an intrinsic motive that emphasizes learning development within the problem solver. These motives confirm that problem solving is, in fact, lifelong learning. In light of these findings, the 4W framework is concerned with some features of value internalization as it is ‘a psychological outcome of conscious mind reasoning about values’ ( Yazdani & Akbarilakeh, 2017, p.1 ).

The 4W framework is complicated enough in terms of learning. One issue is concerned with the educational environments ( Jucevičienė, 2008 ) required to enable the 4W framework. First, the learning paradigm, rather than direct instruction, lies at the foundation of such environments. Second, such educational environments include the following dimensions: (1) educational goal; (2) learning capacity of the learners; (3) educational content relevant to the educational goal: ways and means of communicating educational content as information presented in advance (they may be real, people among them, as well as virtual); (5) methods and means of developing educational content in the process of learners’ performance; (6) physical environment relevant to the educational goal and conditions of its implementation as well as different items in the environment; (7) individuals involved in the implementation of the educational goal.

Another issue is related to exercising this framework in practice. Despite being aware of the 4W framework, a person may still not want to practice problem solving through values, since most of the solutions are going to be complicated, or may even be painful. One idea worth looking into is to reveal the extent to which problem solving through values can become a habit of mind. Profound focus on personal values, context analysis, and highlighting various perspectives can involve changes in the problem solver’s habit of mind. The constant practice of problem solving through values could first become ‘the epistemic habit of mind’ ( Mezirow, 2009, p.93 ), which means a personal way of knowing things and how to use that knowledge. This echoes Kirkman (2017) findings. The developed capability to notice moral values in situations that students encountered changed some students’ habit of mind as ‘for having “ruined” things by making it impossible not to attend to values in such situations!’ (the feedback from one student; Kirkman, 2017, p.12 ). However, this is not enough, as only those problems that require a value-based approach are addressed. Inevitably, the problem solver eventually encounters the challenges of nurturing ‘the moral-ethical habit of mind’ ( Mezirow, 2009, p.93 ). In pursuance to develop such habits of mind, the curriculum should include the necessity of the practising of the 4W framework.

Thinking based on values when solving problems enables the problem solver to engage in thoughtful reflection in contrast to pragmatic and superficial thinking supported by the consumer society. Reflection begins from the first stage of the 4W framework. As personal values are the basis for the desired solution, the problem solver is also involved in self-reflection. The conscious and continuous reflection on himself/herself and the problematic situation reinforce each step of the 4W framework. Moreover, the fourth stage (‘The rationale for the solution: Why’) involves the problem solver in critical reflection as it concerned with justification of ‘the why , the reasons for and the consequences of what we do’ (italic, bold in original; Mezirow, 1990, p.8 ). Exercising the 4W framework in practice could foster reflective practice. Empirical evidence shows that reflective practice directly impacts knowledge, skills and may lead to changes in personal belief systems and world views ( Slade, Burnham, Catalana, & Waters, 2019 ). Thus, with the help of reflective practice it is possible to identify in more detail how and to what extent the 4W framework has been mastered, what knowledge gained, capabilities developed, how point of views changed, and what influence the change process.

Critical issues related to the development of problem solving through values need to be distinguished when considering and examining options for the implementation of the 4W framework at educational institutions. First, the question to what extent can the 4W framework be incorporated into various subjects needs to be answered. Researchers could focus on applying the 4W framework to specific subjects in the humanities and social sciences. The case is with STEM subjects. Though value issues of sustainable development and ecology are of great importance, in reality STEM teaching is often restricted to the development of knowledge and skills, leaving aside the thinking about values. The special task of the researchers is to help practitioners to apply the 4W framework in STEM subjects. Considering this, researchers could employ the concept of ‘dialogic space’ ( Wegerif, 2011, p.3 ) which places particular importance of dialogue in the process of education emphasizing both the voices of teachers and students, and materials. In addition, the dimensions of educational environments could be useful aligning the 4W framework with STEM subjects. As STEM teaching is more based on solving various special tasks and/or integrating problem-based learning, the 4W framework could be a meaningful tool through which content is mastered, skills are developed, knowledge is acquired by solving pre-prepared specific tasks. In this case, the 4W framework could act as a mean addressing values in STEM teaching.

Second is the question of how to enable the process of problem solving through values. In the current paper, the concept of enabling is understood as an integral component of the empowerment. Juceviciene et al. (2010) specify that at least two perspectives can be employed to explain empowerment : a) through the power of legitimacy (according to Freire, 1996 ); and b) through the perspective of conditions for the acquisition of the required knowledge, capabilities, and competence, i.e., enabling. In this paper the 4W framework does not entail the issue of legitimacy. This issue may occur, for example, when a teacher in economics is expected to provide students with subject knowledge only, rather than adding tasks that involve problem solving through values. Yet, the issue of legitimacy is often implicit. A widespread phenomenon exists that teaching is limited to certain periods that do not have enough time for problem solving through values. The issue of legitimacy as an organizational task that supports/or not the implementation of the 4W framework in any curriculum is a question that calls for further discussion.

Third (if not the first), the issue of an educator’s competence to apply such a framework needs to be addressed. In order for a teacher to be a successful enabler, he/she should have the necessary competence. This is related to the specific pedagogical knowledge and skills, which are highly dependent on the peculiarities of the subject being taught. Nowadays actualities are encouraging to pay attention to STEM subjects and their teacher training. For researchers and teacher training institutions, who will be interested in implementing the 4W framework in STEM subjects, it would be useful to draw attention to ‘a material-dialogic approach to pedagogy’ ( Hetherington & Wegerif, 2018, p.27 ). This approach creates the conditions for a deep learning of STEM subjects revealing additional opportunities for problem solving through values in teaching. Highlighting these opportunities is a task for further research.

In contrast to traditional problem solving models, the 4W framework is more concerned with educational purposes. The prescriptive approach to teaching ( Thorne, 1994 ) is applied to the 4W framework. This approach focuses on providing guidelines that enable students to make sound decisions by making explicit value judgements. The limitation is that the 4W framework is focused on thinking but not executing. It does not include the fifth stage, which would focus on the execution of the decision how to solve the problem. This stage may contain some deviation from the predefined process of the solution of the problem.

6. Conclusions

The current paper focuses on revealing the essence of the 4W framework, which is based on enabling the problem solver to draw attention to when, how, and why it is essential to think about values during the problem solving process from the perspective of it’s design. Accordingly, the 4W framework advocates the coherent approach when solving a problem by using a creative potential of values.

The 4W framework allows the problem solver to look through the lens of his/her values twice. The first time, while formulating the problem solving goal as the desired outcome. The second time is when the problem solver looks deeper into his/her values while exploring alternative ways to solve problems. The problem solver is encouraged to reason about, find, accept, reject, compare values, and become responsible for the consequences of the choices grounded on his/her values. Thus, the problem solver could benefit from the 4W framework especially when dealing with issues having crucial consequences.

An educational approach reveals that the 4W framework could enable the development of value-grounded problem solving capability. As problem solving encourages the development of higher-order thinking skills, the consistent inclusion of values enriches them.

The 4W framework requires the educational environments for its enablement. The enablement process of problem solving through values could be based on the perspective of conditions for the acquisition of the required knowledge and capability. Continuous practice of this framework not only encourages reflection, but can also contribute to the creation of the epistemic habit of mind. Applying the 4W framework to specific subjects in the humanities and social sciences might face less challenge than STEM ones. The issue of an educator’s competence to apply such a framework is highly important. The discussed issues present significant challenges for researchers and educators. Caring that the curriculum of different courses should foresee problem solving through values, both practicing and empirical research are necessary.

Declaration of interests

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Both authors have approved the final article.

  • Argandoña A. Fostering values in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics. 2003; 45 (1–2):15–28. https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1023/A:1024164210743.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barber S. A truly “Transformative” MBA: Executive education for the fourth industrial revolution. Journal of Pedagogic Development. 2018; 8 (2):44–55. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barnett R. McGraw-Hill Education; UK): 2007. Will to learn: Being a student in an age of uncertainty. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baron R.A., Zhao H., Miao Q. Personal motives, moral disengagement, and unethical decisions by entrepreneurs: Cognitive mechanisms on the “slippery slope” Journal of Business Ethics. 2015; 128 (1):107–118. doi: 10.1007/s10551-014-2078-y. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Basadur M., Ellspermann S.J., Evans G.W. A new methodology for formulating ill-structured problems. Omega. 1994; 22 (6):627–645. doi: 10.1016/0305-0483(94)90053-1. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blanco E., Schirmbeck F., Costa C. International Conference on Remote Engineering and Virtual Instrumentation . Springer; Cham: 2018. Vocational Education for the Industrial Revolution; pp. 649–658. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chua B.L., Tan O.S., Liu W.C. Journey into the problem-solving process: Cognitive functions in a PBL environment. Innovations in Education and Teaching International. 2016; 53 (2):191–202. doi: 10.1080/14703297.2014.961502. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Collins R.H., Sibthorp J., Gookin J. Developing ill-structured problem-solving skills through wilderness education. Journal of Experiential Education. 2016; 39 (2):179–195. doi: 10.1177/1053825916639611. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Csapó B., Funke J., editors. The nature of problem solving: Using research to inspire 21st century learning. OECD Publishing; 2017. The development and assessment of problem solving in 21st-century schools. (Chapter 1). [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dollinger S.J., Burke P.A., Gump N.W. Creativity and values. Creativity Research Journal. 2007; 19 (2-3):91–103. doi: 10.1080/10400410701395028. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Donovan S.J., Guss C.D., Naslund D. Improving dynamic decision making through training and self-reflection. Judgment and Decision Making. 2015; 10 (4):284–295. http://digitalcommons.unf.edu/apsy_facpub/2 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dorst K. Design problems and design paradoxes. Design Issues. 2006; 22 (3):4–17. doi: 10.1162/desi.2006.22.3.4. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dostál J. Theory of problem solving. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences. 2015; 174 :2798–2805. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.970. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fields A.M. Ill-structured problems and the reference consultation: The librarian’s role in developing student expertise. Reference Services Review. 2006; 34 (3):405–420. doi: 10.1108/00907320610701554. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Freire P. Continuum; New York: 1996. Pedagogy of the oppressed (revised) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Funke J. Problem solving: What are the important questions?. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society; 2014. pp. 493–498. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall D.J., Davis R.A. Engaging multiple perspectives: A value-based decision-making model. Decision Support Systems. 2007; 43 (4):1588–1604. doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2006.03.004. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall D.J., Paradice D. Investigating value-based decision bias and mediation: do you do as you think? Communications of the ACM. 2007; 50 (4):81–85. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Halstead J.M. Values and values education in schools. In: Halstead J.M., Taylor M.J., editors. Values in education and education in values. The Falmer Press; London: 1996. pp. 3–14. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harland T., Pickering N. Routledge; 2010. Values in higher education teaching. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hester P.T., MacG K. Springer; New York: 2017. Systemic decision making: Fundamentals for addressing problems and messes. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hetherington L., Wegerif R. Developing a material-dialogic approach to pedagogy to guide science teacher education. Journal of Education for Teaching. 2018; 44 (1):27–43. doi: 10.1080/02607476.2018.1422611. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Huitt W. Problem solving and decision making: Consideration of individual differences using the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Journal of Psychological Type. 1992; 24 (1):33–44. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ims K.J., Zsolnai L. The future international manager. Palgrave Macmillan; London: 2009. Holistic problem solving; pp. 116–129. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jonassen D. Supporting problem solving in PBL. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-based Learning. 2011; 5 (2):95–119. doi: 10.7771/1541-5015.1256. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jonassen D.H. Instructional design models for well-structured and III-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. Educational Technology Research and Development. 1997; 45 (1):65–94. doi: 10.1007/BF02299613. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jucevičienė P. Educational and learning environments as a factor for socioeducational empowering of innovation. Socialiniai mokslai. 2008; 1 :58–70. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jucevičienė P., Gudaitytė D., Karenauskaitė V., Lipinskienė D., Stanikūnienė B., Tautkevičienė G. Technologija; Kaunas: 2010. Universiteto edukacinė galia: Atsakas XXI amžiaus iššūkiams [The educational power of university: the response to the challenges of the 21st century] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kasof J., Chen C., Himsel A., Greenberger E. Values and creativity. Creativity Research Journal. 2007; 19 (2–3):105–122. doi: 10.1080/10400410701397164. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keeney R.L. Creativity in decision making with value-focused thinking. MIT Sloan Management Review. 1994; 35 (4):33–41. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kirkman R. Problem-based learning in engineering ethics courses. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-based Learning. 2017; 11 (1) doi: 10.7771/1541-5015.1610. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lebedeva N., Schwartz S., Plucker J., Van De Vijver F. Domains of everyday creativity and personal values. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019; 9 :1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02681. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Linstone H.A. Multiple perspectives: Concept, applications, and user guidelines. Systems Practice. 1989; 2 (3):307–331. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Litzinger T.A., Meter P.V., Firetto C.M., Passmore L.J., Masters C.B., Turns S.R.…Zappe S.E. A cognitive study of problem solving in statics. Journal of Engineering Education. 2010; 99 (4):337–353. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maslow A.H. Vaga; Vilnius: 2011. Būties psichologija. [Psychology of Being] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayer R., Wittrock M. Problem solving. In: Alexander P., Winne P., editors. Handbook of educational psychology. Psychology Press; New York, NY: 2006. pp. 287–303. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLaughlin T. The educative importance of ethos. British Journal of Educational Studies. 2005; 53 (3):306–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8527.2005.00297.x. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McLaughlin T.H. Technologija; Kaunas: 1997. Šiuolaikinė ugdymo filosofija: demokratiškumas, vertybės, įvairovė [Contemporary philosophy of education: democracy, values, diversity] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mezirow J. Jossey-Bass Publishers; San Francisco: 1990. Fostering critical reflection in adulthood; pp. 1–12. https://my.liberatedleaders.com.au/wp-content/uploads/2017/02/How-Critical-Reflection-triggers-Transformative-Learning-Mezirow.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mezirow J. Contemporary theories of learning. Routledge; 2009. An overview on transformative learning; pp. 90–105. (Chapter 6) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mitroff I. Šviesa; Kaunas: 2000. Kaip neklysti šiais beprotiškais laikais: ar mokame spręsti esmines problemas. [How not to get lost in these crazy times: do we know how to solve essential problems] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morton L. Teaching creative problem solving: A paradigmatic approach. Cal. WL Rev. 1997; 34 :375. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nadda P. Need for value based education. International Education and Research Journal. 2017; 3 (2) http://ierj.in/journal/index.php/ierj/article/view/690/659 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newell A., Simon H.A. Prentice-Hall; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: 1972. Human problem solving. [ Google Scholar ]
  • OECD . PISA, OECD Publishing; Paris: 2013. PISA 2012 assessment and analytical framework: Mathematics, reading, science, problem solving and financial literacy . https://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisaproducts/PISA%202012%20framework%20e-book_final.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • OECD . PISA, OECD Publishing; 2018. PISA 2015 results in focus . https://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisa-2015-results-in-focus.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Loughlin A., McFadzean E. Toward a holistic theory of strategic problem solving. Team Performance Management: An International Journal. 1999; 5 (3):103–120. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Perkins D.N. Decision making and its development. In: Callan E., Grotzer T., Kagan J., Nisbett R.E., Perkins D.N., Shulman L.S., editors. Education and a civil society: Teaching evidence-based decision making. American Academy of Arts and Sciences; Cambridge, MA: 2009. pp. 1–28. (Chapter 1) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Roccas S., Sagiv L., Navon M. Values and behavior. Cham: Springer; 2017. Methodological issues in studying personal values; pp. 15–50. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rogers C.R. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; Boston: 1995. On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of psychotherapy. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saito M. Amartya Sen’s capability approach to education: A critical exploration. Journal of Philosophy of Education. 2003; 37 (1):17–33. doi: 10.1111/1467-9752.3701002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz S.H. Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In: Zanna M.P., editor. Vol. 25. Academic Press; 1992. pp. 1–65. (Advances in experimental social psychology). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz S.H. Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of human values? Journal of social issues. 1994; 50 (4):19–45. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz S.H. An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture. 2012; 2 (1):1–20. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1116. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sen A. Development as capability expansion. The community development reader. 1990:41–58. http://www.masterhdfs.org/masterHDFS/wp-content/uploads/2014/05/Sen-development.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sheehan N.T., Schmidt J.A. Preparing accounting students for ethical decision making: Developing individual codes of conduct based on personal values. Journal of Accounting Education. 2015; 33 (3):183–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jaccedu.2015.06.001. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shepherd D.A., Patzelt H., Baron R.A. “I care about nature, but…”: Disengaging values in assessing opportunities that cause harm. The Academy of Management Journal. 2013; 56 (5):1251–1273. doi: 10.5465/amj.2011.0776. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shin N., Jonassen D.H., McGee S. Predictors of well‐structured and ill‐structured problem solving in an astronomy simulation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 2003; 40 (1):6–33. doi: 10.1002/tea.10058. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slade M.L., Burnham T.J., Catalana S.M., Waters T. The impact of reflective practice on teacher candidates’ learning. International Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning. 2019; 13 (2):15. doi: 10.20429/ijsotl.2019.130215. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snyder H. Literature review as a research methodology: An overview and guidelines. Journal of Business Research. 2019; 104 :333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.07.039. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sternberg R. Teaching for ethical reasoning. International Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 1 (1):35–50. doi: 10.4471/ijep.2012.03. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sternberg R. Speculations on the role of successful intelligence in solving contemporary world problems. Journal of Intelligence. 2017; 6 (1):4. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence6010004. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thorne D.M. Environmental ethics in international business education: Descriptive and prescriptive dimensions. Journal of Teaching in International Business. 1994; 5 (1–2):109–122. doi: 10.1300/J066v05n01_08. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Treffinger D.J., Isaksen S.G. Creative problem solving: The history, development, and implications for gifted education and talent development. The Gifted Child Quarterly. 2005; 49 (4):342–353. doi: 10.1177/001698620504900407. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Verplanken B., Holland R.W. Motivated decision making: Effects of activation and self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002; 82 (3):434–447. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.434. [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Webster R.S. Re-enchanting education and spiritual wellbeing. Routledge; 2017. Being spiritually educated; pp. 73–85. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wegerif R. Towards a dialogic theory of how children learn to think. Thinking Skills and Creativity. 2011; 6 (3):179–190. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2011.08.002. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yang M., Evans S., Vladimirova D., Rana P. Value uncaptured perspective for sustainable business model innovation. Journal of Cleaner Production. 2017; 140 :1794–1804. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.07.102. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yazdani S., Akbarilakeh M. The model of value-based curriculum for medicine and surgery education in Iran. Journal of Minimally Invasive Surgical Sciences. 2017; 6 (3) doi: 10.5812/minsurgery.14053. [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zsolnai L. Transaction Publishers; New Brunswick and London: 2008. Responsible decision making. [ Google Scholar ]

Our systems are now restored following recent technical disruption, and we’re working hard to catch up on publishing. We apologise for the inconvenience caused. Find out more: https://www.cambridge.org/universitypress/about-us/news-and-blogs/cambridge-university-press-publishing-update-following-technical-disruption

We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings .

Login Alert

  • > The Psychology of Problem Solving
  • > Feeling and Thinking: Implications for Problem Solving

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Book contents

  • Frontmatter
  • Contributors
  • PART I INTRODUCTION
  • PART II RELEVANT ABILITIES AND SKILLS
  • PART III STATES AND STRATEGIES
  • 8 Motivating Self-Regulated Problem Solvers
  • 9 Feeling and Thinking: Implications for Problem Solving
  • 10 The Fundamental Computational Biases of Human Cognition: Heuristics That (Sometimes) Impair Decision Making and Problem Solving
  • 11 Analogical Transfer in Problem Solving
  • PART IV CONCLUSION AND INTEGRATION

9 - Feeling and Thinking: Implications for Problem Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

INTRODUCTION

Consistent with the classic juxtaposition of reason and emotion, moods and emotions have long been assumed to interfere with problem solving. Recent advances in psychology's understanding of the interplay of feeling and thinking suggest a more complex story: Positive as well as negative moods and emotions can facilitate as well as inhibit problem solving, depending on the nature of the task. Moreover, the same feeling may have differential effects at different stages of the problem-solving process. In addition, nonaffective feelings, such as bodily sensations and cognitive experiences (e.g., fluency of recall or perception), may also influence problem solving, often paralleling the effects observed for affective feelings. This chapter summarizes key lessons learned about the interplay of feeling and thinking and addresses their implications for problem solving. To set the stage, we begin with a summary of key elements of the problem-solving process.

ELEMENTS OF PROBLEM SOLVING

In the most general sense, “a problem arises when we have a goal – a state of affairs that we want to achieve – and it is not immediately apparent how the goal can be attained” (Holyoak, 1995, p. 269). Consistent with the spatial metaphors of ordinary language use, where we “search for a way to reach the goal,” “get lost” in a problem, meet “roadblocks” or have to “backtrack,” problem solving is typically conceptualized as search through a metaphorical space (Duncker, 1945).

Access options

Save book to kindle.

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle .

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service .

  • Feeling and Thinking: Implications for Problem Solving
  • By Norbert Schwarz , University of Michigan, Ian Skurnik , University of Michigan
  • Edited by Janet E. Davidson , Lewis and Clark College, Portland , Robert J. Sternberg , Yale University, Connecticut
  • Book: The Psychology of Problem Solving
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511615771.010

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox .

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive .

Logo for College of DuPage Digital Press

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

7 Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving

This module is about how a solid working knowledge of psychological principles can help you to think more effectively, so you can succeed in school and life. You might be inclined to believe that—because you have been thinking for as long as you can remember, because you are able to figure out the solution to many problems, because you feel capable of using logic to argue a point, because you can evaluate whether the things you read and hear make sense—you do not need any special training in thinking. But this, of course, is one of the key barriers to helping people think better. If you do not believe that there is anything wrong, why try to fix it?

The human brain is indeed a remarkable thinking machine, capable of amazing, complex, creative, logical thoughts. Why, then, are we telling you that you need to learn how to think? Mainly because one major lesson from cognitive psychology is that these capabilities of the human brain are relatively infrequently realized. Many psychologists believe that people are essentially “cognitive misers.” It is not that we are lazy, but that we have a tendency to expend the least amount of mental effort necessary. Although you may not realize it, it actually takes a great deal of energy to think. Careful, deliberative reasoning and critical thinking are very difficult. Because we seem to be successful without going to the trouble of using these skills well, it feels unnecessary to develop them. As you shall see, however, there are many pitfalls in the cognitive processes described in this module. When people do not devote extra effort to learning and improving reasoning, problem solving, and critical thinking skills, they make many errors.

As is true for memory, if you develop the cognitive skills presented in this module, you will be more successful in school. It is important that you realize, however, that these skills will help you far beyond school, even more so than a good memory will. Although it is somewhat useful to have a good memory, ten years from now no potential employer will care how many questions you got right on multiple choice exams during college. All of them will, however, recognize whether you are a logical, analytical, critical thinker. With these thinking skills, you will be an effective, persuasive communicator and an excellent problem solver.

The module begins by describing different kinds of thought and knowledge, especially conceptual knowledge and critical thinking. An understanding of these differences will be valuable as you progress through school and encounter different assignments that require you to tap into different kinds of knowledge. The second section covers deductive and inductive reasoning, which are processes we use to construct and evaluate strong arguments. They are essential skills to have whenever you are trying to persuade someone (including yourself) of some point, or to respond to someone’s efforts to persuade you. The module ends with a section about problem solving. A solid understanding of the key processes involved in problem solving will help you to handle many daily challenges.

7.1. Different kinds of thought

7.2. Reasoning and Judgment

7.3. Problem Solving

READING WITH PURPOSE

Remember and understand.

