This site uses cookies. We do this to provide you with a better experience. By clicking “Okay,” you accept. View the Privacy Policy  for details.

The science of reading: research and resources

Explore the latest evidence, insights from thought leaders, and our new guide to real-world implementation.

What is the science of reading?

The science of reading is a dynamic body of convergent evidence that informs effective instruction in all areas of literacy development.

Unlock the power of literacy with evidence-aligned instruction

Contrary to popular belief, learning to read isn’t a natural process. The science of reading builds on thousands of studies which confirm an explicit, systematic approach to literacy instruction is critical for student success. Today, schools from New York to Texas are putting this science into practice.

Cover of 'Science of Reading 2.0', easy implementation guide

Introducing the Science of Reading 2.0

The next generation of the science of reading connects research about reading and learning to help educators implement effective literacy instruction. Unlock the power of literacy for your students with this easy-to-use guide.

Overlapping circles in yellow, blue, and green

The science of reading, explained

Learn about the two proven aspects of reading success—word recognition and language comprehension—and the science of implementation.

Semi-transparent green square on yellow background

Principles of effective literacy instruction

Discover the 10 trusted steps for bringing effective instruction to your students, combining methods from the “I do, we do, you do model” with Rosenshine’s principles of instruction.

Stylized orange and yellow butterfly wings on a white background

Stories from the field

Get inspiration from other educators, with real-world case studies about the impact of the science of reading in classrooms around the country.

Explore the evidence

Designed with guidance from the Every Student Succeeds Act (ESSA), our efficacy studies meet the highest standards.

Two elementary school girls work together to sound out words in their book at school.

A quick view of our evidence

Learn more about our research validations and third-party, independent research in schools across the United States.

Teacher standing over students while they are engaged in an activity.

A summary of our research

Explore the evidence behind our trusted, proven literacy solutions, all part of the One95 Literacy Ecosystem™.

Laura Stewart

Delivering on the promise of education starts with the mastery of the most fundamental skill—the ability to read. Literacy is the foundation of every student’s learning journey and future success. Of equal importance, reading can bring immense joy to life.

Laura Stewart

young teacher eagerly helps her elementary students with a reading activity

Putting reading research into practice

Today, we know a great deal about what works in reading instruction thanks to the science of reading. But our work as educators doesn’t end there—effective implementation is essential. Implementation science bridges the gap between research and real-world practice, providing insight on how to realize literacy success.

Evidence-aligned resources

Enhance outcomes with the right instructional materials, at the right time, backed by high-quality research.

Instructional practices

Follow best practices for literacy instruction to help students stay engaged, develop new skills, and become independent readers.

Strategic coaching

Provide feedback and support to help educators build knowledge, confidence, and instructional ability.

Student writing in classroom with teacher in background

Frequently asked questions

Here are the two top questions educators ask as they begin their science of reading journey. Explore our FAQ page or get in touch for more information. We are here to help you.

How does the science of reading help students learn to read?

The science of reading is the current body of evidence on how children learn to read. A structured literacy approach, based in the science of reading, is the best approach for nearly all children. The evidence we have is consistent and overwhelming: we know that explicit and systematic phonics instruction typically leads to better outcomes in learning to read.

What does the science of reading replace in education today?

It replaces reading practices and theories—such as three cueing, balanced literacy, and whole-language instruction—that have been proven false. For example, there was a pervasive concept that children should use pictures or context to guess at an unknown word. That is not an effective and efficient route to word recognition; utilizing decoding, however, is an efficient and effective route to word recognition. Another pervasive and persistent idea is that there are hundreds of ways to learn to read, and no set of instructional principles that will be the most effective for most children. These are just not true, and the science of reading has helped to shine a light on that.

Science of reading resources for today

Learn more with real-world success stories, expert insights, practical tips for implementation, and more.

95 Percent Group Experts Corner with Kelly Butler & Kymyona Burk

Experts corner: Literacy rising in Mississippi

Yes, Mississippi’s success CAN be replicated to raise literacy achievement for all students

The legislature must adopt a comprehensive early literacy law that includes support for teachers and administrators, support for students, support for parents and families, and funding for implementation. The state education agency must ensure that there is an office dedicated to overseeing implementation and monitoring progress. None of this will work without buy-in, so it is critical to communicate with all stakeholders.

Dr. Kymyona Burk

Dr. Kymyona Burk

Featured insights

Teacher and student giving each other a high five in a library. Elementary school boy having a private lesson with his teacher. Tutoring and student support in a primary school.

Guide to literacy instruction

why research matters ESSA

Research in literacy education: Why evidence matters

Building a sound wall-Where do I begin

Building a sound wall: Where do I begin?

students in elementary classroom sounding out words together

Phonemic awareness lesson plans

mother and daughter reading in the park together

Why is reading important? The lifelong benefits of reading 

African American teacher helping her elementary school students

Spotlight Tennessee: Explicit phonics instruction is raising the bar

of students ended the school year above the 40% mark in reading fluency on the aimsweb assessment

Get to know the only science of reading course you need

We needed something that was relatively cost-effective, that was asynchronous, that would allow our teachers to do it in bite-sized pieces. We also like the videos and the research coupled with some takeaway tools that you could try out—so it checked all the boxes for us in terms of full scale implementation…I haven’t seen a professional development tool that can change teacher knowledge in such a profound way that’s asynchronous and affordable.

Casey Bertram | Superintendent of Bozeman Public Schools

Casey Bertram Superintendent of Bozeman Public Schools, Montana

Explore more insights

95 Percent Group icon that looks like a multicolored infinity symbol.

One solution for literacy instruction

Based on the science of reading and backed by nearly two decades of research, the One95 Literacy Ecosystem™ is proven to advance reading skills in students from diverse backgrounds, grades, and abilities.

It integrates evidence-aligned products and professional learning resources into one cohesive system for instruction across tiers—so you’re ready and able to help every child realize their reading potential.

One solution for literacy instruction

or more of students can learn to read with high-quality, evidence-based instruction.

View of a small group using colorful manipulatives for reading instruction.

Build your expertise

Improve your instructional practices, get support through ongoing coaching, or attend an upcoming workshop or webinar with experts in the Science of Reading.

Explore professional learning opportunities

of elementary teachers have never been trained in strategies for teaching phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension (EAB Report on Literacy).

