•   Hjem
  • Politihøgskolen
  • Publikasjoner fra Cristin - PHS
  • Vis innførsel

Vis enkel innførsel

Structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation: A method aimed at providing a broad and objective starting point for a criminal investigation

dc.contributor.authorSunde, Nina
dc.date.accessioned2021-02-02T09:21:18Z
dc.date.available2021-02-02T09:21:18Z
dc.date.created2021-01-25T12:18:43Z
dc.date.issued2020
dc.identifier.citationThe Police Journal. 2020, 1-17.en_US
dc.identifier.issn0032-258X
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/11250/2725725
dc.descriptionMetoden ble første beskrevet i antologien "Etterforskning under lupen" som ble publisert i serien PHS forskning i 2019 (http://hdl.handle.net/11250/2634563)
dc.description.abstractThe Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and objective investigation. The method aims to protect the innocent, while also enabling the detectives to discover the full scope of the incident under investigation. SHDCI builds on theory and principles from cognitive psychology, scientific methodology, logical reasoning, law and criminal investigation best practice. The method is developed in a Norwegian context, but builds on universally accepted legal principles, and SHDCI may therefore be relevant for implementation in other jurisdictions.en_US
dc.language.isoengen_US
dc.publisherSageen_US
dc.subjectetterforskningen_US
dc.subjectpolice investigationen_US
dc.subjecthypothesis developmenten_US
dc.subjecthypoteseutviklingen_US
dc.subjectobjectivityen_US
dc.subjectobjektiviteten_US
dc.subjectbias mitigationen_US
dc.subjectbiasreduksjonen_US
dc.subjectpresumption of innocenceen_US
dc.subjectuskyldspresumsjonen_US
dc.titleStructured hypothesis development in criminal investigation: A method aimed at providing a broad and objective starting point for a criminal investigationen_US
dc.typePeer revieweden_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.description.versionacceptedVersionen_US
dc.source.pagenumber1-17en_US
dc.source.journalThe Police Journalen_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/0032258X20982328
dc.identifier.cristin1878334
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextpostprint
cristin.fulltext
cristin.fulltext
cristin.fulltext
cristin.qualitycode1

Tilhørende fil(er)

Thumbnail

Denne innførselen finnes i følgende samling(er)

  • Artikler - Vitenskapelige [198]
  • Publikasjoner fra Cristin - PHS [26]

Improving Criminal Investigations with Structured Analytic Techniques

  • First Online: 18 June 2020

Cite this chapter

structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  • Max M. Houck 13  

Part of the book series: Advanced Sciences and Technologies for Security Applications ((ASTSA))

599 Accesses

3 Altmetric

The intelligence community has used structured analytic techniques, methods designed to reduce bias and increase transparency of process, for years. The techniques force analysts out of routine thinking and away from heuristic habits in order to increase creativity, more comprehensively evaluate the questions, and create a document trail that reveals the thinking process that led to the intelligence product. These methods can be adapted for use in criminal investigations to help reduce bias, improve accuracy, and avoid both wrongful convictions (over 2200 to date) and reparations (more than $2.2 billion) while optimizing resources. The methods shift the investigator from intuitive, daily thinking (System 1, in Kahneman’s terminology) to a more analytical approach (System 2) that creates a transparent process, regardless of the outcome. Structured analytical techniques are simple to use, inexpensive, and largely visual; they promote transparency, creativity, and group discussion, leading to better-supported results.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

Supporting Variability in Criminal Intelligence Analysis: From Expert Intuition to Critical and Rigorous Analysis

structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

The Evidence Base

structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

CrimeFighter Investigator: Criminal Network Sense-Making

http://www.law.umich.edu/special/exoneration/Pages/about.aspx

The tendency to mistakenly perceive connections and meaning between unrelated things. A classic example of apophany is Skinner’s Box, where a hungry pigeon was put in a box and randomly given food pellets. Because the pigeon got a food pellet while performing some action, it correlated the food and the action. As the pigeon increased the number of times it performed that action, it strengthened the false correlation between the non-correlated events (the feeding was random) (Skinner 1948 ). In humans, one example is the Gambler’s Fallacy, where a perceived “hot streak” is in fact related to chance (Burns and Corpus 2004 ).

If your answer was $0.10, that’s System 1 talking. If the bat costs $1 more than the ball, the ball has to be $0.05 ($1.05 + $0.05 = $1.10). If the ball cost $0.10, the bat and ball would cost $1.20.

Carbon-based compounds in the juice are absorbed into the paper’s fibers. Because lemon juice ink is a weak acid, it softens the fibers in the paper. When the paper is warmed, the added heat causes some of the chemical bonds in the dried juice to break down and carbon is released. When the carbon comes into contact with air, it oxidizes, turning the juice pattern brown and, thus, visible.