By reading and studying Module 7, you should be able to remember and describe:

  • Concepts and inferences (7.1)
  • Procedural knowledge (7.1)
  • Metacognition (7.1)
  • Characteristics of critical thinking:  skepticism; identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions; reasoning and problem solving skills  (7.1)
  • Reasoning:  deductive reasoning, deductively valid argument, inductive reasoning, inductively strong argument, availability heuristic, representativeness heuristic  (7.2)
  • Fixation:  functional fixedness, mental set  (7.3)
  • Algorithms, heuristics, and the role of confirmation bias (7.3)
  • Effective problem solving sequence (7.3)

By reading and thinking about how the concepts in Module 6 apply to real life, you should be able to:

  • Identify which type of knowledge a piece of information is (7.1)
  • Recognize examples of deductive and inductive reasoning (7.2)
  • Recognize judgments that have probably been influenced by the availability heuristic (7.2)
  • Recognize examples of problem solving heuristics and algorithms (7.3)

Analyze, Evaluate, and Create

By reading and thinking about Module 6, participating in classroom activities, and completing out-of-class assignments, you should be able to:

  • Use the principles of critical thinking to evaluate information (7.1)
  • Explain whether examples of reasoning arguments are deductively valid or inductively strong (7.2)
  • Outline how you could try to solve a problem from your life using the effective problem solving sequence (7.3)

7.1. Different kinds of thought and knowledge

  • Take a few minutes to write down everything that you know about dogs.
  • Do you believe that:
  • Psychic ability exists?
  • Hypnosis is an altered state of consciousness?
  • Magnet therapy is effective for relieving pain?
  • Aerobic exercise is an effective treatment for depression?
  • UFO’s from outer space have visited earth?

On what do you base your belief or disbelief for the questions above?

Of course, we all know what is meant by the words  think  and  knowledge . You probably also realize that they are not unitary concepts; there are different kinds of thought and knowledge. In this section, let us look at some of these differences. If you are familiar with these different kinds of thought and pay attention to them in your classes, it will help you to focus on the right goals, learn more effectively, and succeed in school. Different assignments and requirements in school call on you to use different kinds of knowledge or thought, so it will be very helpful for you to learn to recognize them (Anderson, et al. 2001).

Factual and conceptual knowledge

Module 5 introduced the idea of declarative memory, which is composed of facts and episodes. If you have ever played a trivia game or watched Jeopardy on TV, you realize that the human brain is able to hold an extraordinary number of facts. Likewise, you realize that each of us has an enormous store of episodes, essentially facts about events that happened in our own lives. It may be difficult to keep that in mind when we are struggling to retrieve one of those facts while taking an exam, however. Part of the problem is that, in contradiction to the advice from Module 5, many students continue to try to memorize course material as a series of unrelated facts (picture a history student simply trying to memorize history as a set of unrelated dates without any coherent story tying them together). Facts in the real world are not random and unorganized, however. It is the way that they are organized that constitutes a second key kind of knowledge, conceptual.

Concepts are nothing more than our mental representations of categories of things in the world. For example, think about dogs. When you do this, you might remember specific facts about dogs, such as they have fur and they bark. You may also recall dogs that you have encountered and picture them in your mind. All of this information (and more) makes up your concept of dog. You can have concepts of simple categories (e.g., triangle), complex categories (e.g., small dogs that sleep all day, eat out of the garbage, and bark at leaves), kinds of people (e.g., psychology professors), events (e.g., birthday parties), and abstract ideas (e.g., justice). Gregory Murphy (2002) refers to concepts as the “glue that holds our mental life together” (p. 1). Very simply, summarizing the world by using concepts is one of the most important cognitive tasks that we do. Our conceptual knowledge  is  our knowledge about the world. Individual concepts are related to each other to form a rich interconnected network of knowledge. For example, think about how the following concepts might be related to each other: dog, pet, play, Frisbee, chew toy, shoe. Or, of more obvious use to you now, how these concepts are related: working memory, long-term memory, declarative memory, procedural memory, and rehearsal? Because our minds have a natural tendency to organize information conceptually, when students try to remember course material as isolated facts, they are working against their strengths.

One last important point about concepts is that they allow you to instantly know a great deal of information about something. For example, if someone hands you a small red object and says, “here is an apple,” they do not have to tell you, “it is something you can eat.” You already know that you can eat it because it is true by virtue of the fact that the object is an apple; this is called drawing an  inference , assuming that something is true on the basis of your previous knowledge (for example, of category membership or of how the world works) or logical reasoning.

Procedural knowledge

Physical skills, such as tying your shoes, doing a cartwheel, and driving a car (or doing all three at the same time, but don’t try this at home) are certainly a kind of knowledge. They are procedural knowledge, the same idea as procedural memory that you saw in Module 5. Mental skills, such as reading, debating, and planning a psychology experiment, are procedural knowledge, as well. In short, procedural knowledge is the knowledge how to do something (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993).

Metacognitive knowledge

Floyd used to think that he had a great memory. Now, he has a better memory. Why? Because he finally realized that his memory was not as great as he once thought it was. Because Floyd eventually learned that he often forgets where he put things, he finally developed the habit of putting things in the same place. (Unfortunately, he did not learn this lesson before losing at least 5 watches and a wedding ring.) Because he finally realized that he often forgets to do things, he finally started using the To Do list app on his phone. And so on. Floyd’s insights about the real limitations of his memory have allowed him to remember things that he used to forget.

All of us have knowledge about the way our own minds work. You may know that you have a good memory for people’s names and a poor memory for math formulas. Someone else might realize that they have difficulty remembering to do things, like stopping at the store on the way home. Others still know that they tend to overlook details. This knowledge about our own thinking is actually quite important; it is called metacognitive knowledge, or  metacognition . Like other kinds of thinking skills, it is subject to error. For example, in unpublished research, one of the authors surveyed about 120 General Psychology students on the first day of the term. Among other questions, the students were asked them to predict their grade in the class and report their current Grade Point Average. Two-thirds of the students predicted that their grade in the course would be higher than their GPA. (The reality is that at our college, students tend to earn lower grades in psychology than their overall GPA.) Another example: Students routinely report that they thought they had done well on an exam, only to discover, to their dismay, that they were wrong (more on that important problem in a moment). Both errors reveal a breakdown in metacognition.

The Dunning-Kruger Effect

In general, most college students probably do not study enough. For example, using data from the National Survey of Student Engagement, Fosnacht, McCormack, and Lerma (2018) reported that first-year students at 4-year colleges in the U.S. averaged less than 14 hours per week preparing for classes. The typical suggestion is that you should spend two hours outside of class for every hour in class, or 24 – 30 hours per week for a full-time student. Clearly, students in general are nowhere near that recommended mark. Many observers, including some faculty, believe that this shortfall is a result of students being too busy or lazy. Now, it may be true that many students are too busy, with work and family obligations, for example. Others, are not particularly motivated in school, and therefore might correctly be labeled lazy. A third possible explanation, however, is that some students might not think they need to spend this much time. And this is a matter of metacognition. Consider the scenario that we mentioned above, students thinking they had done well on an exam only to discover that they did not. Justin Kruger and David Dunning examined scenarios very much like this in 1999. Kruger and Dunning gave research participants tests measuring humor, logic, and grammar. Then, they asked the participants to assess their own abilities and test performance in these areas. They found that participants in general tended to overestimate their abilities, already a problem with metacognition. Importantly, the participants who scored the lowest overestimated their abilities the most. Specifically, students who scored in the bottom quarter (averaging in the 12th percentile) thought they had scored in the 62nd percentile. This has become known as the  Dunning-Kruger effect . Many individual faculty members have replicated these results with their own student on their course exams, including the authors of this book. Think about it. Some students who just took an exam and performed poorly believe that they did well before seeing their score. It seems very likely that these are the very same students who stopped studying the night before because they thought they were “done.” Quite simply, it is not just that they did not know the material. They did not know that they did not know the material. That is poor metacognition.

In order to develop good metacognitive skills, you should continually monitor your thinking and seek frequent feedback on the accuracy of your thinking (Medina, Castleberry, & Persky 2017). For example, in classes get in the habit of predicting your exam grades. As soon as possible after taking an exam, try to find out which questions you missed and try to figure out why. If you do this soon enough, you may be able to recall the way it felt when you originally answered the question. Did you feel confident that you had answered the question correctly? Then you have just discovered an opportunity to improve your metacognition. Be on the lookout for that feeling and respond with caution.

concept :  a mental representation of a category of things in the world

Dunning-Kruger effect : individuals who are less competent tend to overestimate their abilities more than individuals who are more competent do

inference : an assumption about the truth of something that is not stated. Inferences come from our prior knowledge and experience, and from logical reasoning

metacognition :  knowledge about one’s own cognitive processes; thinking about your thinking

Critical thinking

One particular kind of knowledge or thinking skill that is related to metacognition is  critical thinking (Chew, 2020). You may have noticed that critical thinking is an objective in many college courses, and thus it could be a legitimate topic to cover in nearly any college course. It is particularly appropriate in psychology, however. As the science of (behavior and) mental processes, psychology is obviously well suited to be the discipline through which you should be introduced to this important way of thinking.

More importantly, there is a particular need to use critical thinking in psychology. We are all, in a way, experts in human behavior and mental processes, having engaged in them literally since birth. Thus, perhaps more than in any other class, students typically approach psychology with very clear ideas and opinions about its subject matter. That is, students already “know” a lot about psychology. The problem is, “it ain’t so much the things we don’t know that get us into trouble. It’s the things we know that just ain’t so” (Ward, quoted in Gilovich 1991). Indeed, many of students’ preconceptions about psychology are just plain wrong. Randolph Smith (2002) wrote a book about critical thinking in psychology called  Challenging Your Preconceptions,  highlighting this fact. On the other hand, many of students’ preconceptions about psychology are just plain right! But wait, how do you know which of your preconceptions are right and which are wrong? And when you come across a research finding or theory in this class that contradicts your preconceptions, what will you do? Will you stick to your original idea, discounting the information from the class? Will you immediately change your mind? Critical thinking can help us sort through this confusing mess.

But what is critical thinking? The goal of critical thinking is simple to state (but extraordinarily difficult to achieve): it is to be right, to draw the correct conclusions, to believe in things that are true and to disbelieve things that are false. We will provide two definitions of critical thinking (or, if you like, one large definition with two distinct parts). First, a more conceptual one: Critical thinking is thinking like a scientist in your everyday life (Schmaltz, Jansen, & Wenckowski, 2017).  Our second definition is more operational; it is simply a list of skills that are essential to be a critical thinker. Critical thinking entails solid reasoning and problem solving skills; skepticism; and an ability to identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions. Excellent deductive and inductive reasoning, and problem solving skills contribute to critical thinking. So, you can consider the subject matter of sections 7.2 and 7.3 to be part of critical thinking. Because we will be devoting considerable time to these concepts in the rest of the module, let us begin with a discussion about the other aspects of critical thinking.

Let’s address that first part of the definition. Scientists form hypotheses, or predictions about some possible future observations. Then, they collect data, or information (think of this as making those future observations). They do their best to make unbiased observations using reliable techniques that have been verified by others. Then, and only then, they draw a conclusion about what those observations mean. Oh, and do not forget the most important part. “Conclusion” is probably not the most appropriate word because this conclusion is only tentative. A scientist is always prepared that someone else might come along and produce new observations that would require a new conclusion be drawn. Wow! If you like to be right, you could do a lot worse than using a process like this.

A Critical Thinker’s Toolkit 

Now for the second part of the definition. Good critical thinkers (and scientists) rely on a variety of tools to evaluate information. Perhaps the most recognizable tool for critical thinking is  skepticism (and this term provides the clearest link to the thinking like a scientist definition, as you are about to see). Some people intend it as an insult when they call someone a skeptic. But if someone calls you a skeptic, if they are using the term correctly, you should consider it a great compliment. Simply put, skepticism is a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided. People from Missouri should recognize this principle, as Missouri is known as the Show-Me State. As a skeptic, you are not inclined to believe something just because someone said so, because someone else believes it, or because it sounds reasonable. You must be persuaded by high quality evidence.

Of course, if that evidence is produced, you have a responsibility as a skeptic to change your belief. Failure to change a belief in the face of good evidence is not skepticism; skepticism has open mindedness at its core. M. Neil Browne and Stuart Keeley (2018) use the term weak sense critical thinking to describe critical thinking behaviors that are used only to strengthen a prior belief. Strong sense critical thinking, on the other hand, has as its goal reaching the best conclusion. Sometimes that means strengthening your prior belief, but sometimes it means changing your belief to accommodate the better evidence.

Many times, a failure to think critically or weak sense critical thinking is related to a  bias , an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice. Everybody has biases, but many people are unaware of them. Awareness of your own biases gives you the opportunity to control or counteract them. Unfortunately, however, many people are happy to let their biases creep into their attempts to persuade others; indeed, it is a key part of their persuasive strategy. To see how these biases influence messages, just look at the different descriptions and explanations of the same events given by people of different ages or income brackets, or conservative versus liberal commentators, or by commentators from different parts of the world. Of course, to be successful, these people who are consciously using their biases must disguise them. Even undisguised biases can be difficult to identify, so disguised ones can be nearly impossible.

Here are some common sources of biases:

  • Personal values and beliefs.  Some people believe that human beings are basically driven to seek power and that they are typically in competition with one another over scarce resources. These beliefs are similar to the world-view that political scientists call “realism.” Other people believe that human beings prefer to cooperate and that, given the chance, they will do so. These beliefs are similar to the world-view known as “idealism.” For many people, these deeply held beliefs can influence, or bias, their interpretations of such wide ranging situations as the behavior of nations and their leaders or the behavior of the driver in the car ahead of you. For example, if your worldview is that people are typically in competition and someone cuts you off on the highway, you may assume that the driver did it purposely to get ahead of you. Other types of beliefs about the way the world is or the way the world should be, for example, political beliefs, can similarly become a significant source of bias.
  • Racism, sexism, ageism and other forms of prejudice and bigotry.  These are, sadly, a common source of bias in many people. They are essentially a special kind of “belief about the way the world is.” These beliefs—for example, that women do not make effective leaders—lead people to ignore contradictory evidence (examples of effective women leaders, or research that disputes the belief) and to interpret ambiguous evidence in a way consistent with the belief.
  • Self-interest.  When particular people benefit from things turning out a certain way, they can sometimes be very susceptible to letting that interest bias them. For example, a company that will earn a profit if they sell their product may have a bias in the way that they give information about their product. A union that will benefit if its members get a generous contract might have a bias in the way it presents information about salaries at competing organizations. (Note that our inclusion of examples describing both companies and unions is an explicit attempt to control for our own personal biases). Home buyers are often dismayed to discover that they purchased their dream house from someone whose self-interest led them to lie about flooding problems in the basement or back yard. This principle, the biasing power of self-interest, is likely what led to the famous phrase  Caveat Emptor  (let the buyer beware) .  

Knowing that these types of biases exist will help you evaluate evidence more critically. Do not forget, though, that people are not always keen to let you discover the sources of biases in their arguments. For example, companies or political organizations can sometimes disguise their support of a research study by contracting with a university professor, who comes complete with a seemingly unbiased institutional affiliation, to conduct the study.

People’s biases, conscious or unconscious, can lead them to make omissions, distortions, and assumptions that undermine our ability to correctly evaluate evidence. It is essential that you look for these elements. Always ask, what is missing, what is not as it appears, and what is being assumed here? For example, consider this (fictional) chart from an ad reporting customer satisfaction at 4 local health clubs.

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Clearly, from the results of the chart, one would be tempted to give Club C a try, as customer satisfaction is much higher than for the other 3 clubs.

There are so many distortions and omissions in this chart, however, that it is actually quite meaningless. First, how was satisfaction measured? Do the bars represent responses to a survey? If so, how were the questions asked? Most importantly, where is the missing scale for the chart? Although the differences look quite large, are they really?

Well, here is the same chart, with a different scale, this time labeled:

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Club C is not so impressive any more, is it? In fact, all of the health clubs have customer satisfaction ratings (whatever that means) between 85% and 88%. In the first chart, the entire scale of the graph included only the percentages between 83 and 89. This “judicious” choice of scale—some would call it a distortion—and omission of that scale from the chart make the tiny differences among the clubs seem important, however.

Also, in order to be a critical thinker, you need to learn to pay attention to the assumptions that underlie a message. Let us briefly illustrate the role of assumptions by touching on some people’s beliefs about the criminal justice system in the US. Some believe that a major problem with our judicial system is that many criminals go free because of legal technicalities. Others believe that a major problem is that many innocent people are convicted of crimes. The simple fact is, both types of errors occur. A person’s conclusion about which flaw in our judicial system is the greater tragedy is based on an assumption about which of these is the more serious error (letting the guilty go free or convicting the innocent). This type of assumption is called a value assumption (Browne and Keeley, 2018). It reflects the differences in values that people develop, differences that may lead us to disregard valid evidence that does not fit in with our particular values.

Oh, by the way, some students probably noticed this, but the seven tips for evaluating information that we shared in Module 1 are related to this. Actually, they are part of this section. The tips are, to a very large degree, set of ideas you can use to help you identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions. If you do not remember this section, we strongly recommend you take a few minutes to review it.

skepticism :  a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided

bias : an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice

  • Which of your beliefs (or disbeliefs) from the Activate exercise for this section were derived from a process of critical thinking? If some of your beliefs were not based on critical thinking, are you willing to reassess these beliefs? If the answer is no, why do you think that is? If the answer is yes, what concrete steps will you take?

7.2 Reasoning and Judgment

  • What percentage of kidnappings are committed by strangers?
  • Which area of the house is riskiest: kitchen, bathroom, or stairs?
  • What is the most common cancer in the US?
  • What percentage of workplace homicides are committed by co-workers?

An essential set of procedural thinking skills is  reasoning , the ability to generate and evaluate solid conclusions from a set of statements or evidence. You should note that these conclusions (when they are generated instead of being evaluated) are one key type of inference that we described in Section 7.1. There are two main types of reasoning, deductive and inductive.

Deductive reasoning

Suppose your teacher tells you that if you get an A on the final exam in a course, you will get an A for the whole course. Then, you get an A on the final exam. What will your final course grade be? Most people can see instantly that you can conclude with certainty that you will get an A for the course. This is a type of reasoning called  deductive reasoning , which is defined as reasoning in which a conclusion is guaranteed to be true as long as the statements leading to it are true. The three statements can be listed as an  argument , with two beginning statements and a conclusion:

Statement 1: If you get an A on the final exam, you will get an A for the course

Statement 2: You get an A on the final exam

Conclusion: You will get an A for the course

This particular arrangement, in which true beginning statements lead to a guaranteed true conclusion, is known as a  deductively valid argument . Although deductive reasoning is often the subject of abstract, brain-teasing, puzzle-like word problems, it is actually an extremely important type of everyday reasoning. It is just hard to recognize sometimes. For example, imagine that you are looking for your car keys and you realize that they are either in the kitchen drawer or in your book bag. After looking in the kitchen drawer, you instantly know that they must be in your book bag. That conclusion results from a simple deductive reasoning argument. In addition, solid deductive reasoning skills are necessary for you to succeed in the sciences, philosophy, math, computer programming, and any endeavor involving the use of logic to persuade others to your point of view or to evaluate others’ arguments.

Cognitive psychologists, and before them philosophers, have been quite interested in deductive reasoning, not so much for its practical applications, but for the insights it can offer them about the ways that human beings think. One of the early ideas to emerge from the examination of deductive reasoning is that people learn (or develop) mental versions of rules that allow them to solve these types of reasoning problems (Braine, 1978; Braine, Reiser, & Rumain, 1984). The best way to see this point of view is to realize that there are different possible rules, and some of them are very simple. For example, consider this rule of logic:

therefore q

Logical rules are often presented abstractly, as letters, in order to imply that they can be used in very many specific situations. Here is a concrete version of the of the same rule:

I’ll either have pizza or a hamburger for dinner tonight (p or q)

I won’t have pizza (not p)

Therefore, I’ll have a hamburger (therefore q)

This kind of reasoning seems so natural, so easy, that it is quite plausible that we would use a version of this rule in our daily lives. At least, it seems more plausible than some of the alternative possibilities—for example, that we need to have experience with the specific situation (pizza or hamburger, in this case) in order to solve this type of problem easily. So perhaps there is a form of natural logic (Rips, 1990) that contains very simple versions of logical rules. When we are faced with a reasoning problem that maps onto one of these rules, we use the rule.

But be very careful; things are not always as easy as they seem. Even these simple rules are not so simple. For example, consider the following rule. Many people fail to realize that this rule is just as valid as the pizza or hamburger rule above.

if p, then q

therefore, not p

Concrete version:

If I eat dinner, then I will have dessert

I did not have dessert

Therefore, I did not eat dinner

The simple fact is, it can be very difficult for people to apply rules of deductive logic correctly; as a result, they make many errors when trying to do so. Is this a deductively valid argument or not?

Students who like school study a lot

Students who study a lot get good grades

Jane does not like school

Therefore, Jane does not get good grades

Many people are surprised to discover that this is not a logically valid argument; the conclusion is not guaranteed to be true from the beginning statements. Although the first statement says that students who like school study a lot, it does NOT say that students who do not like school do not study a lot. In other words, it may very well be possible to study a lot without liking school. Even people who sometimes get problems like this right might not be using the rules of deductive reasoning. Instead, they might just be making judgments for examples they know, in this case, remembering instances of people who get good grades despite not liking school.

Making deductive reasoning even more difficult is the fact that there are two important properties that an argument may have. One, it can be valid or invalid (meaning that the conclusion does or does not follow logically from the statements leading up to it). Two, an argument (or more correctly, its conclusion) can be true or false. Here is an example of an argument that is logically valid, but has a false conclusion (at least we think it is false).

Either you are eleven feet tall or the Grand Canyon was created by a spaceship crashing into the earth.

You are not eleven feet tall

Therefore the Grand Canyon was created by a spaceship crashing into the earth

This argument has the exact same form as the pizza or hamburger argument above, making it is deductively valid. The conclusion is so false, however, that it is absurd (of course, the reason the conclusion is false is that the first statement is false). When people are judging arguments, they tend to not observe the difference between deductive validity and the empirical truth of statements or conclusions. If the elements of an argument happen to be true, people are likely to judge the argument logically valid; if the elements are false, they will very likely judge it invalid (Markovits & Bouffard-Bouchard, 1992; Moshman & Franks, 1986). Thus, it seems a stretch to say that people are using these logical rules to judge the validity of arguments. Many psychologists believe that most people actually have very limited deductive reasoning skills (Johnson-Laird, 1999). They argue that when faced with a problem for which deductive logic is required, people resort to some simpler technique, such as matching terms that appear in the statements and the conclusion (Evans, 1982). This might not seem like a problem, but what if reasoners believe that the elements are true and they happen to be wrong; they will would believe that they are using a form of reasoning that guarantees they are correct and yet be wrong.

deductive reasoning :  a type of reasoning in which the conclusion is guaranteed to be true any time the statements leading up to it are true

argument :  a set of statements in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion

deductively valid argument :  an argument for which true beginning statements guarantee that the conclusion is true

Inductive reasoning and judgment

Every day, you make many judgments about the likelihood of one thing or another. Whether you realize it or not, you are practicing  inductive reasoning   on a daily basis. In inductive reasoning arguments, a conclusion is likely whenever the statements preceding it are true. The first thing to notice about inductive reasoning is that, by definition, you can never be sure about your conclusion; you can only estimate how likely the conclusion is. Inductive reasoning may lead you to focus on Memory Encoding and Recoding when you study for the exam, but it is possible the instructor will ask more questions about Memory Retrieval instead. Unlike deductive reasoning, the conclusions you reach through inductive reasoning are only probable, not certain. That is why scientists consider inductive reasoning weaker than deductive reasoning. But imagine how hard it would be for us to function if we could not act unless we were certain about the outcome.