Get more content like this

Stay up to date on the latest insights, free resources, and more.

By completing this form, you indicate your consent to receiving marketing communications.

Skip to content

The Science of Reading: The Basics

If you prefer, read the text-version of the infographic.

The Science of Reading: The Basics

There are so many pieces to the Science of Reading that it can be difficult to know where to start. Here are a few highlights about what the Science of Reading IS and what it IS NOT. This knowledge will help you on your journey to teaching all children, including those with diverse needs and cultural backgrounds, to read.

A Collection of Research

Research, over time, from multiple fields of study using methods that confirm and disconfirm theories on how children best learn to read.

Teaching Based on the 5 Big Ideas

Phonemic Awareness - The ability to identify and play with individual sounds in spoken words.

Phonics - Reading instruction on understanding how letters and groups of letters link to sounds to form letter- sound relationships and spelling patterns.

Fluency - The ability to read words, phrases, sentences, and stories correctly, with enough speed, and expression .

Vocabulary - Knowing what words mean and how to say and use them correctly.

Comprehension - The ability to understand what you are reading.

Ever Evolving

There is new research and evidence all the time. As populations, communities, and approaches evolve, so should practice.

What it is NOT

A program, an intervention, or a product that you can buy..

The Science of Reading could be considered an approach to teaching reading that is based on decades of research and evidence. It is NOT a specific program.

Phonics-based programs that drill phonics skills.

Phonics is an integral part of teaching reading based on science, but it is just one of the five big ideas that should be taught so all children can learn to read.

Complete and no more study needs to be done.

As with any science, it is never complete. We can always know more. More study happens all the time and researchers, teachers, and families can work together to bring the best research into classrooms.

Suggested Citation

National Center on Improving Literacy (2022). The Science of Reading: The Basics. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Elementary and Secondary Education, Office of Special Education Programs, National Center on Improving Literacy . Retrieved from https://www.improvingliteracy.org .

PDF icon

Get started on your Science of Reading journey with these highlights about what it IS and what it IS NOT.

Related Resources

Apple on Desk

What are the characteristics of effective literacy instruction?

Science of reading: defining guide.

The "Science of Reading: Defining Guide" provides a firm definition of what the science of reading is, what it is not, and how all stakeholders can understand its potential to transform reading instruction.

Topic:  Evidence-based, General Literacy

science of reading case study

More on Evidence-based

  • A Snapshot of RTI Implementation a Decade Later: New Picture, Same Story
  • Are Individual Differences in Response to Intervention Influenced by the Methods and Measures Used to Define Response?
  • Comparison of Reading Growth Among Students With Severe Reading Deficits Who Received Intervention to Typically Achieving Students and Students Receiving Special Education
  • Important Pieces of School-Based Intervention for Students with or at Risk for Dyslexia: Evidence from 40 Years of Research
  • NCIL Intensification Framework

The research reported here is funded by awards to the National Center on Improving Literacy from the Office of Elementary and Secondary Education, in partnership with the Office of Special Education Programs (Award #: S283D160003). The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not represent views of OESE, OSEP, or the U.S. Department of Education. Copyright © 2024 National Center on Improving Literacy. https://www.improvingliteracy.org

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

The PMC website is updating on October 15, 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

The Science of Reading and Its Educational Implications

Mark s. seidenberg.

Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison

Research in cognitive science and neuroscience has made enormous progress toward understanding skilled reading, the acquisition of reading skill, the brain bases of reading, the causes of developmental reading impairments and how such impairments can be treated. My question is: if the science is so good, why do so many people read so poorly? I mainly focus on the United States, which fares poorly on cross-national comparisons of literacy, with about 25-30% of the population exhibiting literacy skills that are low by standard metrics. I consider three possible contributing factors, all of which turn on issues concerning the relationships between written and spoken language. They are: the fact that English has a deep alphabetic orthography; how reading is taught; and the impact of linguistic variability as manifested in the Black-White “achievement gap”. I conclude that there are opportunities to increase literacy levels by making better use of what we have learned about reading and language, but also institutional obstacles and understudied issues for which more evidence is badly needed.

Is there an area in cognitive science and cognitive neuroscience that has been more successful than the study of reading? Let's not underestimate the amount that has been learned. We, the community of scientists who study reading (including my colleagues Perfetti and Treiman, whose own research is described in accompanying articles) understand the basic mechanisms that support skilled reading, how reading skill is acquired, and the proximal causes of reading impairments.

We understand the fundamental problem facing the beginning reader: how to relate a new code, a written script, to an existing code, spoken language. We know which behaviors of 4 year old pre-readers are strong predictors of later reading ability, how children make the transition from pre-reader to reader, and the obstacles that many encounter. We know what distinguishes good and poor readers, younger and older skilled readers, “typical” readers from those who are atypical because of either constitutional factors (such as a hearing or learning impairment) or environmental ones (for example, poor schooling or poverty).

We know how basic skills that provide the child's entry into reading relate to other types of knowledge and capacities that support comprehending texts of increasing variety and difficulty. We understand that some aspects of reading are universal (because people's brains are essentially alike) and that some are not (because of differences among writing systems and the languages they represent).

Neuroimaging studies have been successful in identifying the main brain circuits involved in reading and the anomalous ways they develop in dyslexics, and several probable causes of such impairments. We have computational models that specify the mechanisms that underlie basic reading skills, how children acquire them, and how differences in experience (with spoken language and reading) and individual differences (in learning and memory capacities, motivation and other factors) result in varied reading outcomes. This vast research base has led to the development of intervention and remediation methods that can reliably help many children who need it. Researchers disagree about many details—it's science, not the Ten Commandments—but there is remarkable consensus about the basic theory of how reading works and the causes of reading successes and failures (for reviews, see Rayner et al., 2001; Pennington, 2006 ; Morris et al., 2010; Gabrieli, 2009 ; Pugh et al., 2012 ).