Named for the English Franciscan friar, William of Ockham (c. 1287–1347), a scholastic philosopher and theologian.

www.dictionary.com

Brooks, L. n.d. “Brainstorm Questions, Not Solutions”, at: http://99u.com/articles/52335/brainstorm-questions-not-solutions

The model was developed at Gordon Training International in the 1970s with different terminology than is commonly used now ( gordontraining.com ). The model is frequently but incorrectly attributed to Abraham Maslow . It is conceptually similar to the Johari Window (Luft and Ingham 1961 ) which lists knowns and unknowns for an individual in relation to others in a heuristic exercise.

A slang military phrase for Scientific Wild-Ass Guess (Safire 2004 ).

Both women are pregnant.

Aitken, C., Roberts, P., & Jackson, G. (2010). Fundamentals of probability and statistical evidence in criminal proceedings: Guidance for judges, lawyers, forensic scientists and expert witnesses . London: Royal Statistical Society.

Google Scholar  

Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2003). “Coherent arbitrariness”: Stable demand curves without stable preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118 (1), 73–106.

Artner, S. J., Girven, R., & Bruce, J. B. (2016). Assessing the value of structured analytic techniques in the US intelligence community . Santa Monica: Rand Corporation.

Bennett, G. E. (1933). Assumptions. The Accounting Review, 8 (2), 157–159.

Burns, B., & Corpus, B. (2004). Randomness and inductions from streaks: “Gambler’s fallacy” versus “hot hand”. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11 (1), 179.

Central Intelligence Agency. (2009). A tradecraft primer: Structured analytic techniques for improving intelligence analysis . Central Intelligence Agency: Langley.

Dewey, J. (1933). How we think . Buffalo: Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1910).

Dror, I. E., & Hampikian, G. (2011). Subjectivity and bias in forensic DNA mixture interpretation. Science & Justice, 51 (4), 204–208.

Dror, I. E., Charlton, D., & Péron, A. E. (2006). Contextual information renders experts vulnerable to making erroneous identifications. Forensic Science International, 156 (1), 74–78.

Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1978). Confidence in judgment: Persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 85 (5), 395.

Ericsson, K. A., Hoffman, R. R., Kozbelt, A., & Williams, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gawande, A. (2010). The checklist manifesto . Profile Books.

George, R. Z., & Bruce, J. B. (Eds.). (2014). Analyzing intelligence: National security practitioners’ perspectives (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109 (1), 75.

Gross, S., O’Brien, B., Hu, C., & Kennedy, E. (2014). Rate of false conviction in capital cases. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111 (20), 7230–7235.

Hall, C. C., Ariss, L., & Todorov, A. (2007). The illusion of knowledge: When more information reduces accuracy and increases confidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103 (2), 277–290.

Herzog, M., & Fahle, M. (1997). The role of feedback in learning a Vernier discrimination task. Vision Research, 37 (15), 2133–2141.

Heuer, R. J. (1999). Psychology of intelligence analysis (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Center for the Study of Intelligence, Central Intelligence Agency. Available at: https://www.cia.gov/library/center-for-the-study-of-intelligence/csi-publications/books-and-monographs/psychology-of-intelligence-analysis/

Heuer, R. J. (2015). Psychology of intelligence analysis . Langley: CIA Center for the Study of Intelligence. https://www.cia.gov/library/center-for-the-study-of-intelligence/csi-publications/books-and-monographs/psychology-of-intelligenceanalysis/index.html .

Heuer, R. J., & Pherson, R. H. (2015). Structured analytic techniques for intelligence analysis (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: CQ Press.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow . New York: Macmillan.

Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2006). Would you be happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion. Science, 312 (5782), 1908–1910.

Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (6), 1121.

Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13 (5), 585–589.

Loftus, E. F., & Zanni, G. (1975). Eyewitness testimony: The influence of the wording of a question. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 5 (1), 86–88.

Luft, J., & Ingham, H. (1961). The johari window. Human Relations Training News, 5 (1), 6–7.

Major, J. (2008). Communicating with intelligence . Lanham: Scarecrow Press.

Mercier, H. (2017). Confirmation bias—myside bias. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions (2nd ed.). Abingdon: Routledge.

Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review, 63 , 81–97.

Nisbett, R. E., Zukier, H., & Lemley, R. E. (1981). The dilution effect: Nondiagnostic information weakens the implications of diagnostic information. Cognitive Psychology, 13 (2), 248–277.

Nkwake, A. M. (2013). Why are assumptions important? In Working with assumptions in international development program evaluation (pp. 93–111). New York: Springer.

Pugh, S. (1991). Total design: Integrated models for successful product engineering . New Jersey: Addison-Wesley.

Ross, L., Lepper, M. R., & Hubbard, M. (1975). Perseverance in self-perception and social perception: Biased attributional processes in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (5), 880–892.

Rossmo, D. K. (2008). Criminal investigative failures . London: CRC press.

Safire, W. (2004). No uncertain terms . New York: Simon and Schuster.

Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review, 63 (2), 129–138.

Skinner, B. F. (1948). ‘Superstition’ in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38 (2), 168.

Slovic, P. (2018). Behavioral problems of adhering to a decision policy . Unpublished manuscript, available at: https://scholarsbank.uoregon.edu/xmlui/handle/1794/23607 .