Inductive reasoning can be represented as logical arguments consisting of statements and a conclusion, just as deductive reasoning can be. In an inductive argument, you are given some statements and a conclusion (or you are given some statements and must draw a conclusion). An argument is  inductively strong   if the conclusion would be very probable whenever the statements are true. So, for example, here is an inductively strong argument:

  • Statement #1: The forecaster on Channel 2 said it is going to rain today.
  • Statement #2: The forecaster on Channel 5 said it is going to rain today.
  • Statement #3: It is very cloudy and humid.
  • Statement #4: You just heard thunder.
  • Conclusion (or judgment): It is going to rain today.

Think of the statements as evidence, on the basis of which you will draw a conclusion. So, based on the evidence presented in the four statements, it is very likely that it will rain today. Will it definitely rain today? Certainly not. We can all think of times that the weather forecaster was wrong.

A true story: Some years ago psychology student was watching a baseball playoff game between the St. Louis Cardinals and the Los Angeles Dodgers. A graphic on the screen had just informed the audience that the Cardinal at bat, (Hall of Fame shortstop) Ozzie Smith, a switch hitter batting left-handed for this plate appearance, had never, in nearly 3000 career at-bats, hit a home run left-handed. The student, who had just learned about inductive reasoning in his psychology class, turned to his companion (a Cardinals fan) and smugly said, “It is an inductively strong argument that Ozzie Smith will not hit a home run.” He turned back to face the television just in time to watch the ball sail over the right field fence for a home run. Although the student felt foolish at the time, he was not wrong. It was an inductively strong argument; 3000 at-bats is an awful lot of evidence suggesting that the Wizard of Ozz (as he was known) would not be hitting one out of the park (think of each at-bat without a home run as a statement in an inductive argument). Sadly (for the die-hard Cubs fan and Cardinals-hating student), despite the strength of the argument, the conclusion was wrong.

Given the possibility that we might draw an incorrect conclusion even with an inductively strong argument, we really want to be sure that we do, in fact, make inductively strong arguments. If we judge something probable, it had better be probable. If we judge something nearly impossible, it had better not happen. Think of inductive reasoning, then, as making reasonably accurate judgments of the probability of some conclusion given a set of evidence.

We base many decisions in our lives on inductive reasoning. For example:

Statement #1: Psychology is not my best subject

Statement #2: My psychology instructor has a reputation for giving difficult exams

Statement #3: My first psychology exam was much harder than I expected

Judgment: The next exam will probably be very difficult.

Decision: I will study tonight instead of watching Netflix.

Some other examples of judgments that people commonly make in a school context include judgments of the likelihood that:

  • A particular class will be interesting/useful/difficult
  • You will be able to finish writing a paper by next week if you go out tonight
  • Your laptop’s battery will last through the next trip to the library
  • You will not miss anything important if you skip class tomorrow
  • Your instructor will not notice if you skip class tomorrow
  • You will be able to find a book that you will need for a paper
  • There will be an essay question about Memory Encoding on the next exam

Tversky and Kahneman (1983) recognized that there are two general ways that we might make these judgments; they termed them extensional (i.e., following the laws of probability) and intuitive (i.e., using shortcuts or heuristics, see below). We will use a similar distinction between Type 1 and Type 2 thinking, as described by Keith Stanovich and his colleagues (Evans and Stanovich, 2013; Stanovich and West, 2000). Type 1 thinking is fast, automatic, effortful, and emotional. In fact, it is hardly fair to call it reasoning at all, as judgments just seem to pop into one’s head. Type 2 thinking , on the other hand, is slow, effortful, and logical. So obviously, it is more likely to lead to a correct judgment, or an optimal decision. The problem is, we tend to over-rely on Type 1. Now, we are not saying that Type 2 is the right way to go for every decision or judgment we make. It seems a bit much, for example, to engage in a step-by-step logical reasoning procedure to decide whether we will have chicken or fish for dinner tonight.

Many bad decisions in some very important contexts, however, can be traced back to poor judgments of the likelihood of certain risks or outcomes that result from the use of Type 1 when a more logical reasoning process would have been more appropriate. For example:

Statement #1: It is late at night.

Statement #2: Albert has been drinking beer for the past five hours at a party.

Statement #3: Albert is not exactly sure where he is or how far away home is.

Judgment: Albert will have no difficulty walking home.

Decision: He walks home alone.

As you can see in this example, the three statements backing up the judgment do not really support it. In other words, this argument is not inductively strong because it is based on judgments that ignore the laws of probability. What are the chances that someone facing these conditions will be able to walk home alone easily? And one need not be drunk to make poor decisions based on judgments that just pop into our heads.

The truth is that many of our probability judgments do not come very close to what the laws of probability say they should be. Think about it. In order for us to reason in accordance with these laws, we would need to know the laws of probability, which would allow us to calculate the relationship between particular pieces of evidence and the probability of some outcome (i.e., how much likelihood should change given a piece of evidence), and we would have to do these heavy math calculations in our heads. After all, that is what Type 2 requires. Needless to say, even if we were motivated, we often do not even know how to apply Type 2 reasoning in many cases.

So what do we do when we don’t have the knowledge, skills, or time required to make the correct mathematical judgment? Do we hold off and wait until we can get better evidence? Do we read up on probability and fire up our calculator app so we can compute the correct probability? Of course not. We rely on Type 1 thinking. We “wing it.” That is, we come up with a likelihood estimate using some means at our disposal. Psychologists use the term heuristic to describe the type of “winging it” we are talking about. A  heuristic   is a shortcut strategy that we use to make some judgment or solve some problem (see Section 7.3). Heuristics are easy and quick, think of them as the basic procedures that are characteristic of Type 1.  They can absolutely lead to reasonably good judgments and decisions in some situations (like choosing between chicken and fish for dinner). They are, however, far from foolproof. There are, in fact, quite a lot of situations in which heuristics can lead us to make incorrect judgments, and in many cases the decisions based on those judgments can have serious consequences.

Let us return to the activity that begins this section. You were asked to judge the likelihood (or frequency) of certain events and risks. You were free to come up with your own evidence (or statements) to make these judgments. This is where a heuristic crops up. As a judgment shortcut, we tend to generate specific examples of those very events to help us decide their likelihood or frequency. For example, if we are asked to judge how common, frequent, or likely a particular type of cancer is, many of our statements would be examples of specific cancer cases:

Statement #1: Andy Kaufman (comedian) had lung cancer.

Statement #2: Colin Powell (US Secretary of State) had prostate cancer.

Statement #3: Bob Marley (musician) had skin and brain cancer

Statement #4: Sandra Day O’Connor (Supreme Court Justice) had breast cancer.

Statement #5: Fred Rogers (children’s entertainer) had stomach cancer.

Statement #6: Robin Roberts (news anchor) had breast cancer.

Statement #7: Bette Davis (actress) had breast cancer.

Judgment: Breast cancer is the most common type.

Your own experience or memory may also tell you that breast cancer is the most common type. But it is not (although it is common). Actually, skin cancer is the most common type in the US. We make the same types of misjudgments all the time because we do not generate the examples or evidence according to their actual frequencies or probabilities. Instead, we have a tendency (or bias) to search for the examples in memory; if they are easy to retrieve, we assume that they are common. To rephrase this in the language of the heuristic, events seem more likely to the extent that they are available to memory. This bias has been termed the  availability heuristic   (Kahneman and Tversky, 1974).

The fact that we use the availability heuristic does not automatically mean that our judgment is wrong. The reason we use heuristics in the first place is that they work fairly well in many cases (and, of course that they are easy to use). So, the easiest examples to think of sometimes are the most common ones. Is it more likely that a member of the U.S. Senate is a man or a woman? Most people have a much easier time generating examples of male senators. And as it turns out, the U.S. Senate has many more men than women (74 to 26 in 2020). In this case, then, the availability heuristic would lead you to make the correct judgment; it is far more likely that a senator would be a man.

In many other cases, however, the availability heuristic will lead us astray. This is because events can be memorable for many reasons other than their frequency. Section 5.2, Encoding Meaning, suggested that one good way to encode the meaning of some information is to form a mental image of it. Thus, information that has been pictured mentally will be more available to memory. Indeed, an event that is vivid and easily pictured will trick many people into supposing that type of event is more common than it actually is. Repetition of information will also make it more memorable. So, if the same event is described to you in a magazine, on the evening news, on a podcast that you listen to, and in your Facebook feed; it will be very available to memory. Again, the availability heuristic will cause you to misperceive the frequency of these types of events.

Most interestingly, information that is unusual is more memorable. Suppose we give you the following list of words to remember: box, flower, letter, platypus, oven, boat, newspaper, purse, drum, car. Very likely, the easiest word to remember would be platypus, the unusual one. The same thing occurs with memories of events. An event may be available to memory because it is unusual, yet the availability heuristic leads us to judge that the event is common. Did you catch that? In these cases, the availability heuristic makes us think the exact opposite of the true frequency. We end up thinking something is common because it is unusual (and therefore memorable). Yikes.

The misapplication of the availability heuristic sometimes has unfortunate results. For example, if you went to K-12 school in the US over the past 10 years, it is extremely likely that you have participated in lockdown and active shooter drills. Of course, everyone is trying to prevent the tragedy of another school shooting. And believe us, we are not trying to minimize how terrible the tragedy is. But the truth of the matter is, school shootings are extremely rare. Because the federal government does not keep a database of school shootings, the Washington Post has maintained their own running tally. Between 1999 and January 2020 (the date of the most recent school shooting with a death in the US at of the time this paragraph was written), the Post reported a total of 254 people died in school shootings in the US. Not 254 per year, 254 total. That is an average of 12 per year. Of course, that is 254 people who should not have died (particularly because many were children), but in a country with approximately 60,000,000 students and teachers, this is a very small risk.

But many students and teachers are terrified that they will be victims of school shootings because of the availability heuristic. It is so easy to think of examples (they are very available to memory) that people believe the event is very common. It is not. And there is a downside to this. We happen to believe that there is an enormous gun violence problem in the United States. According the the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, there were 39,773 firearm deaths in the US in 2017. Fifteen of those deaths were in school shootings, according to the Post. 60% of those deaths were suicides. When people pay attention to the school shooting risk (low), they often fail to notice the much larger risk.

And examples like this are by no means unique. The authors of this book have been teaching psychology since the 1990’s. We have been able to make the exact same arguments about the misapplication of the availability heuristics and keep them current by simply swapping out for the “fear of the day.” In the 1990’s it was children being kidnapped by strangers (it was known as “stranger danger”) despite the facts that kidnappings accounted for only 2% of the violent crimes committed against children, and only 24% of kidnappings are committed by strangers (US Department of Justice, 2007). This fear overlapped with the fear of terrorism that gripped the country after the 2001 terrorist attacks on the World Trade Center and US Pentagon and still plagues the population of the US somewhat in 2020. After a well-publicized, sensational act of violence, people are extremely likely to increase their estimates of the chances that they, too, will be victims of terror. Think about the reality, however. In October of 2001, a terrorist mailed anthrax spores to members of the US government and a number of media companies. A total of five people died as a result of this attack. The nation was nearly paralyzed by the fear of dying from the attack; in reality the probability of an individual person dying was 0.00000002.

The availability heuristic can lead you to make incorrect judgments in a school setting as well. For example, suppose you are trying to decide if you should take a class from a particular math professor. You might try to make a judgment of how good a teacher she is by recalling instances of friends and acquaintances making comments about her teaching skill. You may have some examples that suggest that she is a poor teacher very available to memory, so on the basis of the availability heuristic you judge her a poor teacher and decide to take the class from someone else. What if, however, the instances you recalled were all from the same person, and this person happens to be a very colorful storyteller? The subsequent ease of remembering the instances might not indicate that the professor is a poor teacher after all.

Although the availability heuristic is obviously important, it is not the only judgment heuristic we use. Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman examined the role of heuristics in inductive reasoning in a long series of studies. Kahneman received a Nobel Prize in Economics for this research in 2002, and Tversky would have certainly received one as well if he had not died of melanoma at age 59 in 1996 (Nobel Prizes are not awarded posthumously). Kahneman and Tversky demonstrated repeatedly that people do not reason in ways that are consistent with the laws of probability. They identified several heuristic strategies that people use instead to make judgments about likelihood. The importance of this work for economics (and the reason that Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Prize) is that earlier economic theories had assumed that people do make judgments rationally, that is, in agreement with the laws of probability.

Another common heuristic that people use for making judgments is the  representativeness heuristic (Kahneman & Tversky 1973). Suppose we describe a person to you. He is quiet and shy, has an unassuming personality, and likes to work with numbers. Is this person more likely to be an accountant or an attorney? If you said accountant, you were probably using the representativeness heuristic. Our imaginary person is judged likely to be an accountant because he resembles, or is representative of the concept of, an accountant. When research participants are asked to make judgments such as these, the only thing that seems to matter is the representativeness of the description. For example, if told that the person described is in a room that contains 70 attorneys and 30 accountants, participants will still assume that he is an accountant.

inductive reasoning :  a type of reasoning in which we make judgments about likelihood from sets of evidence

inductively strong argument :  an inductive argument in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion that is probably true

heuristic :  a shortcut strategy that we use to make judgments and solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

availability heuristic :  judging the frequency or likelihood of some event type according to how easily examples of the event can be called to mind (i.e., how available they are to memory)

representativeness heuristic:   judging the likelihood that something is a member of a category on the basis of how much it resembles a typical category member (i.e., how representative it is of the category)

Type 1 thinking : fast, automatic, and emotional thinking.

Type 2 thinking : slow, effortful, and logical thinking.

  • What percentage of workplace homicides are co-worker violence?

Many people get these questions wrong. The answers are 10%; stairs; skin; 6%. How close were your answers? Explain how the availability heuristic might have led you to make the incorrect judgments.

  • Can you think of some other judgments that you have made (or beliefs that you have) that might have been influenced by the availability heuristic?

7.3 Problem Solving

  • Please take a few minutes to list a number of problems that you are facing right now.
  • Now write about a problem that you recently solved.
  • What is your definition of a problem?

Mary has a problem. Her daughter, ordinarily quite eager to please, appears to delight in being the last person to do anything. Whether getting ready for school, going to piano lessons or karate class, or even going out with her friends, she seems unwilling or unable to get ready on time. Other people have different kinds of problems. For example, many students work at jobs, have numerous family commitments, and are facing a course schedule full of difficult exams, assignments, papers, and speeches. How can they find enough time to devote to their studies and still fulfill their other obligations? Speaking of students and their problems: Show that a ball thrown vertically upward with initial velocity v0 takes twice as much time to return as to reach the highest point (from Spiegel, 1981).

These are three very different situations, but we have called them all problems. What makes them all the same, despite the differences? A psychologist might define a  problem   as a situation with an initial state, a goal state, and a set of possible intermediate states. Somewhat more meaningfully, we might consider a problem a situation in which you are in here one state (e.g., daughter is always late), you want to be there in another state (e.g., daughter is not always late), and with no obvious way to get from here to there. Defined this way, each of the three situations we outlined can now be seen as an example of the same general concept, a problem. At this point, you might begin to wonder what is not a problem, given such a general definition. It seems that nearly every non-routine task we engage in could qualify as a problem. As long as you realize that problems are not necessarily bad (it can be quite fun and satisfying to rise to the challenge and solve a problem), this may be a useful way to think about it.

Can we identify a set of problem-solving skills that would apply to these very different kinds of situations? That task, in a nutshell, is a major goal of this section. Let us try to begin to make sense of the wide variety of ways that problems can be solved with an important observation: the process of solving problems can be divided into two key parts. First, people have to notice, comprehend, and represent the problem properly in their minds (called  problem representation ). Second, they have to apply some kind of solution strategy to the problem. Psychologists have studied both of these key parts of the process in detail.

When you first think about the problem-solving process, you might guess that most of our difficulties would occur because we are failing in the second step, the application of strategies. Although this can be a significant difficulty much of the time, the more important source of difficulty is probably problem representation. In short, we often fail to solve a problem because we are looking at it, or thinking about it, the wrong way.

problem :  a situation in which we are in an initial state, have a desired goal state, and there is a number of possible intermediate states (i.e., there is no obvious way to get from the initial to the goal state)

problem representation :  noticing, comprehending and forming a mental conception of a problem

Defining and Mentally Representing Problems in Order to Solve Them

So, the main obstacle to solving a problem is that we do not clearly understand exactly what the problem is. Recall the problem with Mary’s daughter always being late. One way to represent, or to think about, this problem is that she is being defiant. She refuses to get ready in time. This type of representation or definition suggests a particular type of solution. Another way to think about the problem, however, is to consider the possibility that she is simply being sidetracked by interesting diversions. This different conception of what the problem is (i.e., different representation) suggests a very different solution strategy. For example, if Mary defines the problem as defiance, she may be tempted to solve the problem using some kind of coercive tactics, that is, to assert her authority as her mother and force her to listen. On the other hand, if Mary defines the problem as distraction, she may try to solve it by simply removing the distracting objects.

As you might guess, when a problem is represented one way, the solution may seem very difficult, or even impossible. Seen another way, the solution might be very easy. For example, consider the following problem (from Nasar, 1998):

Two bicyclists start 20 miles apart and head toward each other, each going at a steady rate of 10 miles per hour. At the same time, a fly that travels at a steady 15 miles per hour starts from the front wheel of the southbound bicycle and flies to the front wheel of the northbound one, then turns around and flies to the front wheel of the southbound one again, and continues in this manner until he is crushed between the two front wheels. Question: what total distance did the fly cover?

Please take a few minutes to try to solve this problem.

Most people represent this problem as a question about a fly because, well, that is how the question is asked. The solution, using this representation, is to figure out how far the fly travels on the first leg of its journey, then add this total to how far it travels on the second leg of its journey (when it turns around and returns to the first bicycle), then continue to add the smaller distance from each leg of the journey until you converge on the correct answer. You would have to be quite skilled at math to solve this problem, and you would probably need some time and pencil and paper to do it.

If you consider a different representation, however, you can solve this problem in your head. Instead of thinking about it as a question about a fly, think about it as a question about the bicycles. They are 20 miles apart, and each is traveling 10 miles per hour. How long will it take for the bicycles to reach each other? Right, one hour. The fly is traveling 15 miles per hour; therefore, it will travel a total of 15 miles back and forth in the hour before the bicycles meet. Represented one way (as a problem about a fly), the problem is quite difficult. Represented another way (as a problem about two bicycles), it is easy. Changing your representation of a problem is sometimes the best—sometimes the only—way to solve it.

Unfortunately, however, changing a problem’s representation is not the easiest thing in the world to do. Often, problem solvers get stuck looking at a problem one way. This is called  fixation . Most people who represent the preceding problem as a problem about a fly probably do not pause to reconsider, and consequently change, their representation. A parent who thinks her daughter is being defiant is unlikely to consider the possibility that her behavior is far less purposeful.

Problem-solving fixation was examined by a group of German psychologists called Gestalt psychologists during the 1930’s and 1940’s. Karl Dunker, for example, discovered an important type of failure to take a different perspective called  functional fixedness . Imagine being a participant in one of his experiments. You are asked to figure out how to mount two candles on a door and are given an assortment of odds and ends, including a small empty cardboard box and some thumbtacks. Perhaps you have already figured out a solution: tack the box to the door so it forms a platform, then put the candles on top of the box. Most people are able to arrive at this solution. Imagine a slight variation of the procedure, however. What if, instead of being empty, the box had matches in it? Most people given this version of the problem do not arrive at the solution given above. Why? Because it seems to people that when the box contains matches, it already has a function; it is a matchbox. People are unlikely to consider a new function for an object that already has a function. This is functional fixedness.

Mental set is a type of fixation in which the problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past, even though the solution may no longer be useful. It is commonly seen when students do math problems for homework. Often, several problems in a row require the reapplication of the same solution strategy. Then, without warning, the next problem in the set requires a new strategy. Many students attempt to apply the formerly successful strategy on the new problem and therefore cannot come up with a correct answer.

The thing to remember is that you cannot solve a problem unless you correctly identify what it is to begin with (initial state) and what you want the end result to be (goal state). That may mean looking at the problem from a different angle and representing it in a new way. The correct representation does not guarantee a successful solution, but it certainly puts you on the right track.

A bit more optimistically, the Gestalt psychologists discovered what may be considered the opposite of fixation, namely  insight . Sometimes the solution to a problem just seems to pop into your head. Wolfgang Kohler examined insight by posing many different problems to chimpanzees, principally problems pertaining to their acquisition of out-of-reach food. In one version, a banana was placed outside of a chimpanzee’s cage and a short stick inside the cage. The stick was too short to retrieve the banana, but was long enough to retrieve a longer stick also located outside of the cage. This second stick was long enough to retrieve the banana. After trying, and failing, to reach the banana with the shorter stick, the chimpanzee would try a couple of random-seeming attempts, react with some apparent frustration or anger, then suddenly rush to the longer stick, the correct solution fully realized at this point. This sudden appearance of the solution, observed many times with many different problems, was termed insight by Kohler.

Lest you think it pertains to chimpanzees only, Karl Dunker demonstrated that children also solve problems through insight in the 1930s. More importantly, you have probably experienced insight yourself. Think back to a time when you were trying to solve a difficult problem. After struggling for a while, you gave up. Hours later, the solution just popped into your head, perhaps when you were taking a walk, eating dinner, or lying in bed.

fixation :  when a problem solver gets stuck looking at a problem a particular way and cannot change his or her representation of it (or his or her intended solution strategy)

functional fixedness :  a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver cannot think of a new use for an object that already has a function

mental set :  a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past

insight :  a sudden realization of a solution to a problem

Solving Problems by Trial and Error

Correctly identifying the problem and your goal for a solution is a good start, but recall the psychologist’s definition of a problem: it includes a set of possible intermediate states. Viewed this way, a problem can be solved satisfactorily only if one can find a path through some of these intermediate states to the goal. Imagine a fairly routine problem, finding a new route to school when your ordinary route is blocked (by road construction, for example). At each intersection, you may turn left, turn right, or go straight. A satisfactory solution to the problem (of getting to school) is a sequence of selections at each intersection that allows you to wind up at school.

If you had all the time in the world to get to school, you might try choosing intermediate states randomly. At one corner you turn left, the next you go straight, then you go left again, then right, then right, then straight. Unfortunately, trial and error will not necessarily get you where you want to go, and even if it does, it is not the fastest way to get there. For example, when a friend of ours was in college, he got lost on the way to a concert and attempted to find the venue by choosing streets to turn onto randomly (this was long before the use of GPS). Amazingly enough, the strategy worked, although he did end up missing two out of the three bands who played that night.

Trial and error is not all bad, however. B.F. Skinner, a prominent behaviorist psychologist, suggested that people often behave randomly in order to see what effect the behavior has on the environment and what subsequent effect this environmental change has on them. This seems particularly true for the very young person. Picture a child filling a household’s fish tank with toilet paper, for example. To a child trying to develop a repertoire of creative problem-solving strategies, an odd and random behavior might be just the ticket. Eventually, the exasperated parent hopes, the child will discover that many of these random behaviors do not successfully solve problems; in fact, in many cases they create problems. Thus, one would expect a decrease in this random behavior as a child matures. You should realize, however, that the opposite extreme is equally counterproductive. If the children become too rigid, never trying something unexpected and new, their problem solving skills can become too limited.

Effective problem solving seems to call for a happy medium that strikes a balance between using well-founded old strategies and trying new ground and territory. The individual who recognizes a situation in which an old problem-solving strategy would work best, and who can also recognize a situation in which a new untested strategy is necessary is halfway to success.