My question, then, is this: if the science is so advanced, why do so many people read so poorly? In America not long ago we had a “Sputnik moment,” occasioned by the release of the results of the 2009 round of the PISA cross-national assessments of the academic performance of 15 year olds (OECD PISA, 2009). As in previous years, US performance was close to the average for the 34 OECD countries. However, this round was the first to include data from Shanghai and Singapore, which along with Korea, Hong Kong, and Japan, scored higher than the US. These findings received far more attention than the fact that for many years the US has scored lower than countries such as Canada, Australia, Finland and New Zealand on the PISA exercise. The president, the secretary of education, and the commentariat (e.g., Finn, 2009) all treated the results as evidence of a crisis in American education that called for immediate action. But 2010 was not 1957 and so the second “Sputnik moment” passed quickly, rapidly dropping out of public discourse ( Fig. 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms503624f1.jpg

Number of times the phrase “sputnik moment” was uttered on CNN, an American cable news network over a two year period. The Larege spike followed the release of results from the 2009 PISA assessment and coincided with President Obama's 2011 State of the Union address.

Although the PISA results made the news, there is plenty of in-house data about the literacy problem in the US, posted on the Department of Education's web site ( http://ies.ed.gov ). The National Assessment of Adult Literacy (2003) found that about 93 million adults read at “basic” or “below basic” levels. At these levels, a person might be able to find the listing for a television program on cable TV, but not understand the instructions and warnings that come with their blood pressure medication ( Lesgold & Welch-Ross, 2012 ). Results on the NAEP (“the nation's report card”) document the origins of low literacy in the performance of 4 th and 8 th graders ( http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard ). Like everything else about education in the US, this assessment exercise has been the focus of controversy, with different stakeholders spinning the data in different ways. People who emphasize how well American education is doing point to the finding that since 1992, when the modern form of the NAEP was introduced, between 59-67% of 4 th graders and 69-76% of 8 th graders scored at the “basic” level or higher. People who think we should be doing better—I am in this camp—can point to the fact that 66-71% of 4 th graders and 66-71% of 8 th graders scored at “basic” or “below basic” levels. Put another way, there are far too many children scoring in the lowest tier (about a third of the 4 th graders and a quarter of the 8 th graders are “below basic”), and far too few in the highest (6-8% of the 4 th graders and a mere 3% of the 8 th graders are “advanced” readers by this measure). The American polity is in a test-happy phase and so there is much other data about who can read and how well than can be reviewed here. It is fair to say that assessments of adults and children consistently indicate that large numbers of individuals in the US read poorly, and that this has been true for many years.

Low literacy's consequences for the affected individuals and for society are vast, as we all know. It creates serious challenges to fully participating in the workforce, managing your own health care, and advancing your children's education. Looking at these facts, and knowing something about how reading works, I've asked myself whether our science has anything to contribute to improving literacy outcomes in this country and others. It might not. Literacy failure could be due to factors well outside the boundaries of this science: poverty, for example. Poverty has many sequelae, including higher infant mortality rate, atypical brain development, shorter life span, worse health and health care, higher crime and incarceration rates, lower educational achievement, higher dropout rates, poorer schools with less experienced teachers and toward the bottom of a list that could go on much longer, poor reading ( General Accounting Office, 2007 ). Surely reducing poverty would have a bigger impact on literacy than anything inspired by our research. Any person with a politically-acceptable plan to substantially reduce or eliminate poverty should step forward immediately.

If poverty were all that mattered, this article could end here. However, the relationship between socioeconomic status and reading achievement is not simple. It is difficult to isolate effects of SES (itself a complex construct; Duncan & Magnuson, 2005 ) from the many other factors with which it is correlated. Nonetheless, data from a variety of sources suggest that there is much about observed literacy outcomes that SES does not explain. I return to this issue in the final section of this article in the context of the Black-White “achievement gap”, where the confound between SES and achievement is of particular concern. Here I merely want to cite some representative findings suggesting that although poverty has enormous impact, it is not the whole story.

The PISA assessments provide a wealth of data (so to speak) about the relationship between national wealth and reading performance. The 2009 data set includes multiple measures related to a country's economic health for a core group of 34 OECD countries. The main findings are quite interesting. 1 In brief, two economic factors, the country's gross domestic product (GDP) and amount spent on education, are only weakly related to reading performance. The proportion of socio-economically disadvantaged students in each country has a bigger impact, with higher proportions associated with lower scores. However, the US does not score poorly because lower income students are overrepresented; in fact, the US clusters with many OECD countries on this measure, and reading scores in this group vary widely. It is also of interest that parents' education level is a much stronger predictor than economic indicators across countries. Of course, it takes more complex analyses to identify relations among such factors and their relative contributions. Nonetheless, even these descriptive data indicate that both SES and other factors are important determinants of outcomes.

The NAEP assessment also includes information regarding the moderating effects of a variety of factors, including race/ethnicity, gender, and eligibility for subsidized or free school lunch, a common (though rough) proxy for SES. 2 Again there are strong indications that both SES and other factors affect outcomes. There is a large, consistent effect of SES as indexed by the subsidized lunch proxy in every year of testing. However, there are similar results for other factors, such as gender. Females have scored significantly higher than males in every year of NAEP testing. Females also scored significantly higher than males in every participating country/municipality in the 2009 PISA assessment. (The US had one of the smaller gender gaps, whereas Finland, a perennial high-scoring country, had one of the largest.) The gender differences within and across countries may be related in some complex manner to SES, but the consistency of the effect across countries with widely varying economic profiles suggests that SES is not the main determinant.

Insofar as the US does not seem likely to substantially reduce or eliminate poverty any time soon and SES is not the only factor affecting reading outcomes, this article will not end here. To restate the question: what are the main causes of reading failures in the US (and perhaps other countries where similar conditions exist) and does reading science have anything to contribute to substantially reducing them, the considerable impact of poverty notwithstanding? I'll consider this question by examining three quite different kinds of factors often thought to be relevant to literacy outcomes in the US.

Blame English?

One possibility is that a certain number of people are doomed to fail to learn to read well because of intrinsic properties of English. The child's initial task is to learn how the written code relates to the spoken language they already know. The writing system is alphabetic, and we tell beginning readers that letters correspond to sounds, but then we teach them early reading vocabulary that includes HAVE, GIVE, SAID, SOME, WAS, WERE, IS, ME, ONE, WHO, SCHOOL, and many other words with atypical spelling-sound correspondences. These inconsistencies are a much commented-upon property of English. Other alphabetic writing systems are indeed more consistent at this level of analysis, many of them conforming (to a very high degree but not perfectly) to the principle that each symbol in the writing system (a “grapheme” consisting of one or more letters) correspond to a single unit (a phoneme) in the spoken language. English is said to be a “deep” orthography, whereas Italian, German, Russian, Finnish, Korean, Serbo-Croatian, and many other alphabets are “shallow” ( Katz & Frost, 1992 ). Written English is obviously a workable system but the learning curve is steep and a greater proportion of individuals may be left behind than if the writing system were shallow.