Smith, M. R., & Alpert, G. P. (2007). Explaining police bias: A theory of social conditioning and illusory correlation. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34 (10), 1262–1283.

Spencer, K. B., Charbonneau, A. K., & Glaser, J. (2016). Implicit bias and policing. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10 (1), 50–63.

United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. (2011). Criminal intelligence manual for analysts . New York: United Nations. Available at: https://www.unodc.org/documents/organized-crime/Law-Enforcement/Criminal_Intelligence_for_Analysts.pdf .

United States Department of Defense. (2017). FM34–52 Human intelligence collector operations: FM 2-22.3 . Washington, DC.

White, D., Kemp, R. I., Jenkins, R., Matheson, M., & Burton, A. M. (2014). Passport officers’ errors in face matching. PLoS One, 9 (8), e103510.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Forensic Studies & Justice Program, University of South Florida, St. Petersburg, FL, USA

Max M. Houck

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Max M. Houck .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Department of Criminology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA

Bryanna Fox

Department of Criminology, University of South Florida-St. Petersburg, St. Petersburg, FL, USA

Joan A. Reid

College of Public Health, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA

Anthony J. Masys

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Houck, M.M. (2020). Improving Criminal Investigations with Structured Analytic Techniques. In: Fox, B., Reid, J., Masys, A. (eds) Science Informed Policing. Advanced Sciences and Technologies for Security Applications. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-41287-6_7

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-41287-6_7

Published : 18 June 2020

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-41286-9

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-41287-6

eBook Packages : Law and Criminology Law and Criminology (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research
  • DOI: 10.4324/9781315637211-4
  • Corpus ID: 216787963

Investigative decision making

  • K. Ask , I. Fahsing
  • Published in The Routledge International… 20 May 2018
  • Law, Psychology
  • The Routledge International Handbook of Legal and Investigative Psychology

6 Citations

Investigative fatigue: how sleep-circadian factors shape criminal investigations, fooling ourselves and others: confirmation bias and the trustworthiness of qualitative research – part 1 (the threats), structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation: a method aimed at providing a broad and objective starting point for a criminal investigation, the impact of evidence type on police investigators’ perceptions of suspect culpability and evidence reliability, ‘the making of an expert detective’—a european perspective: comparing decision-making in norway and uk, the wrongful conviction law review wrongful convictions with prison sentences in spain exoneree characteristics, crime types, and contributing factors, related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

IMAGES

  1. Visualisation of the Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  2. Submitted By-: Asweta Mali 1 Semester, LLM Criminal Law

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  3. How to Write a Hypothesis

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  4. (PDF) Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  5. Hypothesis testing crime analysis: SUMMARY (1 of 5)

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

  6. Hypothesis Testing

    structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation

VIDEO

  1. "THE STRUCTURED WORLD HYPOTHESIS"

  2. How DNA testing is used in investigations

  3. Nonlinear Transformation :: Structured Hypothesis Sets @ Machine Learning Foundations (機器學習基石)

  4. Testing normality is pointless. Do this instead

  5. Criminal Profiling Building a Profile of Serial Offenders

  6. Day-6 Hypothesis Development and Testing

COMMENTS

  1. Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation ...

    The Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and objective investigation.

  2. Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation

    By Nina Sunde . Keywords: Hypothesis development, criminal investigation, objectivity, bias mitigation, presumption of innocence. Abstract. (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and objective .

  3. PIA: Structured hypothesis development in criminal ...

    The Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and objective investigation.

  4. ‪Nina Sunde‬ - ‪Google Scholar‬

    Structured hypothesis development in criminal investigation: a method aimed at providing a broad and objective starting point for a criminal investigation N Sunde The Police Journal 95 (2), 276-295 , 2022

  5. Criminal Investigation: An Introduction to Principles and ...

    A criminal investigation is one lawfully conducted by a criminal investigation team that, based on reasonable suspicion, collects evidence to determine whether a crime has been committed and,...

  6. Improving Criminal Investigations with Structured Analytic ...

    These methods can be adapted for use in criminal investigations to help reduce bias, improve accuracy, and avoid both wrongful convictions (over 2200 to date) and reparations (more than $2.2 billion) while optimizing resources.

  7. Visualisation of the Structured Hypothesis Development in ...

    The Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and ...

  8. Investigative decision making | Semantic Scholar

    The Structured Hypothesis Development in Criminal Investigation (SHDCI) method aims to assist detectives in developing an adequate set of hypotheses, which prepares the ground for a broad and… Expand.

  9. Have you considered the opposite? A debiasing strategy for ...

    Abstract. Fundamental challenges in human decision-making pose a serious threat to fair evidence evaluation, verdicts in court proceedings, and the administration of justice.

  10. Art, Craft and Science of Detective Work - Stephen Tong, Ben ...

    Abstract. The failure of high-profile criminal investigations and falling detection rates have led to public criticisms of the effectiveness of detective practice.