Solving Problems with Algorithms and Heuristics

For many problems there is a possible strategy available that will guarantee a correct solution. For example, think about math problems. Math lessons often consist of step-by-step procedures that can be used to solve the problems. If you apply the strategy without error, you are guaranteed to arrive at the correct solution to the problem. This approach is called using an  algorithm , a term that denotes the step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution. Because algorithms are sometimes available and come with a guarantee, you might think that most people use them frequently. Unfortunately, however, they do not. As the experience of many students who have struggled through math classes can attest, algorithms can be extremely difficult to use, even when the problem solver knows which algorithm is supposed to work in solving the problem. In problems outside of math class, we often do not even know if an algorithm is available. It is probably fair to say, then, that algorithms are rarely used when people try to solve problems.

Because algorithms are so difficult to use, people often pass up the opportunity to guarantee a correct solution in favor of a strategy that is much easier to use and yields a reasonable chance of coming up with a correct solution. These strategies are called  problem solving heuristics . Similar to what you saw in section 6.2 with reasoning heuristics, a problem solving heuristic is a shortcut strategy that people use when trying to solve problems. It usually works pretty well, but does not guarantee a correct solution to the problem. For example, one problem solving heuristic might be “always move toward the goal” (so when trying to get to school when your regular route is blocked, you would always turn in the direction you think the school is). A heuristic that people might use when doing math homework is “use the same solution strategy that you just used for the previous problem.”

By the way, we hope these last two paragraphs feel familiar to you. They seem to parallel a distinction that you recently learned. Indeed, algorithms and problem-solving heuristics are another example of the distinction between Type 1 thinking and Type 2 thinking.

Although it is probably not worth describing a large number of specific heuristics, two observations about heuristics are worth mentioning. First, heuristics can be very general or they can be very specific, pertaining to a particular type of problem only. For example, “always move toward the goal” is a general strategy that you can apply to countless problem situations. On the other hand, “when you are lost without a functioning gps, pick the most expensive car you can see and follow it” is specific to the problem of being lost. Second, all heuristics are not equally useful. One heuristic that many students know is “when in doubt, choose c for a question on a multiple-choice exam.” This is a dreadful strategy because many instructors intentionally randomize the order of answer choices. Another test-taking heuristic, somewhat more useful, is “look for the answer to one question somewhere else on the exam.”

You really should pay attention to the application of heuristics to test taking. Imagine that while reviewing your answers for a multiple-choice exam before turning it in, you come across a question for which you originally thought the answer was c. Upon reflection, you now think that the answer might be b. Should you change the answer to b, or should you stick with your first impression? Most people will apply the heuristic strategy to “stick with your first impression.” What they do not realize, of course, is that this is a very poor strategy (Lilienfeld et al, 2009). Most of the errors on exams come on questions that were answered wrong originally and were not changed (so they remain wrong). There are many fewer errors where we change a correct answer to an incorrect answer. And, of course, sometimes we change an incorrect answer to a correct answer. In fact, research has shown that it is more common to change a wrong answer to a right answer than vice versa (Bruno, 2001).

The belief in this poor test-taking strategy (stick with your first impression) is based on the  confirmation bias   (Nickerson, 1998; Wason, 1960). You first saw the confirmation bias in Module 1, but because it is so important, we will repeat the information here. People have a bias, or tendency, to notice information that confirms what they already believe. Somebody at one time told you to stick with your first impression, so when you look at the results of an exam you have taken, you will tend to notice the cases that are consistent with that belief. That is, you will notice the cases in which you originally had an answer correct and changed it to the wrong answer. You tend not to notice the other two important (and more common) cases, changing an answer from wrong to right, and leaving a wrong answer unchanged.

Because heuristics by definition do not guarantee a correct solution to a problem, mistakes are bound to occur when we employ them. A poor choice of a specific heuristic will lead to an even higher likelihood of making an error.

algorithm :  a step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution to a problem

problem solving heuristic :  a shortcut strategy that we use to solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

confirmation bias :  people’s tendency to notice information that confirms what they already believe

An Effective Problem-Solving Sequence

You may be left with a big question: If algorithms are hard to use and heuristics often don’t work, how am I supposed to solve problems? Robert Sternberg (1996), as part of his theory of what makes people successfully intelligent (Module 8) described a problem-solving sequence that has been shown to work rather well:

  • Identify the existence of a problem.  In school, problem identification is often easy; problems that you encounter in math classes, for example, are conveniently labeled as problems for you. Outside of school, however, realizing that you have a problem is a key difficulty that you must get past in order to begin solving it. You must be very sensitive to the symptoms that indicate a problem.
  • Define the problem.  Suppose you realize that you have been having many headaches recently. Very likely, you would identify this as a problem. If you define the problem as “headaches,” the solution would probably be to take aspirin or ibuprofen or some other anti-inflammatory medication. If the headaches keep returning, however, you have not really solved the problem—likely because you have mistaken a symptom for the problem itself. Instead, you must find the root cause of the headaches. Stress might be the real problem. For you to successfully solve many problems it may be necessary for you to overcome your fixations and represent the problems differently. One specific strategy that you might find useful is to try to define the problem from someone else’s perspective. How would your parents, spouse, significant other, doctor, etc. define the problem? Somewhere in these different perspectives may lurk the key definition that will allow you to find an easier and permanent solution.
  • Formulate strategy.  Now it is time to begin planning exactly how the problem will be solved. Is there an algorithm or heuristic available for you to use? Remember, heuristics by their very nature guarantee that occasionally you will not be able to solve the problem. One point to keep in mind is that you should look for long-range solutions, which are more likely to address the root cause of a problem than short-range solutions.
  • Represent and organize information.  Similar to the way that the problem itself can be defined, or represented in multiple ways, information within the problem is open to different interpretations. Suppose you are studying for a big exam. You have chapters from a textbook and from a supplemental reader, along with lecture notes that all need to be studied. How should you (represent and) organize these materials? Should you separate them by type of material (text versus reader versus lecture notes), or should you separate them by topic? To solve problems effectively, you must learn to find the most useful representation and organization of information.
  • Allocate resources.  This is perhaps the simplest principle of the problem solving sequence, but it is extremely difficult for many people. First, you must decide whether time, money, skills, effort, goodwill, or some other resource would help to solve the problem Then, you must make the hard choice of deciding which resources to use, realizing that you cannot devote maximum resources to every problem. Very often, the solution to problem is simply to change how resources are allocated (for example, spending more time studying in order to improve grades).
  • Monitor and evaluate solutions.  Pay attention to the solution strategy while you are applying it. If it is not working, you may be able to select another strategy. Another fact you should realize about problem solving is that it never does end. Solving one problem frequently brings up new ones. Good monitoring and evaluation of your problem solutions can help you to anticipate and get a jump on solving the inevitable new problems that will arise.

Please note that this as  an  effective problem-solving sequence, not  the  effective problem solving sequence. Just as you can become fixated and end up representing the problem incorrectly or trying an inefficient solution, you can become stuck applying the problem-solving sequence in an inflexible way. Clearly there are problem situations that can be solved without using these skills in this order.

Additionally, many real-world problems may require that you go back and redefine a problem several times as the situation changes (Sternberg et al. 2000). For example, consider the problem with Mary’s daughter one last time. At first, Mary did represent the problem as one of defiance. When her early strategy of pleading and threatening punishment was unsuccessful, Mary began to observe her daughter more carefully. She noticed that, indeed, her daughter’s attention would be drawn by an irresistible distraction or book. Fresh with a re-representation of the problem, she began a new solution strategy. She began to remind her daughter every few minutes to stay on task and remind her that if she is ready before it is time to leave, she may return to the book or other distracting object at that time. Fortunately, this strategy was successful, so Mary did not have to go back and redefine the problem again.

Pick one or two of the problems that you listed when you first started studying this section and try to work out the steps of Sternberg’s problem solving sequence for each one.

a mental representation of a category of things in the world

an assumption about the truth of something that is not stated. Inferences come from our prior knowledge and experience, and from logical reasoning

knowledge about one’s own cognitive processes; thinking about your thinking

individuals who are less competent tend to overestimate their abilities more than individuals who are more competent do

Thinking like a scientist in your everyday life for the purpose of drawing correct conclusions. It entails skepticism; an ability to identify biases, distortions, omissions, and assumptions; and excellent deductive and inductive reasoning, and problem solving skills.

a way of thinking in which you refrain from drawing a conclusion or changing your mind until good evidence has been provided

an inclination, tendency, leaning, or prejudice

a type of reasoning in which the conclusion is guaranteed to be true any time the statements leading up to it are true

a set of statements in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion

an argument for which true beginning statements guarantee that the conclusion is true

a type of reasoning in which we make judgments about likelihood from sets of evidence

an inductive argument in which the beginning statements lead to a conclusion that is probably true

fast, automatic, and emotional thinking

slow, effortful, and logical thinking

a shortcut strategy that we use to make judgments and solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

udging the frequency or likelihood of some event type according to how easily examples of the event can be called to mind (i.e., how available they are to memory)

judging the likelihood that something is a member of a category on the basis of how much it resembles a typical category member (i.e., how representative it is of the category)

a situation in which we are in an initial state, have a desired goal state, and there is a number of possible intermediate states (i.e., there is no obvious way to get from the initial to the goal state)

noticing, comprehending and forming a mental conception of a problem

when a problem solver gets stuck looking at a problem a particular way and cannot change his or her representation of it (or his or her intended solution strategy)

a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver cannot think of a new use for an object that already has a function

a specific type of fixation in which a problem solver gets stuck using the same solution strategy that has been successful in the past

a sudden realization of a solution to a problem

a step-by-step procedure that guarantees a correct solution to a problem

The tendency to notice and pay attention to information that confirms your prior beliefs and to ignore information that disconfirms them.

a shortcut strategy that we use to solve problems. Although they are easy to use, they do not guarantee correct judgments and solutions

Introduction to Psychology Copyright © 2020 by Ken Gray; Elizabeth Arnott-Hill; and Or'Shaundra Benson is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

42 Problem Solving

[latexpage]

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Describe problem solving strategies
  • Define algorithm and heuristic
  • Explain some common roadblocks to effective problem solving

People face problems every day—usually, multiple problems throughout the day. Sometimes these problems are straightforward: To double a recipe for pizza dough, for example, all that is required is that each ingredient in the recipe be doubled. Sometimes, however, the problems we encounter are more complex. For example, say you have a work deadline, and you must mail a printed copy of a report to your supervisor by the end of the business day. The report is time-sensitive and must be sent overnight. You finished the report last night, but your printer will not work today. What should you do? First, you need to identify the problem and then apply a strategy for solving the problem.

PROBLEM-SOLVING STRATEGIES

When you are presented with a problem—whether it is a complex mathematical problem or a broken printer, how do you solve it? Before finding a solution to the problem, the problem must first be clearly identified. After that, one of many problem solving strategies can be applied, hopefully resulting in a solution.

A problem-solving strategy is a plan of action used to find a solution. Different strategies have different action plans associated with them ( [link] ). For example, a well-known strategy is trial and error . The old adage, “If at first you don’t succeed, try, try again” describes trial and error. In terms of your broken printer, you could try checking the ink levels, and if that doesn’t work, you could check to make sure the paper tray isn’t jammed. Or maybe the printer isn’t actually connected to your laptop. When using trial and error, you would continue to try different solutions until you solved your problem. Although trial and error is not typically one of the most time-efficient strategies, it is a commonly used one.

Problem-Solving Strategies
Method Description Example
Trial and error Continue trying different solutions until problem is solved Restarting phone, turning off WiFi, turning off bluetooth in order to determine why your phone is malfunctioning
Algorithm Step-by-step problem-solving formula Instruction manual for installing new software on your computer
Heuristic General problem-solving framework Working backwards; breaking a task into steps

Another type of strategy is an algorithm. An algorithm is a problem-solving formula that provides you with step-by-step instructions used to achieve a desired outcome (Kahneman, 2011). You can think of an algorithm as a recipe with highly detailed instructions that produce the same result every time they are performed. Algorithms are used frequently in our everyday lives, especially in computer science. When you run a search on the Internet, search engines like Google use algorithms to decide which entries will appear first in your list of results. Facebook also uses algorithms to decide which posts to display on your newsfeed. Can you identify other situations in which algorithms are used?

A heuristic is another type of problem solving strategy. While an algorithm must be followed exactly to produce a correct result, a heuristic is a general problem-solving framework (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). You can think of these as mental shortcuts that are used to solve problems. A “rule of thumb” is an example of a heuristic. Such a rule saves the person time and energy when making a decision, but despite its time-saving characteristics, it is not always the best method for making a rational decision. Different types of heuristics are used in different types of situations, but the impulse to use a heuristic occurs when one of five conditions is met (Pratkanis, 1989):

  • When one is faced with too much information
  • When the time to make a decision is limited
  • When the decision to be made is unimportant
  • When there is access to very little information to use in making the decision
  • When an appropriate heuristic happens to come to mind in the same moment

Working backwards is a useful heuristic in which you begin solving the problem by focusing on the end result. Consider this example: You live in Washington, D.C. and have been invited to a wedding at 4 PM on Saturday in Philadelphia. Knowing that Interstate 95 tends to back up any day of the week, you need to plan your route and time your departure accordingly. If you want to be at the wedding service by 3:30 PM, and it takes 2.5 hours to get to Philadelphia without traffic, what time should you leave your house? You use the working backwards heuristic to plan the events of your day on a regular basis, probably without even thinking about it.

Another useful heuristic is the practice of accomplishing a large goal or task by breaking it into a series of smaller steps. Students often use this common method to complete a large research project or long essay for school. For example, students typically brainstorm, develop a thesis or main topic, research the chosen topic, organize their information into an outline, write a rough draft, revise and edit the rough draft, develop a final draft, organize the references list, and proofread their work before turning in the project. The large task becomes less overwhelming when it is broken down into a series of small steps.

Problem-solving abilities can improve with practice. Many people challenge themselves every day with puzzles and other mental exercises to sharpen their problem-solving skills. Sudoku puzzles appear daily in most newspapers. Typically, a sudoku puzzle is a 9×9 grid. The simple sudoku below ( [link] ) is a 4×4 grid. To solve the puzzle, fill in the empty boxes with a single digit: 1, 2, 3, or 4. Here are the rules: The numbers must total 10 in each bolded box, each row, and each column; however, each digit can only appear once in a bolded box, row, and column. Time yourself as you solve this puzzle and compare your time with a classmate.

A four column by four row Sudoku puzzle is shown. The top left cell contains the number 3. The top right cell contains the number 2. The bottom right cell contains the number 1. The bottom left cell contains the number 4. The cell at the intersection of the second row and the second column contains the number 4. The cell to the right of that contains the number 1. The cell below the cell containing the number 1 contains the number 2. The cell to the left of the cell containing the number 2 contains the number 3.

Here is another popular type of puzzle ( [link] ) that challenges your spatial reasoning skills. Connect all nine dots with four connecting straight lines without lifting your pencil from the paper:

A square shaped outline contains three rows and three columns of dots with equal space between them.

Take a look at the “Puzzling Scales” logic puzzle below ( [link] ). Sam Loyd, a well-known puzzle master, created and refined countless puzzles throughout his lifetime (Cyclopedia of Puzzles, n.d.).

A puzzle involving a scale is shown. At the top of the figure it reads: “Sam Loyds Puzzling Scales.” The first row of the puzzle shows a balanced scale with 3 blocks and a top on the left and 12 marbles on the right. Below this row it reads: “Since the scales now balance.” The next row of the puzzle shows a balanced scale with just the top on the left, and 1 block and 8 marbles on the right. Below this row it reads: “And balance when arranged this way.” The third row shows an unbalanced scale with the top on the left side, which is much lower than the right side. The right side is empty. Below this row it reads: “Then how many marbles will it require to balance with that top?”

PITFALLS TO PROBLEM SOLVING

Not all problems are successfully solved, however. What challenges stop us from successfully solving a problem? Albert Einstein once said, “Insanity is doing the same thing over and over again and expecting a different result.” Imagine a person in a room that has four doorways. One doorway that has always been open in the past is now locked. The person, accustomed to exiting the room by that particular doorway, keeps trying to get out through the same doorway even though the other three doorways are open. The person is stuck—but she just needs to go to another doorway, instead of trying to get out through the locked doorway. A mental set is where you persist in approaching a problem in a way that has worked in the past but is clearly not working now.

Functional fixedness is a type of mental set where you cannot perceive an object being used for something other than what it was designed for. During the Apollo 13 mission to the moon, NASA engineers at Mission Control had to overcome functional fixedness to save the lives of the astronauts aboard the spacecraft. An explosion in a module of the spacecraft damaged multiple systems. The astronauts were in danger of being poisoned by rising levels of carbon dioxide because of problems with the carbon dioxide filters. The engineers found a way for the astronauts to use spare plastic bags, tape, and air hoses to create a makeshift air filter, which saved the lives of the astronauts.

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

Check out this Apollo 13 scene where the group of NASA engineers are given the task of overcoming functional fixedness.

Researchers have investigated whether functional fixedness is affected by culture. In one experiment, individuals from the Shuar group in Ecuador were asked to use an object for a purpose other than that for which the object was originally intended. For example, the participants were told a story about a bear and a rabbit that were separated by a river and asked to select among various objects, including a spoon, a cup, erasers, and so on, to help the animals. The spoon was the only object long enough to span the imaginary river, but if the spoon was presented in a way that reflected its normal usage, it took participants longer to choose the spoon to solve the problem. (German & Barrett, 2005). The researchers wanted to know if exposure to highly specialized tools, as occurs with individuals in industrialized nations, affects their ability to transcend functional fixedness. It was determined that functional fixedness is experienced in both industrialized and nonindustrialized cultures (German & Barrett, 2005).

In order to make good decisions, we use our knowledge and our reasoning. Often, this knowledge and reasoning is sound and solid. Sometimes, however, we are swayed by biases or by others manipulating a situation. For example, let’s say you and three friends wanted to rent a house and had a combined target budget of $1,600. The realtor shows you only very run-down houses for $1,600 and then shows you a very nice house for $2,000. Might you ask each person to pay more in rent to get the $2,000 home? Why would the realtor show you the run-down houses and the nice house? The realtor may be challenging your anchoring bias. An anchoring bias occurs when you focus on one piece of information when making a decision or solving a problem. In this case, you’re so focused on the amount of money you are willing to spend that you may not recognize what kinds of houses are available at that price point.

The confirmation bias is the tendency to focus on information that confirms your existing beliefs. For example, if you think that your professor is not very nice, you notice all of the instances of rude behavior exhibited by the professor while ignoring the countless pleasant interactions he is involved in on a daily basis. Hindsight bias leads you to believe that the event you just experienced was predictable, even though it really wasn’t. In other words, you knew all along that things would turn out the way they did. Representative bias describes a faulty way of thinking, in which you unintentionally stereotype someone or something; for example, you may assume that your professors spend their free time reading books and engaging in intellectual conversation, because the idea of them spending their time playing volleyball or visiting an amusement park does not fit in with your stereotypes of professors.

Finally, the availability heuristic is a heuristic in which you make a decision based on an example, information, or recent experience that is that readily available to you, even though it may not be the best example to inform your decision . Biases tend to “preserve that which is already established—to maintain our preexisting knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, and hypotheses” (Aronson, 1995; Kahneman, 2011). These biases are summarized in [link] .

Summary of Decision Biases
Bias Description
Anchoring Tendency to focus on one particular piece of information when making decisions or problem-solving
Confirmation Focuses on information that confirms existing beliefs
Hindsight Belief that the event just experienced was predictable
Representative Unintentional stereotyping of someone or something
Availability Decision is based upon either an available precedent or an example that may be faulty

Please visit this site to see a clever music video that a high school teacher made to explain these and other cognitive biases to his AP psychology students.

Were you able to determine how many marbles are needed to balance the scales in [link] ? You need nine. Were you able to solve the problems in [link] and [link] ? Here are the answers ( [link] ).

The first puzzle is a Sudoku grid of 16 squares (4 rows of 4 squares) is shown. Half of the numbers were supplied to start the puzzle and are colored blue, and half have been filled in as the puzzle’s solution and are colored red. The numbers in each row of the grid, left to right, are as follows. Row 1:  blue 3, red 1, red 4, blue 2. Row 2: red 2, blue 4, blue 1, red 3. Row 3: red 1, blue 3, blue 2, red 4. Row 4: blue 4, red 2, red 3, blue 1.The second puzzle consists of 9 dots arranged in 3 rows of 3 inside of a square. The solution, four straight lines made without lifting the pencil, is shown in a red line with arrows indicating the direction of movement. In order to solve the puzzle, the lines must extend beyond the borders of the box. The four connecting lines are drawn as follows. Line 1 begins at the top left dot, proceeds through the middle and right dots of the top row, and extends to the right beyond the border of the square. Line 2 extends from the end of line 1, through the right dot of the horizontally centered row, through the middle dot of the bottom row, and beyond the square’s border ending in the space beneath the left dot of the bottom row. Line 3 extends from the end of line 2 upwards through the left dots of the bottom, middle, and top rows. Line 4 extends from the end of line 3 through the middle dot in the middle row and ends at the right dot of the bottom row.

Many different strategies exist for solving problems. Typical strategies include trial and error, applying algorithms, and using heuristics. To solve a large, complicated problem, it often helps to break the problem into smaller steps that can be accomplished individually, leading to an overall solution. Roadblocks to problem solving include a mental set, functional fixedness, and various biases that can cloud decision making skills.

Review Questions

A specific formula for solving a problem is called ________.

  • an algorithm
  • a heuristic
  • a mental set
  • trial and error

A mental shortcut in the form of a general problem-solving framework is called ________.

Which type of bias involves becoming fixated on a single trait of a problem?

  • anchoring bias
  • confirmation bias
  • representative bias
  • availability bias

Which type of bias involves relying on a false stereotype to make a decision?

Critical Thinking Questions

What is functional fixedness and how can overcoming it help you solve problems?

Functional fixedness occurs when you cannot see a use for an object other than the use for which it was intended. For example, if you need something to hold up a tarp in the rain, but only have a pitchfork, you must overcome your expectation that a pitchfork can only be used for garden chores before you realize that you could stick it in the ground and drape the tarp on top of it to hold it up.

How does an algorithm save you time and energy when solving a problem?

An algorithm is a proven formula for achieving a desired outcome. It saves time because if you follow it exactly, you will solve the problem without having to figure out how to solve the problem. It is a bit like not reinventing the wheel.

Personal Application Question

Which type of bias do you recognize in your own decision making processes? How has this bias affected how you’ve made decisions in the past and how can you use your awareness of it to improve your decisions making skills in the future?

Creative Commons License

Share This Book

  • Increase Font Size

OPINION article

The role of motivation in complex problem solving.

\r\nC. Dominik Güss*

  • 1 Department of Psychology, University of North Florida, Jacksonville, FL, United States
  • 2 Trimberg Research Academy, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany

Previous research on Complex Problem Solving (CPS) has primarily focused on cognitive factors as outlined below. The current paper discusses the role of motivation during CPS and argues that motivation, emotion, and cognition interact and cannot be studied in an isolated manner. Motivation is the process that determines the energization and direction of behavior ( Heckhausen, 1991 ).

Three motivation theories and their relation to CPS are examined: McClelland's achievement motivation, Maslow's hierarchy of needs, and Dörner's needs as outlined in PSI-theory. We chose these three theories for several reasons. First, space forces us to be selective. Second, the three theories are among the most prominent motivational theories. Finally, they are need theories postulating several motivations and not just one. A thinking-aloud protocol is provided to illustrate the role of motivational and cognitive dynamics in CPS.

Problems are part of all the domains of human life. The field of CPS investigates problems that are complex, dynamic, and non-transparent ( Dörner, 1996 ). Complex problems consist of many interactively interrelated variables. Dynamic ones change and develop further over time, regardless of whether the involved people take action. And non-transparent problems have many aspects of the problem situation that are unclear or unknown to the involved people.