This hypothesis is contradicted by the consistently high reading achievement in countries such as Canada, New Zealand, Australia, and Singapore where English is also the main language of instruction (Quebec exception duly noted). Although these results suggest that written English is not the whole problem, perhaps performance would be even higher in these countries (and the US) if it were not so peculiar. These cross-national findings are correlational of course. What is needed is more direct evidence as to whether it is easier to learn to read in shallow alphabetic orthographies, holding other factors aside to the extent possible.

Researchers in many countries have attempted to address this question. By now it is quite clear that it is easier to learn to read words and nonwords aloud in shallow alphabetic orthographies compared to English (for reviews, see Aro & Wimmer, 2003 , and several chapters in Snowling & Hulme, 2005, and in Joshi & Aaron, 2006). The advantage for shallow orthographies has been observed in Italian, Spanish, German, French, Finnish, Serbian, Turkish and other languages. “Learning to read in Albanian” is “A skill easily acquired” according to Hoxhallari et al. (2004) because the alphabet is so shallow. Children know the full set of spelling-sound correspondences for Finnish, which has a shallow orthography, by the time formal schooling commences (at age 7 following a compulsory year of preschool). 3 Tested on their skill at reading words and nonwords aloud, children in Wales learning to read in Welsh (which has a shallow alphabetic script) outperform children from the same area who are learning to read in English ( Hanley et al., 2004 ). The Welsh studies permitted comparisons that excluded many potentially confounding socioeconomic and cultural factors. Such studies suggest in short, that shallow is easier. Share (2008) argues that theories of reading have been led astray because of overreliance on studies of English, an “outlier” among writing systems. Perhaps there would be higher literacy achievement in the US if the writing system were more like Finnish or Albanian.

I don't think so. For one thing, this comparative research on reading acquisition makes the mistake of equating the task of reading words and nonwords aloud with “reading” (as in Aro & Wimmer, 2004, Spencer & Hanley, 2003, and many other studies). Children are sometimes called upon to read aloud, in classrooms and in experiments; reading aloud provides overt evidence about the child's knowledge of words and the opportunity to provide explicit feedback (e.g., corrections of mispronunciations). Because of the nature of the writing system, the child's ability to name words and nonwords aloud in English is a major step in reading acquisition. The task has also provided a domain in which to explore statistical learning procedures (Harm & Seidenberg, 1999) that are relevant to language acquisition, visual cognition, and much else. The goal of reading, however, is comprehension. Reading aloud is much more strongly related to comprehension in English than in shallow orthographies (see, e.g., Lindgren, deRenzi, & Richman, 1985 ). In shallow orthographies, reading aloud can be achieved without comprehending what is being said, indeed without knowing the language. I know this to be true because I proved it at my Bar Mitzvah. Modern Hebrew can be written with or without vowels. With the vowels included, the writing system is shallow: words have simple and consistent spelling-sound correspondences, which can be learned rapidly, comprehension not required. Fortuitously, Hebrew is a good “Bar Mitzvah language” ( Seidenberg, 2011 ), as are Finnish, Albanian, Welsh, Italian and other shallow alphabetic orthographies.

One would not want to confuse barking at print with reading comprehension, however. Phil Gough would not have. According to his “simple view of reading” ( Hoover & Gough, 1990 ), children's reading comprehension is a function of decoding skills (recognizing letters, relating print to sound) and knowledge of spoken language (vocabulary and grammar). These skills are dissociable. If the writing system is sufficiently shallow, a person can learn to read aloud without comprehension (my Hebrew). Conversely, a person can know a spoken language quite well without being able to read it (as is true of most 5 year olds who speak English). Among clinicians and researchers, there is a move to reserve the term “dyslexia” for a developmental reading impairment that interferes with acquiring basic print-related skills, especially ability to relate print to sound, independent of spoken language comprehension ( Snowling & Hulme, 2011 ). Other children acquire adequate decoding skills but comprehend texts poorly; these children are also “poor readers,” but a different diagnostic category is needed because their poor reading comprehension is secondary to deficiencies in spoken language.

Granted that it is easy to learn to decode in shallow orthographies, does this confer a comprehension advantage as well? Few studies have closely examined reading aloud, reading comprehension, and spoken language abilities in the same children, although there are some interesting leads. The studies of children learning to read in Welsh and English yielded an interesting tradeoff: whereas the Welsh children performed much better at reading common words and simple nonwords aloud, the English children scored higher when tested on comprehension. As Hanley et al. (2004) noted, “This result suggests that a transparent orthography does not confer any advantages as far as reading comprehension is concerned. As comprehension is clearly the goal of reading, this finding is potentially reassuring for teachers of English” (p. 1408). Why the English children exhibited better comprehension with poorer reading aloud cannot be determined with certainty from these studies. The comparison between Welsh-learning and English-learning children is not entirely clean because the sociolinguistic context is such that English is the dominant language. The Welsh-learning children therefore have substantial knowledge of English (and are bilingual to some degree), whereas the English-learning children have much less knowledge of Welsh and are essentially monolingual. What is clear is that ability to read aloud may say little about the child's reading comprehension.

Durgunoğlu (2006) reached a similar conclusion from extensive studies of reading in Turkish. Turkish has a shallow orthography and a complex, highly productive agglutinating morphology. Summarizing, she noted that “Phonological awareness and decoding develop rapidly in both young and adult readers of Turkish because of the transparent orthography and the special characteristics of phonology and morphology. However, reading comprehension is still a problem.” (2006, p. 226). In her experiments, children's comprehension lagged substantially behind their ability to pronounce words aloud, which she attributes to properties of the spoken language, specifically that complex morphological system, which takes native speakers many years to learn. Whereas comprehension develops more rapidly than production in learning a first language, shallow orthographies create the opposite effect: production—reading aloud—can advance more rapidly than comprehension.