CPS researchers focus exactly on such kinds of problems. Under a narrow perspective, CPS can be defined as thinking that aims to overcome barriers and to reach goals in situations that are complex, dynamic, and non-transparent ( Frensch and Funke, 1995 ). Indeed, past research has shown the influential role of task properties ( Berry and Broadbent, 1984 ; Funke, 1985 ) and of cognitive factors on CPS strategies and performance, such as intelligence (e.g., Süß, 2001 ; Stadler et al., 2015 ), domain-specific knowledge (e.g., Wenke et al., 2005 ), cognitive biases and errors (e.g., Dörner, 1996 ; Güss et al., 2015 ), or self-reflection (e.g., Donovan et al., 2015 ).

Under a broader perspective, CPS can be defined as the study of cognitive, emotional, motivational, and social processes when people are confronted with such complex, dynamic and non-transparent problem situations ( Schoppek and Putz-Osterloh, 2003 ; Dörner and Güss, 2011 , 2013 ; Funke, 2012 ). The assumption here is that focusing solely on cognitive processes reveals an incomplete picture or an inaccurate one.

To study CPS, researchers have often used computer-simulated problem scenarios also called microworlds or virtual environments or strategy games. In these situations, participants are confronted with a complex problem simulated on the computer from which they gather information, and identify solutions. These decisions are then implemented into the system and result in changes to the problem situation.

Previous Research on Motivation and CPS

The idea to study the interaction of motivation, emotion, and cognition is not new ( Simon, 1967 ). However, in practice this has been rarely examined in the field of CPS. One study assessed the need for cognition (i.e., the tendency to engage in thinking and reflecting) and showed how high need of cognition was related to broader information collection and better performance in a management simulation ( Nair and Ramnarayan, 2000 ).

Vollmeyer and Rheinberg (1999 , 2000) explored in two studies the role of motivational factors in CPS. They assessed mastery confidence (similar to self-efficacy), incompetence fear, interest, and challenge as motivational factors. Their results demonstrated that mastery confidence and incompetence fear were good predictors for learning and for knowledge acquisition.

CPS Assessment

Before we describe three theories of motivation and how they might be related and applicable to CPS, we will briefly describe the WINFIRE computer simulation ( Gerdes et al., 1993 ; Schaub, 2009 ) and provide a part of a thinking-aloud protocol of one participant while working on WINFIRE. WINFIRE is the simulation of small cities surrounded by forests. Participants take the role of fire-fighting commanders who try to protect cities and forests from approaching fires. Participants can give a series of commands to several fire trucks and helicopters. In WINFIRE quick decisions and multitasking are required in order to avoid fires spreading. In one study, participants were also instructed to think aloud, i.e., to say aloud everything that went through their minds while working on WINFIRE. These thinking-aloud protocols, also called verbal protocols, were audiotaped and transcribed in five countries and compared (see Güss et al., 2010 ).

The following is a verbatim WINFIRE thinking-aloud protocol of a US participant ( Güss et al., 2010 ):

Ok, I don't see any fires yet. I'm trying to figure out how the helicopters pick up the water from the ponds. I put helicopters on patrol mode. Not really sure what that does. It doesn't seem to be moving. Oh, there it goes, it's moving…I guess you have to wait till there's a fire showing…Ok, fire just started in the middle, so I have to get some people to extinguish it. Ok, now I have another fire going here. I'm in trouble here. Ok. Ok, when I click extinguish, it don't seem to respond. Guess I'm not clear how to get trucks right to the fire. Ok, one fire has been extinguished, but a new one started in the same area. I'm getting more trucks out there trying to figure out, how to get helicopters to the pond. I still haven't figured that out, because they have to pick up the water. Ok, got a pretty good fire going here, so I'm going to put all the trucks on action, ok, water thing is making me mad. Ok. I'm not sure how it goes? Ok, the forest is burning up now—extinguish! Ok, ok, I'm in big trouble here…

Psychological Theories of Motivation and their Application to CPS

Mcclelland's human motivation theory.

In his Human Motivation theory, McClelland distinguishes three needs (power, affiliation, and achievement) and argues that human motivation is a response to changes in affective states. A specific situation will cause a change in the affective state through the non-specific response of the autonomic nervous system. This response will motivate a person toward a goal to reach a different affective state ( McClelland et al., 1953 ). An affective state may either be positive or negative, determining the direction of motivated behavior as either approach oriented, i.e., to maintain the state, or avoidance oriented, i.e., to avoid or discontinue the state ( McClelland et al., 1953 ).

Motivation intensity varies among individuals based on perception of the stimulus and the adaptive abilities of the individual. Hence, when a discrepancy exists between expectation and perception, then a person will be motivated to eliminate this discrepancy ( McClelland et al., 1953 ). In the statement from the thinking-aloud protocol we can infer the participant's achievement motivation, “ Guess I'm not clear how to get trucks right to the fire. Ok, one fire has been extinguished, but a new one started in the same area.” The participant at first begins to give up and reduce effort, but then achieves a step toward the goal. This achievement causes the reevaluation of the discrepancy between ability and the goal as not too large to overcome. This realization motivates the participant to continue working through the scenario. Whereas, the need for achievement seems to guide CPS, the needs for power and affiliation cannot be observed in the current thinking-aloud protocol.

Based on the previous discussion we can derive the following predictions:

Prediction 1 : Approach-orientation will lead to greater engagement in CPS compared to avoidance-orientation.

Prediction 2 : Based on an individual's experience either power, affiliation, or achievement will become dominant and guide the strategic approach in CPS.

Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs

Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs ( Maslow, 1943 , 1954 ) suggests that everyone has five basic needs that act as motivating forces in a person's life. Maslow's hierarchy takes the form of a pyramid in which needs lower in the pyramid are primary motivators. They have to be met before higher needs can become motivating forces. At the bottom of the pyramid are the most basic needs beginning with physiological needs, such as hunger, and followed by safety needs. Then follow the psychological needs of belongingness and love, and then esteem. Once these four groups of needs have been met, a person may reach the self-fulfillment stage of self-actualization at which time a person can be motivated to achieve ones full potential ( Maslow, 1943 ).

The first four groups of needs are external motivators because they motivate through both deficiency and fulfillment. In essence, a person fulfills a need which then releases the next unsatisfied need to be the dominant motivator ( Maslow, 1943 , 1954 ). The safety need is often understood as seeking shelter, but Maslow also understands safety also as wanting “a predictable, orderly world” ( Maslow, 1943 , p. 377), “an organized world rather than an unorganized or unstructured one” ( Maslow, 1943 , p. 377). Safety refers to the “common preference for familiar rather than unfamiliar things” ( Maslow, 1943 , p. 379).

In this sense the safety need becomes active when the person does not understand what is happening in the microworld, as the following passage of the thinking-aloud protocol illustrates. “ I put helicopters on patrol mode. Not really sure what that does. It doesn't seem to be moving.” The safety need is demonstrated in the person's desire for organization, since unknown and unexpected events are seen as threats to safety.

The esteem need as a motivator becomes evident through the statement, “ Guess I'm not clear how to get trucks right to the fire.” The participant becomes aware of his inability to control the situation which affects his self-esteem. The esteem need is never fulfilled in the described situation and remains the primary motivator. The following statements show how affected the participant's esteem need is by the inability to control the burning fires. “ Ok. I'm not sure how it goes? Ok, the forest is burning up now—extinguish! Ok, ok, I'm in big trouble .”

Prediction 3 : A strong safety need will be related to elaborate and detailed information collection in CPS compared to low safety need.

Prediction 4 : People with high esteem needs will be affected more by difficulties in CPS and engage more often in behaviors to protect their esteem compared to people with low esteem needs.

Dörner's Theory of Motivation as Part of PSI-Theory

PSI-theory described the interaction of cognitive, emotional, and motivational processes ( Dörner, 2003 ; Dörner and Güss, 2011 ). Only a small part of the theory is examined here. Briefly, the theory encompasses five basic human needs: the existential needs (thirst, hunger, and pain avoidance), the sexuality need, and the social need for affiliation (group binding), the need for certainty (predictability), and the need for competence (mastery). If the environment is unpredictable, the certainty need becomes active. If we are not able to cope with problems, the competence need becomes active. The need for competence also becomes active when any other need becomes activated. With an increase in needs, the arousal increases.

The first three needs cannot be observed or inferred from the thinking-aloud protocol provided. Statements like, “I'm trying to figure out how the helicopters pick up the water from the ponds.” and “Guess I'm not clear how to get trucks right to the fire,” demonstrate the needs for certainty and competence, i.e., to make the environment predictable and controllable.

The following statements reflect the participant's need for competence, i.e., the inefficacy or incapability of coping with problems. “ I'm in trouble here…ok, water thing is making me mad .” Not being able to extinguish the fires that are approaching cities and are destroying forests is experienced as anger. The arousal rises as the resolution level of thinking decreases. So, the participant does not think about different options in an elaborate manner. Yet, the participant becomes aware of his failure. The competence need then causes the participant to search for possible solutions, “ I still haven't figured that out because they have to pick up the water…” The need for competence is satisfied when the problem solver is able to change either the environment or ones views of the environment.

Prediction 5 : A strong certainty need is positively related to a strong competence need.

Prediction 6 : High need for certainty paired with high need for competence can lead to safeguarding behavior, i.e., background monitoring.

Prediction 7 : An increase in the competence and uncertainty needs leads to increased arousal and a lower resolution level of thinking. CPS becomes one-dimensional and possible long-term and side-effects are not considered adequately.

Summary and Evaluation

We have briefly discussed three motivation theories and their relation to CPS referring to one thinking-aloud protocol: McClelland's achievement motivation, Maslow's hierarchy of needs, and Dörner's needs as outlined in PSI-theory.

A Comparison of Three Need Theories in the Context of CPS.

www.frontiersin.org

Comparing the scope of the three theories and referring to the scope and different needs covered in the three theories, McClelland's theory describes three needs (power, affiliation, and achievement), Maslow's theory describes five groups of needs (physiological, safety, love and belonging, esteem, self-actualization), and Dörner's theory describes five different needs (existential, sexuality, affiliation, certainty, and competence).

All three theories can be applied to CPS. McClelland's need for achievement, Maslow's needs for esteem and safety, and Dörner's needs for certainty and competence could be inferred from the thinking–aloud passage. The need for affiliation which is a part of each of the three theories could play an important role when groups solve complex problems.

The existential needs and the need for affiliation outlined in PSI-theory can also be found in Maslow's hierarchy of needs. These two theories differ in the adaptability of the needs. However, Maslow's esteem needs are only activated as the primary motivator as the physiological needs, belongingness, and love needs are met. The needs are more fluidly described as motivators in PSI-theory. One need becomes the dominant motive according to the expectancy–value principle. Expectancy stands for the estimated likelihood of success. The value of a motive stands for the strength of the need. According to McClelland's theory, the role of three motivations develops through life experience in a specific culture; and often times, one of the three becomes the main driving force for a person, almost like a personality trait. In that sense, there is not much flexibility.

Motivation and emotion are closely related as became partially clear in the discussion of McClelland's theory. Emotions are discussed in detail in PSI-theory, but space does not allow us to discuss those in detail here (see Dörner, 2003 ). Emotions are not described in detail in Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs.

Individual differences in motivation and needs are discussed in two of the three theories. According to McClelland, a person develops an individual achievement motive by learning one's own abilities from past achievements and failures. Based on different learning histories, different persons will have a different dominant motivation guiding behavior in a given situation. Learning history also influences the competence need in PSI-theory. Additionally PSI-theory assumes individual differences that are simulated through different individual motivational parameters in the theory. The certainty need, for example, becomes active when there is a deviation from a given set point. Individual differences are related to different set points and how sensitive the deviations are (e.g., deviation starts quickly vs. deviation starts slowly).

The thinking-aloud example from the WINFIRE microworld described earlier demonstrates that a person's CPS process is influenced by the person's needs. We have focused in our discussion on motivational processes that are considered in the framework of need theories. Beyond that, other motivational theories exist that focus on the importance of motivation for learning and achievement (e.g., expectancy, reasons for engagement, see Eccles and Wigfield, 2002 ). Thus, the applicability of these theories to CPS could be explored in future studies as well.

We discussed the three motivational theories of McClelland's Achievement Motivation, Maslow's Hierarchy of Need, and Dörner's Theory of Motivation as part of PSI-Theory. Although, the theories differ our discussion has shown that the three theories can be applied to CPS. Problem solving is a motivated process and determined by human motivations and needs.

Author Contributions

The first author CG conceptualized the manuscript, selected the thinking-aloud passage, the second author MB primarily summarized McClellands and Maslow's theories. All authors contributed to writing up the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Berry, D. C., and Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 36A, 209–231. doi: 10.1080/14640748408402156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donovan, S., Güss, C. D., and Naslund, D. (2015). Improving dynamic decision making through training and self-reflection. Judgm. Decis. Making 10, 284–295.

Google Scholar

Dörner, D. (1996). The logic of failure. Recognizing and Avoiding Error in Complex Situations . New York, NY: Basic Books.

Dörner, D. (2003). Bauplan für eine Seele [Blueprint for a soul] . Reinbek: Rowohlt.

Dörner, D., and Güss, C. D. (2011). A psychological analysis of Adolf Hitler's decision making as Commander in Chief: summa confidentia et nimius metus. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 15, 37–49. doi: 10.1037/a0022375

Dörner, D., and Güss, C. D. (2013). PSI: a computational architecture of cognition, motivation, and emotion. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 17, 297–317. doi: 10.1037/a0032947

Eccles, J. S., and Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 109–132. doi: 10.1146/annurevsych.53.100901.135153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frensch, P., and Funke, J. (eds.) (1995). Complex Problem Solving: The European Perspective . Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

Funke, J. (1985). Steuerung dynamischer Systeme durch Aufbau und Anwendung subjektiver Kausalmodelle [Control of dynamic systems via Construction and Application of subjective causal models]. Zeitschrift für Psychol. 193, 443–465.

Funke, J. (2012). “Complex problem solving,” in Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning , ed N. M. Seel (Heidelberg: Springer), 682–685.

Gerdes, J., Dörner, D., and Pfeiffer, E. (1993). Interaktive Computersimulation “Winfire.” [The Interactive Computer Simulation “Winfire”] . Otto-Friedrich-Universität Bamberg: Lehrstuhl Psychologie, II.

Güss, C. D., Tuason, M. T., and Gerhard, C. (2010). Cross-national comparisons of complex problem-solving strategies in two microworlds. Cogn. Sci. 34, 489–520. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01087.x

Güss, C. D., Tuason, M. T., and Orduña, L. V. (2015). Strategies, tactics, and errors in dynamic decision making. J. Dyn. Decis. Making 1, 1–14. doi: 10.11588/jddm.2015.1.13131

CrossRef Full Text

Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and Action . New York, NY: Springer.

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50, 370–396. doi: 10.1037/h0054346

Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and Personality . New York, NY: Harper and Row.

McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., and Lowell, E. L. (1953). The Achievement Motive . East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Nair, K. U., and Ramnarayan, S. (2000). Individual differences in need for cognition and complex problem solving. J. Res. Pers. 34, 305–328. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1999.2274

Schaub, H. (2009). Fire Simulation . Ottobrunn: IABG.

Schoppek, W., and Putz-Osterloh, W. (2003). Individuelle Unterschiede und die Bearbeitung komplexer Probleme. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychol. 24, 163–173. doi: 10.1024/0170-1789.24.3.163

Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychol. Rev. 74, 29–39. doi: 10.1037/h0024127

Süß, H.-M. (2001). “Die Rolle von Intelligenz und Wissen für erfolgreiches Handeln in komplexen Problemsituationen [The role of intelligence and knowledge for successful performance in complex problem solving],” in Komplexität und Kompetenz: Ausgewählte Fragen der Kompetenzforschung , ed G. Franke (Bielefeld: Bertelsmann), 249–275.

Stadler, M., Becker, N., Gödker, M., Leutner, D., and Greiff, S. (2015). Complex problem solving and intelligence: a meta-analysis. Intelligence 53, 92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.005

Vollmeyer, R., and Rheinberg, F. (1999). Motivation and metacognition when learning a complex system. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 14, 541–554. doi: 10.1007/BF03172978

Vollmeyer, R., and Rheinberg, F. (2000). Does motivation affect performance via persistence? Learn. Instruct. 10, 293–309. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(99)00031-6

Wenke, D., Frensch, P. A., and Funke, J. (2005). “Complex problem solving and intelligence: empirical relation and causal direction,” in Cognition and Intelligence: Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind , eds R. J. Sternberg and J. E. Pretz (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 160–187.

Keywords: complex problem solving, dynamic decision making, simulation, motivation, PSI-theory, Maslow's hierarchy of needs, achievement motivation

Citation: Güss CD, Burger ML and Dörner D (2017) The Role of Motivation in Complex Problem Solving. Front. Psychol . 8:851. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00851

Received: 16 March 2017; Accepted: 09 May 2017; Published: 23 May 2017.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2017 Güss, Burger and Dörner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: C. Dominik Güss, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

What are the Factors that Affect Problem-Solving Activities? Part 1

  • Categories : Resource management
  • Tags : Project management

What are the Factors that Affect Problem-Solving Activities?  Part 1

Identification of the Problem

The most important of factors that affect problem-solving activities is realization of the problem. A problem is decided by the purpose. For instance, manufacturing managers evaluated based on the percentage of time they have operated the production lines do not have a problem with operating the production line without orders from their sales division. On the other hand, the sales division will have a major problem with this action if there are no orders and excessive inventory piles up as a result of this action.

Identification or realization of the problem, keeping the big picture in mind, is the first and most important step toward problem solving. They key to doing so lies in understanding the purpose of the action. The basic steps toward this direction include:

  • Defining the problem.
  • Identifying the potential causes for the problems.
  • Listing out the various solutions.
  • Selecting the best alternative.
  • Planning implementation.
  • Monitoring and verifying the implementation.

Image Credit: flickr.com/Martino Franchi

Personality Types

In 1987, M. McCaulley undertook one of the earliest research projects to link individual differences in personality to problem-solving approaches. He used Carl Jung’s theory of individual preferences to correlate the four mental processes of sensing, intuiting, thinking, and feeling to decision-making preferences. Sensing individuals considers facts, details, and reality when making decisions to solve problems. Intuitive individuals try to understand the meaningfulness of the facts, the relationships among the facts, and the possibilities of future events that can be imagined from these facts to make decisions, and usually develop new, original solutions. Thinking individuals tend to use logic and objective analysis during problem solving, and Feeling individuals tend to veer toward subjective considerations of values and feelings in the problem-solving process. Sensing and Intuitive people approach problems through their perceptions, and they prefer flexibility and adaptability. Thinking- and Feeling-oriented people usually make judgments and tend to prefer the problem-solving process to demonstrate closure.

Individuals preferring introversion take time to think and clarify their ideas before acting, while those preferring extroversion talk through their ideas to clarify them before acting. Introverts remain concerned with their own understanding of important concepts and ideas, whereas extroverts seek feedback from the environment.

Temperament

The ability of a person to solve problems depends on both personality type and temperament. People motivated toward a goal , or those who are high achievers, take that extra effort and initiative to find the root cause of problems and solve it. Others go by the routine procedure and do the minimum required.

High-risk takers who usually find themselves in more problems generally tend to be more adept in solving problems, also.

A far bigger personality dimension, however, lies in the positive treatment of the problem, or considering it as an opportunity to learn new things. A negatively charged problem impedes solution.

Thinking Patterns

Another of the major factors that affect problem-solving activities includes the thought processes or thinking patterns of the concerned individual.

The major thought process dimensions include:

  • Strategic thinking or a bigger long-term focus instead of short-term departmental focus.
  • Emotional thinking or judging whether a solution is right or wrong based on emotional commitment.
  • Realistic thinking or the approach of starting from what can be done and fixing the essential problem first.
  • Empirical thinking or judging whether the situation is right or wrong based on past experiences.

Problem solvers need to choose the appropriate thinking pattern based on the situation.

Besides such dimensions, the ability to think systematically through a rational process, such as systems things, thought and effect process, and contingent thinking, and the ability to forge hypothesis improves the thinking processes.

Skills and Technical Competency

The ability to solve a problem depends greatly on the person’s competency relative to the problem in hand. For instance, a team leader skilled in computer networking might be able to manage a network failure, create ad hoc procedures until the systems are restored, or effectively direct the recovery by functional experts. A team leader with no clue on networking would remain totally at sea and at the mercy of the functional experts.

At times problem-solving requires creativity and innovation, which again depends on the personality and temperament of the person, and the culture of the organization.

Hierarchies

Hierarchical organizations that tend to give importance to designations and fixed job descriptions , insist on adherence to procedures, and do not encourage ad hoc measures, stifle creativity and innovation and have a profound impact on problem-solving activities.

The ability to solve problems often depends on the administrative mazes and bureaucratic hurdles. For instance, a computer expert working in human resources might be the best person to recover a crashed system. This person, however, might not have the necessary permissions or authorization to access the main server, and the work remains disrupted until the authorized repair personnel arrive from far away.

External Environment

The external environment of an organization remains the root cause of many problems in a project, and the solution depends on the external environment itself. For instance, availability of skilled manpower depends on the labor market, running of machinery depends on the provision of energy by the utility provider, and starting operations depends on compliance with the procedures to securing the necessary permits. The best approach to problem solving is having a good understanding of the state of the external environment to reconcile the business operations with the external environment.

A business cannot control or alter the external environment. It can only harness it to its advantage. In this realization lies the key to solving most problems.

  • Huitt, William, G. “Problem Solving and Decision Making: Consideration of Individual Differences Using the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator.” Retrieved from https://teach.valdosta.edu/whuitt/papers/prbsmbti.html on 21 October 2010.
  • Shibata, Hidetoshi. “Problem Solving: Definition, terminology, and patterns.”Retrieved from https://www.mediafrontier.com/Article/PS/PS.htm on 21 October 2010.
  • McNamara, Carter. “ Basic Guidelines to Problem Solving and Decision Making.”. Retrieved from https://www.managementhelp.org/prsn _prd/prb_bsc.htm on 21 October 2010.

Logo

12 Apr What Affects Problem Solving

Many factors affect the problem solving process and hence it can become complicated and drawn out when they are unaccounted for. Acknowledging the factors that affect the process and taking them into account when forming a solution gives teams the best chance of solving the problem effectively. Below we have outlined the key factors affecting the problem solving process.

Understanding the problem 

The most important factor in solving a problem is to first fully understand it. This includes understanding the bigger picture it sits within, the factors and stakeholders involved, the causes of the problem and any potential solutions. Effective solutions are unlikely to be discovered if the exact problem is not fully understood.

Personality types/Temperament 

McCauley (1987) was one of the first authors to link personalities to problem solving skills. Attributes like patience, communication, team skills and cognitive skills can all affect an individual’s likelihood of solving a problem. Different individuals will take different approaches to solving problems and experience varying degrees of success. For this reason, as a manager, it is important to select team members for a project whose skills align with the problem at hand.

Skills/Competencies

Individual’s skills will also affect the problem solving process. For example, a straight-forward technical issue may appear very complicated to an individual from a non-technical background. Skill levels are most commonly determined by experience and training and for this reason it is important to expose newer team members to a wide variety of problems, as well as providing training.

Resources available

Although many individuals believe they have the capabilities to solve a certain problem, the resources available to them can often slow-down the process. These resources may be in the form of technology, human capital or finance. For example, a team may come up with a solution for an inefficient transport system by suggesting new vehicles are purchased. Despite the solution solving the problem entirely, it may not fit within the budget. This is why only realistic solutions should be pursued and resources should not be wasted on other projects.

External factors should also always be taken into account when solving a problem, as factors that may not seem to directly affect the problem can often play a part. Examples include competitor actions, fluctuations in the economy, government restrictions and environmental issues.