Even within English, accuracy in reading aloud and reading comprehension frequently decouple. Skilled readers are able to read and comprehend many words they mispronounce. Here are some I collected from students and colleagues—words they did not know how to pronounce or systematically mispronounced for many years:

EgregiousQuay
PiquantHegemony
SuaveAutomata
RapportChaos
CoitusFacade
ClitorisEnnui
EpitomeSleight
SegueUranus

All true. A graduate student who spent a portion of his youth immersed in the computer game Chaos: The Battle of Wizards didn't realize until much later that it was connected to the spoken form /ke i -αs/. The two pronunciations of URANUS seem to be in free variation in the US. People can be more adept at engaging in coitus than pronouncing it. A personal example: the Seidenberg and McClelland (1989) model learned to pronounce QUAY as /kwe i / because the training lexicon was created by hand and that is how I thought it was pronounced (it was corrected in later models). As a land-locked kid growing up on the south side of Chicago, I knew the word from reading but not speech. These cases show that a person can know the meaning of a written word but lack secure knowledge of the pronunciation. People frequently generate erroneous pronunciations that they would not have heard in spoken language. If words like these were used in a reading-aloud experiment, even adult, highly skilled readers of English would perform more poorly than Welsh or Turkish subjects.

Nation and Cocksey (2009) found that 7 year old English-speaking children often know the meanings of words they incorrectly read aloud. Familiarity with the spoken form of a word (as indexed by auditory lexical decision performance) was related to accuracy in reading it aloud, especially for words with irregular spelling-sound correspondences. Across subjects, 521 words were read aloud incorrectly; the correct definition was provided for 328 of them (63%). Of course the fact that ability to comprehend and pronounce words can dissociate should have been obvious from the mere existence of severely hearing impaired deaf individuals who do not receive oral training, do not know the pronunciations of words, but are nonetheless good readers. (The fact that it difficult to become a skilled reader under these conditions is an important but separate issue; Goldin-Meadow & Mayberry, 2001 ).

In summary, reading aloud is not a good index of reading comprehension or a basis for evaluating “ease of learning to read” different writing systems. We should therefore be skeptical of claims that it is easy to learn to read in shallow orthographies, and of the corollary belief that English is especially difficult. Over the past 20 years or so, researchers in many countries have correctly recognized the importance of obtaining data about reading in languages other than English, but attempted to correct the imbalance by replicating studies that had been conducted in English using reading aloud, a task that is more closely related to comprehension in that language precisely because of orthographic idiosyncrasies they were trying to surmount.

The relationship between writing systems and spoken languages

The orthographic depth hypothesis is an example of a very interesting idea that drew attention to an important issue (differences in how writing systems represent phonology and their potential impact on reading) and stimulated an enormous amount of research but turned out to be wrong. The hypothesis narrowly focused on the computation of phonology from print. Given the dependence of reading on spoken language, it seemed to follow that writing systems for which it was easier to compute phonology, the shallow ones, would also be easier to comprehend, other factors being equal. This prediction did not turn out to be correct because other factors are manifestly unequal. Looking across languages and writing systems, it can be seen that the properties of writing systems are related to properties of the languages they represent, in particular the complexity of the language's inflectional morphology. Inflectional morphology is an especially important component of language because it is an interface system conveying information about words and the syntactic structures in which they participate, and a major source of typological variation. Languages such as Welsh and Turkish have shallow writing systems but they are morphologically complex, marking properties such as case, number and gender. English and the Sinitic languages (Mandarin, Taiwanese, Cantonese, et al.) exhibit the opposite pattern: the writing systems are deep but their inflectional systems are simple. Looking at English, Gough had observed that early reading comprehension is a function of knowledge of print and knowledge of spoken language. With a cross-linguistic perspective it becomes clear that the two components are not independent. What has to be learned about print depends on properties of the writing system, which bear a non-arbitrary relationship to spoken language typology.

I have attempted to unify these broad cross-linguistic tendencies under the concept of “grapholinguistic equilibrium” ( Seidenberg, 2011 ). The writing systems that have survived support comprehension about equally well. A writing system's capacity to support comprehension can be thought of as a constant that is maintained via trade-offs between orthographic complexity (“depth”, number and complexity of symbols, etc.) and spoken language complexity (particularly morphosyntactic). For languages such as Welsh or Turkish, the spelling-sound correspondences are easily learned, but the morphology is not. These conditions allow children to accurately read aloud sentences that they would not be able to produce or fully comprehend given their still-developing knowledge of the spoken language. Written English is deep but the inflectional system is trivial and little impediment to comprehension. Under these conditions, children easily produce and comprehend sentences that they cannot accurately read aloud.

A deep orthography would be highly dysfunctional, possibly unlearnable, in languages with complex morphosyntax. To illustrate consider Serbo-Croatian. Classic studies focused on its highly consistent spelling-sound correspondences, so different from those in English ( Katz & Frost, 1992 ). My colleagues and I have been more interested in its inflectional system ( Mirković et al., 2004 , 2011 ), which is also very different from English. The system is unquestionably complex. Both nouns and verbs are inflected and there are inflections for number, gender, case, and tense. The inflections are not independent: Number on nouns, for example, depends on case and gender. The inflections are not discrete beads on a string, either: the system is fusional, such that a single suffix encodes multiple inflections. Then there is an additional wrinkle: the realization of an inflection depends on phonological properties of the root to which it is attached ( Table 1 ). The base form SAVETNIK (masculine, “advisor”) is zero-inflected. The final consonant K [/k/] is not retained throughout the inflectional paradigm, changing to C [/ts/] and Č [/tʃ/]. The inflection –E is used for both the vocative singular and the accusative plural; in the former, it is preceded by Č, in the latter by K. It is clear even from this sliver of the language that there is a lot to learn.

WordCase and numberInflection
SAVETNIKNominative singularZero inflected
SAVETNICINominative plural-I
SAVETNIKAGenitive singular-A
SAVETNIČEVocative singular-E
SAVETNIKEAccusative plural-E

Note: All forms are masculine gender. K = /k/ as in Kevin, Č = /t?/ as in “church,” C = /ts/ as in “pizza.”

Now imagine trying to read this language in a script that is more like English, with single letters that represent multiple vowels (e.g., DOSE, LOSE, POSE). Then toss in a few random consonants with multiple pronunciations, such as C (as in CAP and CENT), G (GOAT, GIN), and Y (YOUNG, EDGY). The complexity of the inflectional system is already high. Adding ambiguity in the pronunciations of written letters would increase it enormously. The proper form of an inflection depends on the pronunciation of the previous consonant, but now the pronunciation of the letter representing that consonant will sometimes also depend on context (as with the ambiguous English letters). There would be further penalties if mastery of a complex morphological system requires formal instruction that itself involves reading.

It would take quantitative analyses or simulation models to determine the effects of additional orthographic indeterminacy and establish when the system would become intractable for human learners. The historical fact that languages with complex morphological systems have shallow orthographies is itself suggestive of pressures to maintain this equilibrium, however. Indeed, many times the alignment of language and writing system has been achieved with active intervention, as with the Armenian alphabet in the fifth century, Hangul in 15th century Korea, and Serbo-Croatian in the 19 th century (see Daniels & Bright, 1996 ).

In summary, there is no free orthographic lunch. The child does gain entry into reading more quickly if the associations between units in the written and spoken languages are simple and consistent. However, learning to read aloud in shallow writing systems is a bit like learning to play the violin in the Suzuki method. Both allow the child to rapidly begin performing with relatively little instruction. A four year old's performance of the “Twinkle Variations” may well be the musical equivalent of barking at print. Being able to pronounce words aloud is a helpful skill to possess if your task is to learn a complex, quasiregular morphological system over a many-year period that extends into formal schooling. But, there is little evidence that precocious knowledge of spelling-sound correspondences confers a comprehension advantage, or that the irregularities in written English present an especial burden. 4

What About How Reading is Taught?

American educators have never been able to settle on how to teach children to read. The issue has been debated since Horace Mann was head of the Massachusetts Board of Education in the 1840s. Mann described letters as “skeleton-shaped, bloodless, ghostly apparitions” and encouraged teaching children to read whole words at a time—a lesson that “would be like an excursion to the fields of Elysium” compared to other practices. Mann's tone—authoritative assertion coupled with contempt for other views—is characteristic of much of the subsequent 150 years of debate. 5

How much of the literacy problem in America is due to the way reading has been taught? Everyone knows about the “reading wars” of the past 30 years–the debate over “phonics” and “whole language” approaches. The 2000s saw the emergence of a compromise called “Balanced Literacy,” said to incorporate the best aspects of the two approaches. “Balanced literacy” is a Treaty of Versailles solution that allowed educators to declare the increasingly troublesome “wars” over without having seriously addressed the underlying causes of the strife. The issues are complex, controversial, and ongoing. Here I want to briefly examine some basic considerations, from the perspective of a scientist who studies how reading works, which suggest that how reading is taught is indeed a significant part of the literacy problem in the US and other countries. There are three main points: (a) Contemporary reading science has had very little impact on educational practice mainly because of a two-cultures problem separating science and education; (b) This disconnection has been harmful. Current practices rest on outdated assumptions about reading and development that make learning to read harder than it needs to be, a sure way to leave many children behind; (c) Connecting the science to educational practice would be beneficial but is extremely difficult to achieve. The current environment limits the amount of collaborative work at the all-important translational interface. In the US, the conflicting and often strongly entrenched interests of various stakeholders—educators, politicians, scientists, taxpayers, labor organizations, parent groups—make it hard to achieve meaningful change within the existing institutional structure of public education.

My comments about the culture of education (by which I mean beliefs and attitudes about how children learn and the functions of schooling, particularly with respect to reading) may seem harsh to readers who are not close to the issues. Many people will naturally assume that although scientists and educators may have different views, both have much to contribute and the path to greater progress is through cooperation. Every academic is aware of the importance of interdisciplinary work and of the challenges involved in communicating across disciplines. We also know that the successful creation of cross-disciplinary bridges can have transformative effects, sometimes leading to the emergence of new fields that are much more than the sum of the disciplinary parts. Such a transformation is needed in education and I hope it can be achieved. The question is how. It may be hard for people who are unfamiliar with the landscape to appreciate just how difficult the challenges are. As someone who has been immersed in these issues for many years I have struggled with finding ways to have a positive impact, and that is reflected in the material that follows (see also Seidenberg, 2012).

You may believe, as I usually do, that the collegial and politically-astute approach is to assume that well-intentioned individuals can transcend their differences in the service of a shared goal. Disciplinary barriers only exist as long as we allow them to. We can all do better jobs communicating what we do and what we've learned. Bridges are built on a foundation of mutual respect for individuals and diverse viewpoints. People are doing the best they can; neither side knows everything. I fully support creative bridge-building and have engaged in it myself, but I have come to question whether good intentions and greater effort can be any more effective going forward than they have been in the past. These positive and sincere impulses might have a better chance of succeeding if there were better understanding of the deep differences between the cultures of science and education, which are manifested in their discordant approaches to reading (see also Seidenberg, forthcoming ).

It is important to note that there is plenty of good science relevant being conducted within schools of education, often in departments such as Educational Psychology; however, it is isolated from programs focused on professional training and the development of curricula and instructional practices. My comments on the culture of education focus on the training-and-practices side. I should also stress that my concerns are not about teachers, but rather about what teachers are taught (about child development in general and reading in particular) and about how curricula and instructional practices are created and evaluated. I am not challenging anyone's integrity, commitment, motivation, effort, sincerity, or intelligence. But I am challenging some deeply-held beliefs that have guided educational policies and practices for many years. I would expect this to be discomfiting for many people, but also recognizable as relevant to their deep commitments to helping students learn.

Finally, I must acknowledge that my treatment of these issues is incomplete, given this article's length limitations. Below I mainly characterize the current situation rather than how it arose. The resistance to the reading science of the recent past also needs to be considered in a historical context, which includes earlier attempts to base educational practices on the science of the moment. It also needs to be considered in light of other challenges to educators' traditional control over educational policies and practices (including federal intervention via legislation such as No Child Left Behind, and powerful new educational philanthropies; Ravitch, 2011 ). I provide this broader context in Seidenberg (forthcoming) .

The Two Cultures of Science and Education

Learning to read is an educational issue, historically the purview of educators, specifically schools of education. The history of education in the US has been extensively documented, mainly from the perspective of educators themselves (e.g., Ravitch, 2000 ; Cremin, 1988 ). Popping up a level, one sees that science and education occupy different territory in the intellectual world (literally so on many university campuses). The result is that people who are studying the same thing—how children learn to read, for example—can nonetheless have little contact. The cultures of education and science are radically different: they have different goals and values, ways of training new practitioners, criteria for evaluating progress. The two cultures also communicate their research at separate conferences sponsored by parallel professional organizations attended by different audiences, and publish their work in different journals. There are publishers that target one audience and not the other. These cross-cultural differences, like many others, are difficult to bridge.