Carskadon, Thomas G, Nancy G McCarley, and Mary H McCaulley. (1987). Compendium of Research Involving the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator . Gainesville, Fl.: Center for Applications of Psychological Type, 1987. Print.

  • Open access
  • Published: 25 May 2023

Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective learning: results from partial least square structural equation modeling and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis

  • Ying Wang 1   na1 ,
  • Ze-Ling Xu 1   na1 ,
  • Jia-Yao Lou 1   na1 &
  • Ke-Da Chen 1  

BMC Medical Education volume  23 , Article number:  382 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

5231 Accesses

2 Citations

Metrics details

The Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development emphasizes the importance of complex problem-solving (CPS) skills in the 21st century. CPS skills have been linked to academic performance, career development, and job competency training. Reflective learning, which includes journal writing, peer reflection, selfreflection, and group discussion, has been explored to improve critical thinking and problem-solving abilities. The development of various thinking modes and abilities, such as algorithmic thinking, creativity, and empathic concern, all affect problem-solving skills. However, there is a lack of an overall theory to relate variables to each other, which means that different theories need to be integrated to focus on how CPS skills can be effectively trained and improved.

Data from 136 medical students were analyzed using partial least square structural equation modeling (PLSSEM) and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA). A hypothesized model examining the associations between the CPS skills and influence factors was constructed.

The evaluation of the structural model showed that some variables had significant influences on CPS skills, while others did not. After deleting the insignificant pathways, a structural model was built, which showed that mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking were observed, while personal distress only had a direct effect on CPS skills. The results of necessity showed that only cooperativity and creativity are necessary conditions for critical thinking. The fsQCA analysis provided clues for each different pathway to the result, with all consistency values being higher than 0.8, and most coverage values being between 0.240 and 0.839. The fsQCA confirmed the validity of the model and provided configurations that enhanced the CPS skills.

Conclusions

This study provides evidence that reflective learning based on multi-dimensional empathy theory and 21 stcentury skills theory can improve CPS skills in medical students. These results have practical implications for learning and suggest that educators should consider incorporating reflective learning strategies that focus on empathy and 21 stcentury skills to enhance CPS skills in their curricula.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

When putting forward the theoretical framework of skills and competencies in the 21st century, the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development takes complex problem-solving (CPS) skills as an important component and brings them into the evaluation system of the Program for International Student Assessment [ 1 ]. Previous research results have proved that there is a significant positive correlation between CPS skills and academic performance [ 2 ], that is, the stronger the problem-solving skill, the better the academic performance. Similarly, it is also considered to have a great influence on career selection [ 3 ], career development [ 4 ], and job competency training [ 5 ]. Therefore, the improvement of the above-mentioned comprehensive qualities, such as learning ability and post competence, and the cultivation of CPS skills, has been emphasized by a variety of teaching strategies, such as problem-based learning (PBL) [ 6 ], context-based learning (CBL) [ 7 ], situational simulation [ 8 ], and reflective learning [ 9 ].

As an important process of metacognition, reflective learning is closely related to CPS skills. Gadbury-Amyot et al. claimed that the use of reflection and writing as educational strategies to promote critical thinking and problem-solving is one of the best ways for students to express their thought processes [ 10 ]. Exploration to improve CPS skills based on reflective learning and training can be seen in medicine, computer science, mathematics, and other industries. According to Bernack, establishing problem-solving training courses could feasibly enhance the abilities of pre-service teachers [ 11 ]. Kellogg suggested that reflection and writing, as educational strategies to promote critical thinking and problem-solving skill, is one of the best ways to improve students’ expression ability and logical thinking [ 12 ]. “Reflective learning” is a common way of exploring problems and solutions in the deliberative environment, a process of learning through experience, and is a necessary learning tool in professional education [ 13 ]. Reflective learning includes journal writing, peer reflection, self-reflection, and group discussion under the guidance of teachers [ 14 ]. Illeris suggested that the result of reflective learning spans cognitive, psychodynamic, and social-societal dimensions [ 15 ]. Through its influence on students’ behavior, thoughts, and emotions, it realizes the training and improvement of students’ various abilities. It has gradually developed into a more efficient and autonomous learning model and has become an indispensable educational and learning tool for many professionals [ 16 ]. Many experts suggest that the implementation of reflective learning can improve students’ critical thinking [ 17 ], insight [ 18 ], empathic concern [ 19 ], computational thinking [ 20 ], and other skills, and this improvement of a variety of thinking modes and abilities will eventually lead to improvement of their CPS skills [ 21 ]. Our research on the factors affecting the CPS skills is based on reflective learning.

The purpose of human problem solving is to promote the understanding of human thinking through a detailed investigation of the way people solve difficult problems, such as logic or chess. Unlike computer simulations, human problem solving is influenced by psychological factors that cannot be ignored [ 22 ]. Therefore, problem solving is dynamic and needs to consider the influence of speculation, social background, and culture, while CPS skills emphasize the process of successful interaction between the problem solver and the dynamic task environment [ 23 ]. CPS skills are collections of self-regulating psychological processes necessary in the face of complex and dynamic non-routine situations across different domains [ 24 ], and comprises a combination of skills, abilities, motivation, and other psychological structures [ 25 , 26 ]. The factors that impact CPS skills are complex and include cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Research shows that the development of a variety of thinking modes and abilities, such as algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, creativity, critical thinking, personal distress, fantasy, perspective-taking, and empathic concern, all affect the problem-solving skill in varying degrees [ 27 , 28 ]. Among them, empathetic concern and critical thinking have been proven to affect problem-solving skill by many studies. After comprehensively exploring the emerging research on the impact of the factors on the CPS skills, we found that previous studies mainly focused on a single causality in the improvement of problem-solving skill, while there is a lack of overall theory to relate variables to each other, which means that we need to integrate different theories to advance existing research and focuson how CPS skills can be effectively trained and improved.

Theoretical background

The literature analysis of CPS skills reveals the current research status. Based on the relevant theories of skills needed in the 21st century [ 1 ], individuals use analytical, reasoning, and cooperative skills to identify and solve problems consistent with their areas of interest [ 29 ]. Kocak proposes that problem-solving skills are shaped by algorithmic thinking, creativity, cooperativity, critical thinking, digital literacy, and effective communication, and develops a model with critical thinking as a mediating factor [ 21 ]. Developing solutions for complex problems is a complicated process, and individuals require critical thinking skills [ 21 , 30 ] to do so. Critical thinking often occurs at the same time as CPS skills and is one of the core objectives of general education in all subjects of higher education [ 29 ]. Critical thinking, closely related to reflective learning [ 17 ], which has been emphasized in many studies, especially in the implementation of learning strategies including reflective learning. In problem-based learning and case-based learning, instructors encourage learners to use critical reflection to engage with subject matter and to develop their own practice in closing any knowledge gaps that may exist [ 31 ]. Additionally, digital literacy involves the ability to assess the accuracy and value of online resources [ 32 ]. In this study, reflective learning was the primary learning strategy [ 33 ]; therefore, digital literacy skills were not observed in detail. Drawing on the above analysis, we developed a theoretical model that identifies algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity as antecedents, and critical thinking as an intermediary variable that influences CPS skills.

Another major area related to affecting CPS skills is empathic concern. The research suggests that students with a higher level of cognitive empathy show more positive attitudes and deal with problems more effectively [ 34 ]. In essence, empathetic concern fosters values, beliefs, attitudes, and assumptions, and affect the CPS skills from the perspective of execution [ 35 , 36 ]. Some studies suggest that reflective learning improves empathy [ 37 ]. Based on Davis’s Interpersonal Reactivity Index [ 38 ], empathy was divided into four dimensions mentioned: Empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress. Nevertheless, some scholars disagree that personal distress belongs to the category of empathy. Personal distress is defined as an over-arousal caused by the lack of boundaries between oneself and others [ 39 , 40 ]. Some studies show that personal distress leads to egoism and overwhelms altruistic activities mediated by empathetic concern [ 41 ]. And there is a statistically significant correlation between personal distress and empathetic concern [ 42 ]. Therefore, we still adhere to the view that the two cannot be regarded as mutually exclusive emotions, bringing personal distress into the scope of our research and exploring its role in CPS skills. Empathetic concern has been proved to associate with prosocial behavior [ 43 ]. In the relationship between empathic concern and prosocial concern, empathic concern elicits an approach orientation toward the target [ 44 ] and is used as a mediator variable in some models. For example, some studies consider empathic concern and personal distress are both mediators of perspective-taking to helping behavior [ 45 ]. Based on the above analysis, we built our theoretical model and assume that personal distress, perspective-taking, and fantasy as antecedents and empathetic concern as intermediary variables that affect the CPS skills.

Above all, the empathic concern and the critical thinking are two remarkable characteristics of the CPS skills, which can play a common role in the CPS skills [ 46 ], however, there is a lack of overall theory to connect them, which means that different theories need to be integrated to promote research. The comprehensive study of the combination of the two aspects can better understand how to improve CPS skills, which cannot be provided by any theory alone. Moreover, the results on the factors affecting the CPS skills also show some inconsistencies. For example, Batson believes that personal distress in empathy inhibits the development of problem-solving skills [ 41 ], whereas Mora disagrees [ 47 ]. A possible reasonable explanation for these contradictory results is that the previous studies on the factors influencing problem-solving skill mainly adopted traditional symmetric methods (such as regression and SEM), which did not fully capture the complexity of the factors that influence problem-solving skills, and the factors affecting the CPS skills are often based on multiple causalities rather than a single causal relationship. Simply evaluating symmetric relationships might lead to divergent results, thus masking the complexity of the problem-solving skill. Considering the complex nature of CPS skills under the condition of reflective learning, it is necessary to check the symmetric and asymmetric relationships between structures to fully understand the strategies and methods to improve CPS skills, therefore, PLS-SEM [ 48 ] and fsQCA [ 49 ] were used in our study comprehensively.

Research model and hypothesis development

Designing the pls-sem research model.

Critical thinking in the field of cognition and empathic concern in the field of emotion are representatives of two different thinking modes affecting the CPS skills. PLS-SEM assumes that fantasy, perspective-taking, personal distress, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity have a direct impact on the CPS skills. Empathetic concern and critical thinking play an intermediary role between these relationships and the CPS skills (Fig.  1 A).

figure 1

Partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) conceptual model and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA) conceptual model: ( A ) The PLS-SEM conceptual model. ( B ) The fsQCA conceptual model

Personal distress and CPS skills

The definition of personal distress in this study pertains to the discomfort and anxiety that respondents experience when observing negative experiences of others, including fear, apprehension, and discomfort. Personal distress is an aspect of emotional empathy [ 38 ]. Some studies show that personal distress and empathy are complex and dynamic emotional experiences [ 50 ]. Personal distress, as an indicator of self-other differentiation and emotional regulation, is a kind of negative emotion. Excessive personal distress can lead to emotional regulation and interpersonal difficulties [ 51 ]. These studies advocate reducing personal distress to relieve stress [ 52 ]. The healthcare sector prioritizes a patient-centered healthcare model, which mandates that we respond to patients’ emotional distress with this principle in mind. However, in practice, health professionals tend to regard emotional health problems as “routine”; therefore, it is necessary to put patients’ emotional and identity issues in the dominant position of the marginal biomedical model used by health professionals [ 53 ]. However, empathic pain is crucial to Hoffman’s moral development framework. He believed that pain can cause significant effects that might lead to action [ 54 ]. Moitra’s research also supports the positive effect of personal distress on problem-solving skill [ 47 ]. Reflection encourages individuals to confront their own embarrassing and uncomfortable past experiences, learn from their errors, and enhance their CPS skills [ 55 ].

Fantasy and CPS skills

Fantasy acts on all aspects of reflective learning. First, to some extent, our brains process information and decisions in an irrational way, and reflection contributes to the cultivation of irrational thinking [ 56 ]. The improvement of subjects’ irrational thinking, including fantasy, can be promoted through reflective learning. Research indicates that individuals with higher fantasy and perspective-taking skills tend to have stronger social understanding [ 57 ]. Second, the development of imagination and fantasy is an important part of cultivating empathic concern [ 58 ]. This is because people understand the world through fantasy, and fantasy gives people hope that the world will become a better place [ 59 ]. For example, Melissa McInnis Brown’s research showed that children who play using fantasies are better at sharing emotions than their peers [ 60 ]. Many studies have proven the role of fantasy in problem-solving. For example, David Weibel pointed out that one can effectively use imagination in an environment, such as in artistic expression or problem-solving [ 61 ]. Fantasy is an imaginative way to find creative solutions that can help people predict the realization of creative structures [ 61 ]. From a sociological point of view, scholars usually regard fantasy as an important factor in cultivating children’s prosocial behaviour [ 57 , 62 ]. Empathic concern requires a person or the whole team to have an overall and largely unconscious “feeling” in terms of emotions, body language, previous experiences, and interpersonal relationships; therefore, this requires significant support from the fantasy system [ 63 ].

Perspective-taking and CPS skills

The effectiveness of group problem solving heavily depends on group member interactions and group composition. For perspective-taking, it provides the possibility for effective communication, which mainly affects the effective presentation of information, effective understanding of that information, conflict resolution, and cooperative interaction [ 64 , 65 ]. In management, perspective-taking has become an important factor in teamwork to solve problems [ 66 ]. During perspective-taking, the problem-solving process can be facilitated by promoting empathic concern, which is evident in the subjects’ cognitive dimension. For example, Falk found that perspective-taking leads to more creative solutions, and team members are more cooperative and facilitate more effective communication [ 64 ]. Bethune and Brown suggested that reflection affects the professional identity of patients by encouraging personal insights and providing different perspectives on patient interaction [ 67 ]. Reflection requires us to think about the past and sum up experiences and lessons from it. Thinking about problems from the standpoint of others can circumvent the limitations of our perspective of looking at problems only through ourselves and can promote the solution of complex problems.

Based on the points discussed above, we propose the following assumptions:

Personal distress is positively related to the CPS skills.

Fantasy is positively related to the CPS skills.

Perspective-taking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Algorithmic thinking and CPS skills

Algorithm thinking draws lessons from the algorithms of computers and artificial intelligence, which enables people to think and deal with things in parallel, process things in data, carry on data and logical reasoning to things, and finally achieve the goal of completing plans and tasks. As one of the core skills in the 21st century, algorithmic thinking abstractly and logically determines the elements used to solve problems through analysis [ 28 ]. One of the major applications of algorithmic thinking is jigsaw puzzle-based learning, which aims to make subjects think about how to build and solve problems, and improve their critical analysis and problem-solving skills [ 68 ]. Hasan Gürbüz leveraged straightforward visual and language templates to help individuals develop models and analyze information about events through games, resulting in improved problem-solving skills [ 69 ]. This mode of thinking, based on logic and steps, is very important for the development of critical thinking and computational thinking [ 28 ]. Many studies have shown that there is a positive correlation between algorithmic thinking and critical thinking [ 70 ]. In reflective learning, algorithmic thinking plays a significant role in computing, as evidenced in this study by recording a short video that necessitates organizing large amounts of data to develop suitable algorithms for analysis [ 71 ].

Creativity and CPS skills

Creativity affects our lives and is vital to the progress of society [ 72 ]. The definition of creativity highlights the integration of novel (original, unexpected) and appropriate (useful, adaptive concerning task constraint) ideas [ 73 ]. Since the 20th century, a large number of scholars in various fields have paid attention to creativity and CPS skills. Creativity is a valuable skill while designing solutions to new challenges that arise in developing societies [ 74 ]. For instance, Garrett noted that creativity plays a crucial role in problem-solving [ 75 ]. In many studies, creativity and critical thinking are interdependent, and creative tasks can improve people’s creativity [ 76 ]. In reflective learning, we utilize divergent thinking that frequently enhances our creativity.

Cooperativity and CPS skills

Many critics believe that cooperativity plays an important role in the cultivation of critical thinking [ 77 ]. Cooperativity receives considerable attention in the learning process due to its association with effective communication. For example, service-learning attaches great importance to cooperation, democratic citizenship, and moral responsibility in the learning process [ 78 ], and preschool educational institutions need to improve the experience through the collaborative exchange, to create favorable conditions for educators to re-examine educational activities, and determine the direction of new relationships through observation [ 79 ]. In reflective learning, subjects become aware of their contradictions and gain valid information, and critically assess peer opinions through active communication, which advances their ideas for program and CPS skills improvement.

Algorithmic thinking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Creativity is positively related to the CPS skills.

Cooperativity is positively related to the CPS skills.

Mediators and CPS skills

This study assumes that empathetic concern and critical thinking act as mediators between the CPS skills and their antecedents.

In Gibbs’s theory, the emotional dimension is a very important aspect of reflective learning [ 80 ]. Madeline Kelly’s research showed that reflection has a positive effect on the improvement of cognitive empathy [ 81 ]; however, there are few studies on the effect of reflective learning on empathy. Cognitive empathy includes fantasy and perspective-taking, while the emotional empathy includes personal distress and empathic concern [ 82 ]. Research shows that the concept of emotional empathy is state empathy, with the focus on altruism [ 83 , 84 ]. Emotional empathy plays an important role in patient-nurse communication [ 85 ]. Failure to deal with or understand emotions will make it difficult for nurses to think rationally and critically about issues that are important to nursing practice [ 86 ]. Therefore, we cannot ignore the influence of empathic concern on the CPS skills in reflective learning. We assumed that the ability of empathic concern can increase altruism and help to improve CPS skills. However, personal distress is usually considered to lead to egoism, which is not conducive to the formation of altruism [ 41 ]. In-depth investigation is necessary to understand its effect on CPS skills. As an important factor in prosocial behavior, the empathic concern serving as a mediator between cognitive behavior and prosocial behavior [ 87 ]. Based on the theories of O’Brien and Gülseven, we constructed a CPS skills model with empathic concern as the mediating variable [ 88 , 89 ].

Effective reflection is characterized by purposeful, focused, and questioning [ 90 ]. In the process of reflection, this mode of thinking requires us to think critically and center on the results. Reflective learning, also known as critical reflection [ 17 ], emphasizes the use of critical thinking. Many critics affirm the results of critical reflection [ 91 , 92 , 93 ]. Parrish and Crookes found that among nursing graduates, reflection helped them to solve problems through thoughtful reasoning and to develop strategies for self-monitoring of their professional competence [ 94 ]. Critical thinking is typically rational thinking, and through combining theory with practice, exploring the similarities and differences between theoretical knowledge and practical experience, and considering a variety of different viewpoints and opinions, the effect of reflective learning can be enhanced. Therefore, speculative reflection is designed to help us identify our shortcomings and think about how to correct and improve them. Critical thinking is widely recognized as an important skill in mediating CPS skills [ 10 ]. Based on the research of Kocak and Tee, we also view critical thinking as an intermediary variable, playing a mediating role in algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity within CPS skills [ 21 , 95 ].

Personal distress indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Fantasy indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Perspective-taking indirectly affects the CPS skills through empathic concern.

Empathic concern is positively related to the CPS skills.

Algorithmic thinking indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Creativity indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Cooperativity indirectly affects the CPS skills through critical thinking.

Critical thinking is positively related to the CPS skills.

Designing the fsQCA configuration model

In this study, a Venn diagram is used to design the fsQCA configuration model (Fig.  1 B), which was used to explore the causal model for improving CPS skills. In the diagram, arrow A represents a combination of perspective-taking, fantasy, and personal distress, and adds configurations that affect the CPS skills through, or including, empathetic concern. Arrow B represents a combination of algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity, and adds configurations that affect the CPS skills through, or including, critical thinking. Arrow C represents the combination of all the variables and represents the complex interaction of these factors to predict the resulting conditions.

Participants

Participants were 136 freshmen and medical majors from a university in southeastern China (‾Xage = 18.47, female = 82.35%, male = 17.65%). The inclusion criterion comprised students who had conducted reflective learning. The exclusion criteria comprised: (1) Students who did not make reflective videos, or (2) students suspected of plagiarizing reflective learning achievements. A total of 163 cases were included in the empirical study of reflective learning, and 136 effective samples were recovered, with an effective recovery rate of 83.44%.

Design and procedure

After receiving appropriate online training, classroom teachers implemented a reflective learning curriculum design among medical students in the autumn of 2021 (Fig.  2 ). Based on the Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Courses, the two rounds of teaching plan lasted a total of 14 weeks was design. In the first round of reflective learning, subjects were asked to read relevant literature, watch relevant video materials, etc., and carry out online learning. They were then asked to record learning videos on their own, and then upload the videos, followed by a double-blind mutual evaluation of learning videos between online students. In the second round of reflective learning, students adjusted their reflective learning according to the feedback from the previous round of mutual evaluation, implemented a second round of deeper material learning exploration, improved their reflective video, and summarized the main points of reflective learning. Teachers evaluated the reflective videos and learning points offline, and students learned and summarized according to the evaluation results. After the end of the entire process, we issued a competency assessment questionnaire to measure learners’ competency levels and the data was collected.

figure 2

Reflective learning process

To measure the constructs under study, existing scales were used (see Table  1 for items associated with each construct and scale reliabilities).

A questionnaire was developed based on the existing mature scale, and the items were slightly adjusted according to the model. The relationship between the retained items and the dimensions was not complementary. Improvement of CPS skills is described as a structure composed of six antecedent variables and two mediating variables with different ways of thinking. The Davis Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) was used for personal distress, fantasy, perspective-taking, and empathic concern [ 38 ], and the Computational Thinking Scale (CTS) was used for critical thinking, algorithmic thinking, creativity, problem solving, and cooperativity [ 74 , 96 , 97 ]. We structured it as personal distress (three items), fantasy (three items), and perspective-taking (two items) as ante-dependent variables, and the mediating effect of empathic concern (three items) on CPS skills (three items) was directly and through empathic concern (3 items). Similarly, algorithmic thinking (3 items), creativity (three items), and cooperativity (three items) acted as ante-variables, both directly and through the mediating effect of critical thinking (three items). All items were evaluated using a Likert 5-point scale, 5 = strongly agree, 4 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 2 = disagree, 1 = strongly disagree, and the scores of items in reverse scoring were reversed. Entries for reverse scoring are marked with * in Table  1 . The questionnaire was translated into Chinese and distributed after discussion with experts.

Data analysis

We use multiple methods to analyze the data. First, PLS-SEM was carried out on the data through Smart-PLS 3.0 software to adapt the complex model analysis and explore the impact of various factors [ 48 ].

We measured the characteristics of the structure using internal consistency reliability, convergence validity, and discrimination validity. Internal consistency reliability was measured using the alpha and combinatorial reliability of Cronbach. And we checked the collinearity of the internal model and evaluated the deviation of the method using a variance inflation factor (VIF). According to the research objectives, we tested two models with different paths with significant correlations. The direct predictive effects of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking, as well as the mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking, on CPS skills were tested. A nonparametric, bias-corrected bootstrap with 5,000 subsamples and a 95% confidence interval was used. The structural model was evaluated by R² and by the significance of the estimated value of pathway relationships. The significance of pathway coefficients was evaluated using the bootstrap subsamples, and the structural model was evaluated using 5000 bootstrap subsamples [ 98 ]. R² values of 0.25, 0.50, or 0.75 are considered weak, moderate, and significant, respectively.

Although PLS-SEM can handle both external (measurement) and internal (structural) models [ 98 ], it is limited by symmetry. Therefore, we used fsQCA 3.1 software [ 49 ] to analyze asymmetry and obtain a sufficient causal combination configuration to study the complex relationship between variables more comprehensively and in detail. According to the fsQCA user guide, data calibration, truth table construction, and causal condition analysis are necessary steps in the process of data analysis [ 49 ]. In the first step, we converted the ordinary data into fuzzy sets by setting the original values from the Likert scale, which corresponded to full membership, cross-over anchors, and full non-membership based on Kallmuenzer’s analysis [ 99 ]. The second step is to construct the truth table and generate different combinations of causal conditions that are sufficient to affect the CPS skills by specifying a consistent cutoff value as the natural breakpoint in the consistency and the case number threshold as 1. The third, we analyzed the necessity of all the variables (critical thinking, creativity, algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, empathetic concern, perspective-taking, personal distress and fantasy) to the CPS skills, and the antecedent variables for mediate variables (critical thinking and empathic concern), and the necessity of mediating variables to the outcome variables. It is generally believed that a condition or combination of conditions is “necessary” or “almost always necessary” when the consistency score is higher than 0.9 [ 49 ]. Finally, we use standard analysis to obtain “intermediate solutions” (i.e., partial logical remainders are incorporated into the solution) to identify causal patterns that affect CPS skills.