Psychologists have been studying reading since the 19 th century and educators have had an approach-avoidance conflict about it ever since. Education as a discipline embraced a few theorists with roots in modern psychology—Dewey, Vygotsky, Piaget, and Bruner among others—whose work underlies the deeply entrenched “constructivist” approach in education ( Tobias & Duffy, 2009 ). There is deep skepticism about the relevance of empirical studies that utilize the tools of modern experimental cognitive and developmental psychology, whether in laboratories or classroom settings (e.g., Coles, 2000 ); however, it co-exists with a readiness to appropriate findings that are consistent with existing beliefs and practices. The special role of science—to find out, to the best of our ability, what is true, letting the implications fall where they may—is subverted if people selectively attend to the findings they find congenial: it transforms research studies into another form of anecdote. Educators also use our research as a source of novel findings that feed the relentless demand for educational innovation. Often this means getting far too carried away far too rapidly with findings that are interesting and new but also not solidly established or understood.

These conflicting attitudes about science and education are at the heart of controversies about reading instruction. What I'll call the Modern Synthesis about learning to read, reading skill, and the relationship between reading and language emerged from work conducted since the 1970s, beginning with Gibson and Levin (1978) , Liberman et al. (1977) , Gough (e.g., Gough & Hillinger, 1980 ; Hoover & Gough, 1990 ), Stanovich (1980) , and others. Almost all of this research was conducted by scientists working outside traditional departments and schools of education. The empirical findings underlying the Modern Synthesis were summarized in several white papers commissioned by various agencies ( Adams, 1990 ; Snow et al., 1998 ; National Reading Panel, 2000 ; Snow, 2002 ; Lonigan & Shanahan, 2009 ). This research called into question basic assumptions underlying how reading is taught and what teachers are taught about reading and development—most importantly the idea that the way that children acquire a first, spoken language provides a good model for learning to read—and yet it has had little subsequent impact on them. The conflicts between scientific and educational approaches to reading continue, centered on three issues.

1. Deciding what is true

One of the major cross-cultural differences concerns attitudes about evidence. There is a movement to encourage evidence-based practices in education, analogous to the ones in medicine and clinical psychology (see http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc ). The effort founders, however, if the stakeholders do not agree on what counts as evidence, or who should decide. Many educators are dismissive of attempts to examine reading from a scientific perspective, which is seen as sterile and reductive, intrinsically incapable of capturing the ineffable character of the learning moment, or the chemistry of a successful classroom ( Coles, 2000 ). Education as a discipline has placed much higher value on observation and hard-earned classroom experience. This division was apparent in reactions to the NRP report (2000) . The panel reviewed the scientific literature relevant to learning to read, having established explicit a priori criteria for what kinds of studies would be considered. Those criteria excluded studies that educators value: mainly, observational, quasi-ethnographic studies of individual schools, teachers, classrooms, and children that do not attempt to conform to basic principles of experimental design or data analysis (see, e.g., Barton & Hamilton, 1998; Rasinski et al., 2011 ). The report was therefore of little interest, to many educators except as evidence for a scientistic bias at odds with the educational establishment's core values ( Krashen, 2001 ). 6

From the perspective of modern studies of cognition, educators' confidence in the reliability of their own observations and experiences in classroom settings is baffling. If teachers really could figure out how reading works and children learn just by observation and experience, there wouldn't be a literacy problem or debates about best practices. But what we can learn about reading this way is limited. Most of what we do when we read is subconscious: we are aware of the result—whether we understood a text or not, whether we found the information we were seeking. Neither teachers nor scientists can directly observe children's mental and neural processes; what can be intuited about them based on classroom experience is limited, and intuitions often conflict. Introspection and systematic personal observation were the main methodologies used by the founders of modern psychology ( Boring, 1953 ), but discovery of their limitations led to the adoption of less observer-dependent methods. The limitations are even greater than the early psychologists could have known. What people observe depends on what they believe (see Cox et al., 2004 , for a striking illustration). Inferences based on observation are subject to deep-seated biases that required Nobel-prize caliber research to uncover ( Kahneman, 2011 ). The limitations of personal observation and experience are among the reasons why we conduct this other, scientific, kind of research: to understand components of reading that would otherwise be hidden from view and to do it in an objective, independently verifiable way. A folk psychology about how we read based on intuition and observation does not become any more reliable when elevated to educational principle, but that is the modern history of educational theorizing about reading.

2. The socio-cultural approach

The Modern Synthesis developed out of research that examined reading within the broader context of research on human language and cognition and their neural and computational bases. Within education, a much more influential approach has emphasized the socio-cultural aspects of literacy, particularly the status of reading in different cultural, linguistic, and socio-economic subgroups (e.g., Gee, 1997 ; Au, 1998 ; Scribner & Cole, 1981 ; Moje & Luke, 2009 ). The approach emphasizes attitudes toward reading within such groups; the varied purposes for which people read in different contexts defined by situation, culture, language, or SES; the relevance of different reading-related activities to learners in these contexts; and how socio-cultural factors affect a child's motivation to learn to read and which classroom practices will be successful.

Much of what is assumed within the socio-cultural approach seems true enough, at an informal level: reading isn't a unitary task; how we read depends on what we are reading and for what purpose; in developing a curriculum it would be wise to take into account the cultural and socio-economic context, including different attitudes toward reading and differences in experiences and opportunities outside the classroom that can greatly affect children's progress. These factors are likely to have a strong impact on the child's motivation to read, a very significant factor that reading scientists have largely ignored.