The result of PLS-SEM

Evaluation of the reflection measurement model.

Except for the perspective-taking, the Cronbach’s alpha in the other dimensions was generally more than 0.7, reaching the standard recommended by Cohen (Table  1 ) [ 100 ]. After examining the external loads in the external model, we observed that most of the loads were more than 0.7, while the PD1 project was still less than 0.7. After checking the Cronbach’s alpha and average variance extracted (AVE), we confirmed that this factor had no negative effect on our research [ 98 ], and was thus retained the project. The sample size of the model is small (less than 300), and the items considered by perspective-taking are 2 (less than 3), so Cronbach’s alpha is easily less than 0.6. The alpha of perspective-taking is more than 0.5, which is still in a slightly plausible range. Therefore, we kept the item of perspective-taking. Secondly, the square root of AVE was greater than 0.5, which accords with the convergence validity [ 101 ]. In addition, we used the Fornell-Larker criteria to evaluate the discriminant validity (Table  2 ).

Evaluation of formative measurement models

The results showed that the VIF of all constructs was lower than the threshold of 3.3 (see Additional file. 1 ) [ 98 ]. In order to further analyze, this study evaluated the quality by blindfolding program (Q 2 ) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The results showed that SRMR = 0.079, not exceeding 0.09 [ 102 ]. The blindfold program showed that Q 2 was greater than 0, which verified the predictive correlation of the research model [ 103 ].

Structural model evaluation

Evaluation of the structural model showed that the R² value was reasonable for exploratory research. Meanwhile, the direct pathway effect of fantasy, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and cooperativity on CPS skills was not significant ( p  > 0.05), and the pathway effect of personal distress on empathic concern was also not significant ( p  > 0.05). The other variables showed significant influences on CPS skills ( p  < 0.05) (Table  3 ). After deleting the insignificant pathways, we built a structural model between the CPS skills and the influencing factors (critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress) (Fig.  3 ). Compared with the hypothetical model, mediating effects of empathic concern and critical thinking were observed; however, personal distress only had a direct effect on CPS skills, which was consistent with the previous view that empathic concern and personal distress should be discussed [ 51 ].

figure 3

Path model and partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) estimates

The result of fsQCA

The results of necessity showed that only cooperativity and creativity are necessary conditions for critical thinking (see Additional file. 2 , Additional file. 3 , and Additional file. 4 ).

FsQCA assessed the complex causal combination that led to improved CPS skills (Tables  4 , 5 , 6 and 7 ). The solution provided clues for each different pathway to the result, with all consistency values being higher than 0.8, and most coverage values being between 0.240 and 0.839 [ 104 ].

As shown in Table  4 , there are six approaches to the final model of complex conditions that lead to high CPS skills, among which the top three in terms of coverage are: (1) To achieve high CPS skills through high critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking (consistency = 0.974, coverage = 0.354). (2) Under conditions of high critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, and creativity, combined with high empathic concern, personal distress, and fantasy, the CPS skills can be improved (consistency = 0.950, coverage = 0.352). (3) A high level of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, personal distress, perspective-taking, and fantasy (consistency = 0.950, coverage = 0.340) can promote the improvement of CPS skills.

To examine the mediating effect of empathic concern and critical thinking on the CPS skill, we analyzed the complex causality of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, and empathic concern. The results showed in Table  5 indicated that the complex causal statement of fantasy, personal distress, perspective-taking, and empathic concern is one way, i.e., high perspective-taking and fantasy improves empathic concern skill (consistency = 0.821; coverage = 0.612), which supports the H7b and H7c assumptions in the SEM model. By contrast, the results of analyzing the complex causal relationship of creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking for critical thinking showed that there is a pathway for the complex causal statement of creativity, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, and critical thinking (consistency = 0.867, coverage = 0.760), which will lead to improved critical thinking ability. This supported the hypotheses of H8a, H8b, and H8c in the SEM model.

The results of further analysis of the complex causal relationship between empathic concern and critical thinking for improved CPS skills (Table  7 ) showed that high empathic concern and critical thinking (consistency = 0.890, coverage = 0.550) will lead to improved CPS skills. This supported the H7d and H8d assumptions in the SEM model.

Discussion and conclusion

Theoretical implication.

To improve its ability to deal with complex practical problems, education has been committed to providing teaching measures that can stimulate subjects’ rational and irrational thinking. Healthcare professionals who utilize reflective learning must apply empathetic concern and critical thinking to confront challenges with high-quality solutions. Although previous studies confirmed the positive effects of empathic concern [ 19 ], and critical thinking [ 17 ] on CPS skills through symmetrical analysis, few studies have tested empathic concern and critical thinking at the same time. There is a dearth of studies that specifically investigate the factors that affect CPS skills in the context of reflective learning. And the previous studies on the factors influencing CPS skills mainly adopted traditional symmetric methods (such as regression and SEM), which did not fully capture the complexity behind the factors of influencing CPS skills. For instance, Hwang discovered that collaboration plays a crucial role in problem-solving, whereas communication may not be essential. In contrast, Kocak holds a contrasting perspective [ 21 , 105 ]. The factors affecting the CPS skills are often based on multiple causalities rather than a single causal relationship. Therefore, based on the theory of multi-dimensional empathy [ 38 ] and 21st-century skills [ 21 ], we analyzed the data of 136 medical students undergoing reflective learning using PLS-SEM and fsQCA, and constructed a hypothetical model to examine the relationships between the CPS skills and influence factors (critical thinking, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, personal distress).

The PLS-SEM results (Table  3 ) showed that a variety of attributes can affect the CPS skills, among which critical thinking and empathic concern play an intermediary role between most antecedents and CPS skills. The fsQCA results partly verified the mediating effect of critical thinking and empathetic concern (Tables  5 and 6 ). In the PLS-SEM results (Table  3 ), personal distress was identified to directly affect the CPS skills; and the effect of personal distress on empathic concern was not shown in the fsQCA solution (Table  5 ), which proved that personal distress directly affects the CPS skills without the intermediary of empathetic concern. This result is similar to that of Jeon [ 106 ]; however, he believed that there is a negative correlation between personal distress and problem solving, which might be related to the different learning patterns (reflective learning) used in this study. Personal distress is a necessary process in reflective learning because the motivation of prosocial behavior eases our uncomfortable state of mind by reducing the disgusting and awakening cues sent out by others [ 107 ]. Reflection urges us to face these emotions and draw lessons from them. Hoffman noted that excessive personal distress can turn others-oriented motivation into self-directed motivation, thus reducing the occurrence of prosocial behavior [ 54 ], which emphasizes the differential treatment of personal distress in different learning modes. In addition, perspective-taking was identified to affect the CPS skills directly (Table  3 ; C1 in Table  4 ) and indirectly (Table  3 ; C1 in Table  5 ). Therefore, some of the results obtained from fSQCA validated the conclusions of PLS-SEM to some extent (Table  8 ).

The fsQCA results provided more configuration solutions of complex causality, which extended the results of PLS-SEM and further revealed the complexity of affecting the CPS skills. For example, the fsQCA results in Table  7 not only proved the mediating effect of empathetic concern and critical thinking, but also suggest that they work together to affect the CPS skills. This demonstrates that CPS skills are impacted by both rational and irrational thinking, and positive emotions play a critical role in fostering CPS skills [ 108 ]. In Table  4 cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, critical thinking, and personal distress all appear in forward solutions with high coverage (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ). This suggested that personal distress, cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, and critical thinking can be regarded as the core conditions to affect the CPS skills, and these conditions make an important contribution in the context of reflective learning. Researches by Chen [ 77 ], Garrett [ 75 ], Geisinger [ 70 ] and Ellis [ 17 ] respectively believed that collaboration, creativity, algorithmic thinking and critical thinking play an important role in CPS skills, and Sze [ 109 ] believed that personal distress could have a positive impact on prosocial behavior and altruism. The studies above provide are similar to our perspective.

Critical thinking is not only one of the basic skills in the 21st Century but also a key ability in reflective learning [ 110 , 111 ]. The process of questioning and reorganizing critical thinking is key to reflective learning. The complex structure of the problem-solving process requires critical thinking skills to find different solutions [ 30 , 112 , 113 , 114 ]. Table  4 higher coverage solutions (C1, C2, C3, NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ) and Table  7 higher coverage solutions (C1, NC1 in Table  7 ) showed that critical thinking ability training is helpful to improve a subject’s CPS skills. By contrast, a lack of critical thinking training is not conducive to improving CPS skills (NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ; NC1 in Table  7 ). Critical thinking is widely considered to be a competency closely linked to CPS skills [ 29 ], and our study approves this perspective. Moreover, cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking also appear in the forward solutions with high coverage (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ), combined with the mediating effect of critical thinking on cooperativity, creativity, algorithmic thinking, and the CPS skills. It is logical that the antecedents of critical thinking, such as cooperativity, creativity and algorithmic thinking, also play a positive role in the CPS skills. The result is similar to the findings of Özgenel [ 115 ], who believed that critical thinking and creative thinking affected problem-solving skill through decision-making style. These results suggested that we should pay attention to the cultivation of a critical thinking ability, especially through the cultivation of cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking, which positively and significantly improve a subject’s ability to solve complex problems.

Empathetic concern relationship with the complex configuration between its antecedent variables provides new ideas and insights to improve our ability to solve complex problems. Empathic concern, as a key factor of prosocial behavior [ 43 ], is also of positive significance to CPS skills in this study. The higher coverage solutions (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ; NC1, NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ; C1 in Table  7 ; NC1 in Table  7 ) showed that training in empathic concern is beneficial to improve a subject’s CPS skills, while a lack of empathic concern training is not conducive to improving CPS skills (NC1, NC2 in Table  4 ; NC1 in Table  7 ). By contrast, fantasy (C2, C3 in Table  4 ) and perspective-taking (C1, C3 in Table  4 ) appeared among the forward solutions with higher coverage in Table  4 . Combined with the mediating effect among empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and the CPS skills, it is not difficult for us to understand that the empathic concern antecedent variable: fantasy, and perspective-taking, also have positive significance for the CPS skills. This result aligns with the research findings of Hashmi [ 57 ] and Davenport [ 66 ]. Moreover, in the absence of empathetic concern, the pathway support of the combination of fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress for CPS skills also verified this positive significance from the other side (C3 in Table  4 ). However, personal distress (C1, C2, C3 in Table  4 ) appears independently in the forward solution, with high coverage in Table  4 , which verifies the direct effect of personal distress on the CPS skills, which was consistent with the results of PLSSEM. These further confirmed the theory of Dorner and Funke, who suggested that complex and dynamic non-routine situations across different domains require a collection of self-regulating psychological processes and a creative combination of knowledge and strategies, and is influenced by motivation and emotion, especially in a high-stakes environment[24]. In addition, according to the observation of the reverse solution of Table  4 , the combination of negative perspectivetaking and negative personal distress will be conducive to the low-level CPS skills (NC1, NC2, NC3, NC5 in Table  4 ). Interestingly, fantasy appeared not only in the forward solution with high coverage (C2, C3 in Table  4 ), but also in the inverse solution with high coverage (NC2, NC3 in Table  4 ), which seemed to suggest that the contribution of fantasy to improving CPS skills is neutral, which requires further research.

Consistent with the principle of causal asymmetry, fsQCA suggested that solutions generated by the same attributes in different areas might have the opposite impact on CPS skills, depending on how they combine or interact with other attributes. The lack or negation of some positive factors will lead to improved CPS skills, while the existence of some negative factors might also lead to similar results, depending on how they are configured with the other factors. For example, solution 4 in Table  4 shows that in the absence of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy, a combination of empathetic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking could also have a positive effect on the improvement of CPS skills. There is a paucity of literature exploring the effects of empathetic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking on CPS skills under conditions of low levels of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy. These insights provide new ideas for exploring improvements in CPS skills. Although PLS-SEM can verify the predetermined relationship between previous factors and the results of interest, it cannot provide these insights.

In addition, considering the complex nature of problem-solving skill under the condition of reflective learning, it is necessary to check the linear and nonlinear relationships between structures to fully understand the strategies and methods to improve CPS skills. In this study, as an ideal approach, PLS-SEM was used to identify the linear (symmetric) causal relationship between the improvement of CPS skills and influence factors. The fSQCA was used to identify the nonlinear (asymmetric), heterogeneous, and dynamic interactions between antecedents and behavioral results. The fSQCA improved identifying sufficient causal conditions for outcomes. The comprehensive application of PLS-SEM and fsQCA helped capture complex multiple causalities in the improvement of CPS skills, which makes theoretical contributions in terms of analytical techniques.

Practical implications

Aquino believes that the implementation of reflective learning strategies is conducive to the improvement of CPS skills [ 116 ], which is of practical significance for the design of learning strategies and training tools, including reflective learning. The PLS-SEM results showed that perspective-taking, as an important condition for affected CPS skills, not only plays a role through the intermediary effect of empathetic concern, but also directly affects the CPS skills. The researchers and learners can train subjects to think for others in the form of team communication and exchange of views. For the sake of others, it is necessary to think about CPS skills solutions from multiple angles and more comprehensively, by thinking about problems from the standpoint and perspective of others. Therefore, it is necessary to adopt evidence-based strategies for training to improve the CPS skills.

While the fsQCA results confirmed the PLS-SEM results, in turn its complex configuration helps researchers and learners to make more informed decisions about learning methods to improve CPS skills. The derived pathways indicated that there is more than one causal configuration that can improve CPS skills, and how to improve depends on a combination of attributes. For example, our results showed that the high level of critical thinking and its antecedent attributes, combined with the high level of empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking, will lead to improvement of the CPS skills. The lack of critical thinking, cooperativity, algorithmic thinking, creativity, and fantasy, which to some extent emphasizes the utility of empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking (Table  4 solution 4), make it necessary to pay attention to training medical students in empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective-taking using reflective learning. Aligned with our own research, it was acknowledged reflective learning as a potent method to enhance empathy [ 37 ]. Medical reflection should focus on cultivating the ability to speculate on materials and self-views, and at the same time, understand decision-making from the situation of others and feel the emotions of others to trigger empathy. Improvement of CPS skills should not only emphasize the reduction of personal distress, but also should look at the role of personal distress critically. At the same time, it also reminds us that we should fully consider the training situation of the subjects in the design of learning strategies. Critical thinking and its antecedents are regarded as the key solutions in fsQCA, which suggests that we can focus on the reflective learning mode when we train subjects for critical thinking, creativity, cooperativity, and algorithmic thinking. We should also consciously use this kind of thinking to solve problems in the process of reflective learning. In the design of other learning strategies, training in critical thinking ability and its antecedent variables, cooperativity, creativity, and algorithmic thinking, can effectively help subjects to improve their CPS skills.

Based on our understanding of how empathic concern and critical thinking work together to improve the CPS skills, we suggest that real and complex problems in life be taken as examples in the choice of reflective teaching strategies, to involve a series of related skills and characteristics, and fully exercise the two modes of thinking. This is because, in reflective learning, subjects internalize the thinking skills taught by others into their own thinking skills, cultivating the ability to monitor and reflect on the whole problem-solving process, and helping subjects to extract useful strategies, experiences, and patterns into their cognitive structure, thereby improving their CPS skills and accumulating more experience for possible intuitive thinking. This is more suitable for problems based on real-life, which is in line with the medical learning problem-based learning and case-based learning models.

In this study, a hypothetical model of the relationship between the CPS skills and influencing factors (critical thinking, cooperation, creativity, algorithmic thinking, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective-taking, and personal distress) was constructed and validated. The model confirmed the mediating effect of critical thinking and empathic concern on the CPS skills, the direct effect of personal distress, and the direct and indirect effect of perspective-taking on the CPS skills. Besides, fsQCA results provided a variety of configurations that enhanced the improvement of CPS skills. The findings not only enriched the theoretical system of affecting CPS skills, but also provided practical guidance for the development of learning strategies and assessment tools aimed at improving CPS skills.

Limitations and future research

Although this study enriches the theoretical and practical knowledge concerning the relationship between CPS skills and critical thinking, empathic concern, and other variables, it also has some limitations. First, the subjects were beginners in terms of reflective learning under the guidance of teachers, and lack experience in reflective learning, which might affect the accuracy and applicability of variables to some extent. In future research, we will improve these shortcomings, practice reflective learning practices in more subjects, and validate the model in a broader learning strategy, which would be very meaningful. Second, based on the model of this study, it is necessary to enrich the paths and develop a variety of learning and training tools to improve the CPS skills in the future research. The development of assessment tools for factors related to the measurement of CPS skills will facilitate targeted training and realize personalized learning practice guidance.

Data Availability

The data sets used and / or analyzed in this study have not been made public. If there is a reasonable need, they can be obtained from and provided by the corresponding author of this article.

Abbreviations

Complex problem-solving

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development

Partial least square structural equation modeling

  • Fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis

Average variance extracted

Variance inflation factor

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

Computational Thinking Scale

Structural equation modeling.

Co-operation OfE, Development. PISA 2012 results: Creative problem solving: Students’ skills in tackling real-life problems (Volume V). In.: OECD Publishing Pisa; 2014.

Amirian K. P-833-To study the effect of problem-solving skill education on first year high school male students in songhor koliayi township in academic year. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27(S1):1–1. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0924-9338(12)75000-9 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Ayalew MM. Multilevel analysis of entrepreneurial intention of engineering graduating students in Ethiopia. J Appl Econ. 2021;24(1):366–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/15140326.2021.1949256 1992.

Elliott TR. Social problem-solving abilities and adjustment to recent-onset spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol. 1999;44(4):315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0090-5550.44.4.315 .

Woods DR, Hrymak AN, Marshall RR, Wood PE, Crowe CM, Hoffman TW, Wright JD, Taylor PA, Woodhouse KA, Bouchard CK. Developing problem solving skills: the McMaster problem solving program. J Eng Educ. 1997;86(2):75–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2168-9830.1997.tb00270.x .

Lohman MC, Finkelstein M. Designing groups in problem-based learning to promote problem-solving skill and self-directedness. Instr Sci. 2000;28(4):291–307. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1003927228005 .

Puplampu V. Nursing students’ and faculty members’ experiences of comfort during transition to context-based learning. Int J Nurs Educ Scholarsh. 2017;14(1). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.teln.2021.02.008 .

Chen SL, Huang TW, Liao IC, Liu C. Development and validation of the simulation learning effectiveness inventory. J Adv Nurs. 2015;71(10):2444–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/jan.12707 .

Miller GP, Molina-Ray C. Beyond technology dependence: critically reflexive thinking in higher education. J Leadersh Stud. 2010;4(1):74–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/jls.20156 .

Gadbury-Amyot CC, Godley LW, Nelson JW Jr. Measuring the level of reflective ability of predoctoral dental students: early outcomes in an e‐portfolio reflection. J Dent Educ. 2019;83(3):275–80. https://doi.org/10.21815/JDE.019.025 .

Bernack C, Holzäpfel L, Leuders T, Renkl A. INITIATING CHANGE ON PRE-SERVICE TECHERS’BELIEFS IN A REFLEXIVE PROBLEM SOLVING COURSEi. CURRENT STATE OF RESEARCH ON MATHEMATICAL BELIEFS XVI 2011:27.

Kellogg RT. The psychology of writing. Oxford University Press; 1999.

Atkins S, Murphy K. Reflection: a review of the literature. J Adv Nurs. 1993;18(8):1188–92. https://doi.org/10.1046/j1365-2648199318081188x .

Lau AKL, Chuk KC, Wei So WK. Reflective practise in clinical teaching. Nurs Health Sci. 2002;4(4):201–8. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1442-2018.2002.00121.x .

Illeris K. Læring (Nordgård, Y.). In.: Oslo: Gyldendal akademisk; 2012.

Barton G, Ryan M. Multimodal approaches to reflective teaching and assessment in higher education. High Educ Res Dev. 2014;33(3):409–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2013.841650 .

Ellis S, Carette B, Anseel F, Lievens F. Systematic reflection: implications for learning from failures and successes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(1):67–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413504106 .

Bulman C, Schutz S. Reflective practice in nursing. John Wiley & Sons; 2013.

Whittock T. Reflexive teaching, reflexive learning. Teach High Educ. 1997;2(2):93–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/1356251970020201 .

Nuninger W, Picardi C, Goy A, Petrone G. Multi-perspective concept mapping in a digital integrated learning environment: Promote active learning through shared perspectives. Educational Technology and the New World of Persistent Learning.edn.: IGI Global; 2019: 114–44.

Kocak O, Coban M, Aydin A, Cakmak N. The mediating role of critical thinking and cooperativity in the 21st century skills of higher education students. Think Skills Creativity. 2021;42:100967. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100967 .

Newell A, Simon HA. Human problem solving. Volume 104. Prentice-hall Englewood Cliffs, NJ; 1972.

Buchner A. Basic topics and approaches to the study of complex problem solving. Complex problem solving: The European perspective 1995:27–63.

Dörner D, Funke J. Complex problem solving: what it is and what it is not. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1153. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01153 .

Fischer A, Neubert JC. The multiple faces of complex problems: a model of problem solving competency and its implications for training and assessment. J Dynamic Decis Mak. 2015;1:6–6. https://doi.org/10.11588/jddm.2015.1.23945 .

Funke J, Fischer A, Holt DV. Competencies for complexity: problem solving in the twenty-first century. Assessment and teaching of 21st century skills.edn.: Springer; 2018: 41–53.

Akpinar Ö. The Effect of Empathy levels of Female School of Physical Education and Sports Students on Problem solving skill levels. Asian J Educ Train. 2020;6(3):406–11.

Doleck T, Bazelais P, Lemay DJ, Saxena A, Basnet RB. Algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, creativity, critical thinking, and problem solving: exploring the relationship between computational thinking skills and academic performance. J Computers Educ 2017, 4(4):355–69 https://doi.org/10.1007/s40692-017-0090-9 .

Ananiadoui K, Claro M. 21st century skills and competences for new millennium learners in OECD countries. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1787/19939019 .

Daud D, Santoso RH. Device learning development using Cabri 3D with problem-solving method based on oriented critical thinking ability and learning achievements of junior high school students. In: 5th Asia Pasific Education Conference (AECON 2018): 2018: Atlantis Press; 2018: 23–28.

Benham MK. The practitioner-scholars’ view of school change: a case-based approach to teaching and learning. Teach Teacher Educ. 1996;12(2):119–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/0742-051X(95)00026-G .

Argelagos E, Pifarré M. Improving information problem solving skills in secondary education through embedded instruction. Comput Hum Behav. 2012;28(2):515–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.10.024 .

Kim KT. The structural relationship among digital literacy, learning strategies, and core competencies among south korean college students. Educational sciences: theory and practice. 2019;19(2):3–21. https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2019.2.001 .

Wink MN, LaRusso MD, Smith RL. Teacher empathy and students with problem behaviors: examining teachers’ perceptions, responses, relationships, and burnout. Psychol Sch. 2021;58(8):1575–96. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.22516 .

Leppma M. The effect of loving-kindness meditation on empathy, perceived social support, and problem-solving appraisal in counseling students. 2011.

Zheng S, Masuda T, Matsunaga M, Noguchi Y, Ohtsubo Y, Yamasue H, Ishii K. Cultural differences in social support seeking: the mediating role of empathic concern. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(12):e0262001.

Imperato A, Strano-Paul L. Impact of reflection on empathy and emotional intelligence in third-year medical students. Acad Psychiatry. 2021;45(3):350–3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40596-020-01371-1 .

Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. 1980.

Kim H, Han S. Does personal distress enhance empathic interaction or block it? Pers Indiv Differ. 2018;124:77–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.005 .

Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. TheScientificWorldJOURNAL. 2006;6:1146–63. https://doi.org/10.1100/tsw.2006.221 .

Batson CD, O’Quin K, Fultz J, Vanderplas M, Isen AM. Influence of self-reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1983;45(3):706. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.706 .

Nomura K, Akai S. Empathy with fictional stories: reconsideration of the fantasy scale of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychol Rep. 2012;110(1):304–14. https://doi.org/10.2466/02070911PR01101304-314 .

Vaish A. Empathy and prosocial behavior. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21816-6 .

Stocks EL, Lishner DA, Waits BL, Downum EM. I’m embarrassed for you: the effect of valuing and perspective taking on empathic embarrassment and empathic concern. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2011;41(1):1–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00699.x .

Archer RL, Diaz-Loving R, Gollwitzer PM, Davis MH, Foushee HC. The role of dispositional empathy and social evaluation in the empathic mediation of helping. 1981. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.4.786 .

Ampuero D, Miranda CE, Delgado LE, Goyen S, Weaver S. Empathy and critical thinking: primary students solving local environmental problems through outdoor learning. J Adventure Educ Outdoor Learn. 2015;15(1):64–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/14729679.2013.848817 .

Moitra M, Buch R, Damor R. Problem solving skills among adolescents in Surat city: a reality check. Natl J Community Med. 2019;10:571–4.

Google Scholar  

Ringle CM, Wende S, Becker J-M. SmartPLS 3. Boenningstedt: SmartPLS GmbH 2015, 584.

Ragin CC, Strand SI, Rubinson C. User’s guide to fuzzy-set/qualitative comparative analysis. Univ Arizona. 2008;87:1–87.

Carrera P, Oceja L, Caballero A, Muñoz D, López-Pérez B, Ambrona T. I feel so sorry! Tapping the joint influence of empathy and personal distress on helping behavior. Motivation and Emotion. 2013;37(2):335–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9302-9 .

Eisenberg N, Eggum ND. Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. The social neuroscience of empathy 2009, 6(2009):71–830. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262012973.003.0007 .

Rodríguez-Nogueira Ó, Moreno-Poyato AR, Álvarez-Álvarez MJ, Pinto-Carral A. Significant socio-emotional learning and improvement of empathy in physiotherapy students through service learning methodology: a mixed methods research. Nurse Educ Today. 2020;90:104437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2020.104437 .

Reynolds F. Communication and clinical effectiveness in rehabilitation. Elsevier Health Sciences; 2004.

Hoffman ML. Empathy and moral development: implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press; 2001.

Joireman J. Empathy and the self-absorption paradox II: self-rumination and self-reflection as mediators between shame, guilt, and empathy. Self and Identity. 2004;3(3):225–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500444000038 .

Stanovich K. Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford University Press; 2011.

Hashmi S, Vanderwert RE, Paine AL, Gerson SA. Doll play prompts social thinking and social talking: representations of internal state language in the brain. Dev Sci. 2021;e13163. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13163 .

Van Reet J. Pretense, Imagination, and Fantasy. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-818872-9.00060-1 .

Zipes J. Why fantasy matters too much. CLCWeb: Comp Literature Cult. 2008;10(4). https://doi.org/10.1353/jae.0.0039 .

Brown MM, Thibodeau RB, Pierucci JM, Gilpin AT. Supporting the development of empathy: the role of theory of mind and fantasy orientation. Soc Dev. 2017;26(4):951–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12232 .

Weibel D, Martarelli CS, Häberli D, Mast FW. The fantasy questionnaire: a measure to assess creative and imaginative fantasy. J Pers Assess. 2018;100(4):431–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1331913 .

Bauer RH, Gilpin AT, Thibodeau-Nielsen RB. Executive functions and imaginative play: Exploring relations with prosocial behaviors using structural equation modeling. Trends in Neuroscience and Education 2021, 25:100165 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2021.100165 .

Becker R. Empathie. Potenziale entdecken.edn.: Springer; 2016: 65–84.

Falk DR, Johnson DW. The effects of perspective-taking and egocentrism on problem solving in heterogeneous and homogeneous groups. J Soc Psychol. 1977;102(1):63–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1977.9713241 .

Johnson DW. Role reversal: A summary and review of the research. Int J Group Tensions 1971.

Davenport SW, Rentsch JR. Managing conflict through team member schema accuracy: a fresh perspective on perspective taking. J Theoretical Social Psychol. 2021;5(4):449–63. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.110 .

Bethune C, Brown JB. Residents’ use of case-based reflection exercises. Can Fam Physician. 2007;53(3):470–6.

Hsu C-C, Wang T-I. Applying game mechanics and student-generated questions to an online puzzle-based game learning system to promote algorithmic thinking skills. Comput Educ. 2018;121:73–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2018.02.002 .

Gürbüz H, Evlioğlu B, Erol ÇS, Gülseçen H, Gülseçen S. What’s the weather like today?”: a computer game to develop algorithmic thinking and problem solving skills of primary school pupils. Educ Inform Technol. 2017;22(3):1133–47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-016-9478-9 .

Geisinger KF. 21st century skills: what are they and how do we assess them? Appl Measur Educ. 2016;29(4):245–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/08957347.2016.1209207 .

Futschek G, Moschitz J. Developing algorithmic thinking by inventing and playing algorithms. Proceedings of the 2010 constructionist approaches to creative learning, thinking and education: Lessons for the 21st century (constructionism 2010) 2010:1–10.

Runco MA, Pritzker SR. Encyclopedia of creativity. Academic press; 2020.

Sternberg RJ, Lubart TI. The concept of creativity: prospects and paradigms. Handb creativity. 1999;1:3–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807916.003 .

Korkmaz Ö, Çakir R, Özden MY. A validity and reliability study of the computational thinking scales (CTS). Comput Hum Behav. 2017;72:558–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.005 .

Garrett RM. Issues in science education: problem-solving, creativity and originality. Int J Sci Educ. 1987;9(2):125–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069870090201 .

Chen C, Kasof J, Himsel AJ, Greenberger E, Dong Q, Xue G. Creativity in drawings of geometric shapes: a cross-cultural examination with the consensual assessment technique. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2002;33(2):171–87. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033002004 .

Chen C-C, Swan K. Using innovative and scientifically-based debate to Build e-Learning Community. Online Learn. 2020;24(3):67–80.

Claes E, Schrooten M, McLaughlin H, Csoba J. Community service learning in complex urban settings: challenges and opportunities for social work education. Social Work Education 2021:1–19 https://doi.org/10.1080/02615479.2021.1948003 .

Nazirova G, Yulchiboeva D, Khaydarov O. The main factors of formation and development of Professional competence of Teachers of Preschool Educational Organizations. REVISTA GEINTEC-GESTAO INOVACAO E TECNOLOGIAS. 2021;11(3):1698–708.

Gibbs G. Learning by doing: a guide to teaching and learning methods. Further Education Unit; 1988.

Kelly M. Enhancing Cognitive Empathy. North Carolina State University; 2021.

Pang Y, Song C, Ma C. Effect of different types of empathy on prosocial behavior: Gratitude as mediator. Front Psychol 2022, 13.

Siem B. The relationship between empathic concern and perceived personal costs for helping and how it is affected by similarity perceptions. J Soc Psychol 2021:1–20 https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2021.1996321 .

Underwood B, Moore B. Perspective-taking and altruism. Psychol Bull. 1982;91(1):143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.143 .

Fatma A, Polat S, Kashimi T. Relationship between the problem-solving skills and empathy skills of operating room nurses. J Nurs Res. 2020;28(2):e75. https://doi.org/10.1097/jnr.0000000000000357 .

Contreras JA, Edwards-Maddox S, Hall A, Lee MA. Effects of reflective practice on baccalaureate nursing students’ stress, anxiety and competency: an integrative review. Worldviews on Evidence‐Based Nursing. 2020;17(3):239–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/wvn.12438 .

Coyne SM, Padilla-Walker LM, Holmgren HG, Davis EJ, Collier KM, Memmott-Elison MK, Hawkins AJ. A meta-analysis of prosocial media on prosocial behavior, aggression, and empathic concern: a multidimensional approach. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(2):331.

O’Brien E, Konrath SH, Grühn D, Hagen AL. Empathic concern and perspective taking: Linear and Quadratic Effects of Age across the Adult Life Span. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2013;68(2):168–75. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs055 .

Gülseven Z, Kumru A, Carlo G, De Guzman MR. The roles of perspective taking, empathic concern, and Prosocial Moral reasoning in the self-reported prosocial behaviors of Filipino and turkish young adults. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022120968265 .

Wrigglesworth S. Understanding reflective practice. Nurs Standard (2014+). 2016;31(8):72. https://doi.org/10.7748/ns.31.8.72.s48 .

Amerson R, Livingston WG. Reflexive photography: an alternative method for documenting the learning process of cultural competence. J Transcult Nurs. 2014;25(2):202–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/1043659613515719 .

Davies SM, Reitmaier AB, Smith LR, Mangan-Danckwart D. Capturing intergenerativity: the use of student reflective journals to identify learning within an undergraduate course in gerontological nursing. J Nurs Educ. 2013;52(3):139–49. https://doi.org/10.3928/01484834-20120213-01 .

Stocker M, Burmester M, Allen M. Optimisation of simulated team training through the application of learning theories: a debate for a conceptual framework. BMC Med Educ. 2014;14(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-14-69 .

Parrish DR, Crookes K. Designing and implementing reflective practice programs–key principles and considerations. Nurse Educ Pract. 2014;14(3):265–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nepr.2013.08.002 .

Tee KN, Leong KE, Abdul Rahim SS. The mediating effects of critical thinking skills on motivation factors for mathematical reasoning ability. Asia-Pacific Educ Researcher. 2018;27:373–82.

Pellas N, Vosinakis S. The effect of simulation games on learning computer programming: a comparative study on high school students’ learning performance by assessing computational problem-solving strategies. Educ Inform Technol. 2018;23(6):2423–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-018-9724-4 .

Sırakaya M, Alsancak Sırakaya D, Korkmaz Ö. The impact of STEM attitude and thinking style on computational thinking determined via Structural equation modeling. J Sci Edu Technol. 2020;29(4):561–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-020-09836-6 .

Hair JF Jr, Hult GTM, Ringle CM, Sarstedt M. A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (. PLS-SEM): Sage publications;; 2021.

Kallmuenzer A, Kraus S, Peters M, Steiner J, Cheng C-F. Entrepreneurship in tourism firms: a mixed-methods analysis of performance driver configurations. Tour Manag. 2019;74:319–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2019.04.002 .

Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (revised ed.). In.: New York: Academic Press; 1977.

Fornell C, Larcker DF. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J Mark Res. 1981;18(1):39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800104 .

Henseler J, Dijkstra TK, Sarstedt M, Ringle CM, Diamantopoulos A, Straub DW, Ketchen DJ Jr, Hair JF, Hult GTM, Calantone RJ. Common beliefs and reality about PLS: Comments on Rönkkö and Evermann (2013). Organizational research methods 2014, 17(2):182–209.

Chin WW. The partial least squares approach to structural equation modeling. Mod methods Bus Res. 1998;295(2):295–336.

Woodside AG. Moving beyond multiple regression analysis to algorithms: calling for adoption of a paradigm shift from symmetric to asymmetric thinking in data analysis and crafting theory. Volume 66. Elsevier; 2013. pp. 463–72.

Hwang G-J, Lai C-L, Liang J-C, Chu H-C, Tsai C-C. A long-term experiment to investigate the relationships between high school students’ perceptions of mobile learning and peer interaction and higher-order thinking tendencies. Education Tech Research Dev. 2018;66:75–93.

Jeong H. Critical thinking disposition, problem solving process, and empathy among nursing students. Adv Sci Technol Lett. 2015;103:44–8.

Batson CD. Prosocial motivation: Is it ever truly altruistic? Advances in experimental social psychology. Volume 20,edn.: Elsevier; 1987: 65–122.

Isen AM. Some ways in which positive affect influences decision making and problem solving. Handb emotions. 2008;3:548–73.

Sze JA, Gyurak A, Goodkind MS, Levenson RW. Greater emotional empathy and prosocial behavior in late life. Emotion. 2012;12(5):1129.

Boyaci S, Atalay N. A Scale Development for 21st Century Skills of Primary School students: a validity and reliability study. Int J Instruction. 2016;9(1):133–48. https://doi.org/10.12973/iji.2016.9111a .

Herout L. Reflexion of the Methods and Forms of University Education in Economy Oriented Study Programmes in the Czech Republic. In: ICERI2015 Proceedings: 8th annual International Conference of Education, Research and Innovation : 2015; 2015.

Aein F, Hosseini R, Naseh L, Safdari F, Banaian S. The effect of problem-solving-based interprofessional learning on critical thinking and satisfaction with learning of nursing and midwifery students. J Educ Health Promotion. 2020;9. https://doi.org/10.4103/jehp.jehp_640_19 .

Giannakopoulos P, Buckley S. Do problem solving, critical thinking and creativity play a role in knowledge management? A theoretical mathematics perspective. In: Proceedings of the 10 th European Conference on Knowledge Management : 2009; 2009: 327–337.

GOGUS A, Göğüş NG, BAHADIR E. Intersections between critical thinking skills and reflective thinking skills toward problem solving. Pamukkale Üniversitesi Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi. 2019;49:1–19. https://doi.org/10.9779/pauefd.526407 .

Özgenel M. Modeling the relationships between school administrators’ creative and critical thinking dispositions with decision making styles and problem solving skills. Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice. 2018;18(3). https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2018.3.0068 .

Aquino HI, Ching DA. Effects of Reflective Learning Resource Material on Achievement of Mathematics Learning Outcome. Int J Educational Manage Dev Stud, 3(1):132–48 https://doi.org/10.53378/352866 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the teachers and students who participated (including the teachers and students of Shulan International Medical College, Zhejiang Shuren University, China), and SmartPLS 3.0 and fsQCA 3.1 for their assistance in analysis of data of this process.

This study was funded by the Provincial Industry-University Cooperation Collaborative Education Project (NO.318 [2022] of the Zhejiang Development Reform Society), the Scientific and technological Innovation activity Plan and New Seedling Talent Plan for College students in Zhejiang Province in 2023, the First-class curriculum project of Zhejiang Province of China (NO.195 [2022] of the Zhejiang Education Office Letter, NO.352 [2022] of the Zhejiang Education Office Letter), the First batch of ideological and political demonstration courses of Zhejiang Province of China (NO.47 [2021] of the Zhejiang Education Letter), and the High-level pre-level program of Zhejiang Shuren University in 2019.

Author information

Ying Wang and Ze-Ling Xu contributed equally to this work.

Authors and Affiliations

Shulan International Medical College, Zhejiang Shuren University, Hangzhou, 310015, China

Ying Wang, Ze-Ling Xu, Jia-Yao Lou & Ke-Da Chen

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

ZLX and JYL conceived the project with the input of YW and KDC. YW, ZLX, and JYL collected and analyzed the relevant data for this study. YW and ZLX are the main authors of this study. All the authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ke-Da Chen .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

Ethical approval was obtained and document informed consent was waived by the Ethics Committee of Zhejiang Shuren University (202201016). All procedures in this study adhered to the World Medical Association (WMA) Declaration of Helsinki (2013) ethical guidelines.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Wang, Y., Xu, ZL., Lou, JY. et al. Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective learning: results from partial least square structural equation modeling and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis. BMC Med Educ 23 , 382 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04326-w

Download citation

Received : 26 April 2022

Accepted : 05 May 2023

Published : 25 May 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04326-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Reflective learning
  • Complex problem-solving skills
  • Partial least squares structural equation modeling

BMC Medical Education

ISSN: 1472-6920

explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

IMAGES

  1. Psychology, topic factors affecting problem solving

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  2. SOLUTION: FACTORS AFFECTING PROBLEM SOLVING , IMPROVING PROBLEM SOLVING

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  3. SOLUTION: FACTORS AFFECTING PROBLEM SOLVING , IMPROVING PROBLEM SOLVING

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  4. Psychological Steps Involved in Problem Solving

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  5. How psychology does define problem solving?

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

  6. Factors affecting Problem Solving #psychology #ignou #psychologist #mapc #mapsychology

    explain factors affecting problem solving in psychology

VIDEO

  1. Factors affecting Problem Solving #psychology #ignou #psychologist #mapc #mapsychology

  2. Psychology: Thinking and Problem Solving

  3. Problem Solving With Common Factors

  4. Boost Your Brain: Try the Five Whys Technique! #motivational #shorts

  5. Are You a Genius🤔? Find Out with This Quick IQ Test!🧠

  6. Problem Solving-Factors Affecting

COMMENTS

  1. Problem-Solving Strategies and Obstacles

    Several mental processes are at work during problem-solving. Among them are: Perceptually recognizing the problem. Representing the problem in memory. Considering relevant information that applies to the problem. Identifying different aspects of the problem. Labeling and describing the problem.

  2. PDF The Psychology of Problem Solving

    The Psychology of Problem Solving Problems are a central part of human life. The Psychology of Problem Solving organizes in one volume much of what psychologists know about problem solving and the factors that contribute to its success or failure. There are chapters by leading experts in this field, includ-

  3. The Problem-Solving Process

    Problem-solving is a mental process that involves discovering, analyzing, and solving problems. The ultimate goal of problem-solving is to overcome obstacles and find a solution that best resolves the issue. The best strategy for solving a problem depends largely on the unique situation. In some cases, people are better off learning everything ...

  4. 7.3 Problem-Solving

    Additional Problem Solving Strategies:. Abstraction - refers to solving the problem within a model of the situation before applying it to reality.; Analogy - is using a solution that solves a similar problem.; Brainstorming - refers to collecting an analyzing a large amount of solutions, especially within a group of people, to combine the solutions and developing them until an optimal ...

  5. Problem Solving

    Cognitive—Problem solving occurs within the problem solver's cognitive system and can only be inferred indirectly from the problem solver's behavior (including biological changes, introspections, and actions during problem solving).. Process—Problem solving involves mental computations in which some operation is applied to a mental representation, sometimes resulting in the creation of ...

  6. Problem Solving and Decision Making

    Problem solving and decision making are both examples of complex, higher-order thinking. Both involve the assessment of the environment, the involvement of working memory or short-term memory, reliance on long term memory, effects of knowledge, and the application of heuristics to complete a behavior. A problem can be defined as an impasse or ...

  7. Cognitive Psychology: The Science of How We Think

    Cognitive psychology is the study of internal mental processes—all of the workings inside your brain, including perception, thinking, memory, attention, language, problem-solving, and learning. Learning about how people think and process information helps researchers and psychologists understand the human brain and assist people with ...

  8. 7.3 Problem Solving

    Solving Puzzles. Problem-solving abilities can improve with practice. Many people challenge themselves every day with puzzles and other mental exercises to sharpen their problem-solving skills. Sudoku puzzles appear daily in most newspapers. Typically, a sudoku puzzle is a 9×9 grid. The simple sudoku below is a 4×4 grid. To solve the puzzle ...

  9. Problem-Solving

    Problem solving involves a set of complex cognitive processes that require thinking and reasoning. A problem occurs when there is a goal that needs to be reached and there is not a clear path to achieving the goal (Mayer 2013).Problems can range in terms of type, complexity, strategy use, domain, and other factors that affect the content and the context of the problem or its solution.

  10. Complex Problem Solving: What It Is and What It Is Not

    1 Department of Psychology, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany; 2 Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University, Heidelberg, Germany; Computer-simulated scenarios have been part of psychological research on problem solving for more than 40 years. The shift in emphasis from simple toy problems to complex, more real-life oriented problems has been accompanied by discussions about the best ...

  11. Problem-Solving Skill

    Problem solving. Problem solving involves identifying a goal, carrying out actions to reach the goal, and overcoming obstacles that interfere with reaching it (Munakata, 2006). It is a complex cognitive skill, relying on different capabilities including attention, perception, memory, concepts, and symbolic processes such as language.

  12. Problem solving through values: A challenge for thinking and capability

    Abstract. The paper aims to introduce the conceptual framework of problem solving through values. The framework consists of problem analysis, selection of value (s) as a background for the solution, the search for alternative ways of the solution, and the rationale for the solution. This framework reveals when, how, and why is important to ...

  13. On the cognitive process of human problem solving

    In cognitive informatics, problem solving is identified as a cognitive process of the brain at the higher cognitive layer that searches a solution for a given problem or finds a path to reach a given goal (Wang, 2007b).Problem solving is one of the 39 fundamental cognitive processes modeled in the LRMB model (Wang et al., 2006).It is recognized that there is a need to seek an axiomatic and ...

  14. 9

    The Psychology of Problem Solving - June 2003. 22 August 2024: Due to technical disruption, we are experiencing some delays to publication. We are working to restore services and apologise for the inconvenience. ... Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122-1131CrossRef Google ...

  15. 7 Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving

    Module 7: Thinking, Reasoning, and Problem-Solving. This module is about how a solid working knowledge of psychological principles can help you to think more effectively, so you can succeed in school and life. You might be inclined to believe that—because you have been thinking for as long as you can remember, because you are able to figure ...

  16. Problem Solving

    Solving Puzzles. Problem-solving abilities can improve with practice. Many people challenge themselves every day with puzzles and other mental exercises to sharpen their problem-solving skills. Sudoku puzzles appear daily in most newspapers. Typically, a sudoku puzzle is a 9×9 grid. The simple sudoku below ([link]) is a 4×4 grid.

  17. The Role of Motivation in Complex Problem Solving

    Under a narrow perspective, CPS can be defined as thinking that aims to overcome barriers and to reach goals in situations that are complex, dynamic, and non-transparent (Frensch and Funke, 1995). Indeed, past research has shown the influential role of task properties (Berry and Broadbent, 1984; Funke, 1985) and of cognitive factors on CPS ...

  18. PDF UNIT 4 IMPEDIMENTS TO PROBLEM Problem Solving Theoretical ...

    It is always ideal to brainstorm the problem and to do this one should follow the rules given below: 1) Express all ideas. 2) Deem no idea too wild to be considered. 3) Quantity is important; every idea that comes to mind should be included. 4) Getting together with others to brainstorm is desirable.

  19. What are the Factors that Affect Problem-Solving Activities? Part 1

    Two major factors that affect problem-solving activities include personality type and temperament. Sensing and Intuitive people approach problems through perception and prefer flexibility and adaptability. Thinking- and Feeling-oriented people usually make judgments. The foundation of problem solving, however, lies in the correct understanding of the problem and its underlying issues. Read the ...

  20. What Affects Problem Solving

    Acknowledging the factors that affect the process and taking them into account when forming a solution gives teams the best chance of solving the problem effectively. Below we have outlined the key factors affecting the problem solving process. Understanding the problem The most important factor in solving a problem is to first fully understand it.

  21. PDF UNIT 4 CREATIVITY AND PROBLEM Intelligence SOLVING

    of problem solving. Finally, factors affecting problem solving will be discussed. 4.1 OBJECTIVES After reading this unit, you will be able to: • Explain meaning, aspects and stages of creativity; • Describe the tests to measure creativity and relate it with intelligence; • Explain meaning, stages and strategies of problem solving; and

  22. Factors influencing the complex problem-solving skills in reflective

    Background The Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development emphasizes the importance of complex problem-solving (CPS) skills in the 21st century. CPS skills have been linked to academic performance, career development, and job competency training. Reflective learning, which includes journal writing, peer reflection, selfreflection, and group discussion, has been explored to improve ...