The socio-cultural research addresses important issues; they are deeply implicated in the “achievement gaps” discussed in the next section. The problem is that socio-cultural paradigm is positioned as an alternative to studies of the types of knowledge and processing mechanisms that underlie reading and how they are acquired, rather than addressing complementary issues. The tension between these approaches furnished the subtext for the “reading wars”. The heart of the conflict was a debate about the validity of what were termed “skills” vs. “literacy” approaches, which, amazingly, were seen as competing alternatives. 7 The scientists were seen as focused on “skills” (e.g., learning to read words and sentences accurately and fluently; vocabulary development), whereas educators emphasized developing “literacy” (the child's appreciation the various types and uses of written language, by individuals with diverse backgrounds, values and cultural traditions). Classroom time is a zero-sum game and so choices between skills and literacy had to be made. Moreover, teaching basic skills to beginning readers was thought to be counterproductive because it stifles children's natural curiosity about reading and their motivation to learn. This basic skills stuff may be necessary but it is also poisonous in large doses, so the child should be exposed to as little of it as possible. The traditional goal of teaching children to read has been replaced by coaching: encouraging the appreciation of and engagement in “multiple literacies.” Educational theorizing has gone “meta” about reading: there's little about how reading works (i.e., its neurocognitive bases), and much about how reading is used (various “literacy practices”) and by whom (various cultural/ethnic/language groups).

This conflict—which I am by no means overstating—arises from a failure to assume a genuinely developmental perspective. The act of reading and comprehending text involves the coordination of cognitive, linguistic, perceptual, motoric, memory and learning capacities. Understanding these capacities, how they develop, and how they are recruited in support of reading is obviously relevant to being able to help children become successful readers. What is relevant to teach (or “facilitate”) depends on where the child is on an extended developmental trajectory. The ability to read and comprehend words and their components is a basic, foundational skill. Helping children achieve this skill, without creating disinterest in reading, is the educational challenge. Acquisition of this foundation allows the child to benefit from other activities that promote further advancement: extended practice reading a variety of texts, with close checks on comprehension; reading texts for different purposes; gaining background knowledge relevant to what is being read. Socio-economic and cultural factors are highly relevant to the child's ability to benefit from schooling, but they do not change the nature of the reading process, or the kinds of knowledge and skills that need to be acquired.

3. Scientific literacy

The gap between the cultures ensures that people coming from the education side have little opportunity to gain an understanding of how research is conducted in relevant disciplines such as cognition, development, and neuroscience. Schools of education socialize prospective teachers into an ideology about children, learning, and reading. Prospective teachers are not exposed to other research that is relevant to their jobs, which is especially damaging given how difficult those jobs are. Educators are unprepared to engage this science in a serious way because they lack the tools to understand what is studied, how it is studied, what is found, what it means, and its relation to other kinds of research. This also leaves educators vulnerable to claims that are intuitively appealing but unproved, overhyped, or discredited. Educators embrace the importance of “critical thinking skills” and “background knowledge” in reading and learning, and so it is ironic when they are missing from discussions of research on reading and learning. I think that this deep ambivalence about the relevance of science to the educational mission explains seemingly contradictory features of educational culture such as the cherry-picking of selected findings, while at the same time discounting the relevance of basic research (e.g., Duke & Martin, 2011 ). I think it also explains why the single most influential educational theorist in America is Lev Vygotsky, who lived in the Soviet Union, wrote in Russian, died in 1934, and never saw an American classroom, or a television, computer, calculator, videogame or smartphone, yet educators are also looking to the latest findings from neuroscience for help (e.g., Willis, 2007 ). It is hard to know what Vygotsky, who founded the socio-cultural framework for education as an alternative to approaches based on psychology and biology, would have thought of this latest development.

Does It Matter?

The people who teach teachers and create curricula don't pay much attention to the science of reading, but is there reason to think that closer alignment of science and education would result in better outcomes? There have always been competing views about how reading should be taught, or, indeed, if it needs to be taught at all. People who have had vastly different educational experiences manage to become skilled readers. We know that teacher quality has a huge impact on educational outcomes (e.g., Hanushek & Rivkin, 2006 ) , but what about different ways of teaching reading?

It should matter. Reading is a learned skill, an “unnatural act” in Gough's memorable phrase. Some children find it easy to learn to read regardless of what happens in the classroom; many are well on their way by the onset of formal schooling. Other children will have difficulty learning to read regardless of what happens in the classroom because they are dyslexic: they have a developmental disorder that interferes with learning to read. Few teacher education programs provide any serious training related to developmental disorders such as dyslexia, how children at risk can be identified, and how such children can be helped. Whereas researchers are closing in on the neural and genetic bases of dyslexia ( Gabrieli, 2009 ), educational theorists are still debating whether dyslexia exists, and if it does, whether knowing that a child has the disorder should have any impact on classroom practices ( Elliott & Gibbs, 2008 ). Many of those children and adults who score poorly on national assessments are undoubtedly dyslexics whose condition has not been identified or addressed.

Between these extremes there is the great majority of children for whom how reading is taught matters a great deal. They are why we should care about what teachers are taught about reading. The main problem is that many of the basic assumptions about how children learn to read that have guided teacher education, classroom practices, and curriculum development have been contradicted by the basic research that lead to the Modern Synthesis. Beliefs about reading, learning, and development, reinforced over many years within the insular culture of schools of education, do not coincide with facts about reading, learning, and development uncovered using a variety of methods in laboratory and naturalistic settings. Rather than repeat details reviewed in sources I've already mentioned, let me try to capture the essence of the problem.

Everyone agrees that children have to acquire basic skills related to processing the visual code (letter recognition, learning about orthographic structure and the relationships between orthography and phonology, etc.), which provide a foundation for developing the ability to comprehend different kinds of texts for different purposes. Beyond this basic observation, there are two contradictory views.

Educators have assumed that basic skills are relatively easy to acquire, but comprehension is hard. Acquiring basic skills is mostly a matter of providing a literacy-rich environment with activities that engage and motivate the child. Learning to read was assumed to be like learning a spoken language. Children do not need to be explicitly instructed in how to read any more than they needed instruction in how to speak a first language. In practice—a Whole Language K-3 classroom—this meant de-emphasizing instruction related to acquiring basic skills. In the appropriate environment, full of “authentic” literature (rather than books written for the purpose of teaching reading), literacy activities focused on extended, “multisensory” engagement with a book (e.g., reading a book to the child, small groups of children reading the book aloud together, making a personal copy of the book, drawing pictures of the book, coloring the book, “writing” about the book using invented spelling, talking about the book, etc.), the child would discover the mechanics. Following John Dewey, discovering how reading works is assumed to have more value than being taught to read. The teacher's role is to promote literacy, not teach reading.

Comprehension, in contrast, was thought to be hard. The great fear was that children might develop basic skills and yet fail to comprehend texts. (Indeed it was thought that an initial focus on phonics would make it harder to become a good comprehender.) And so, inspired by theorists such as Frank Smith (1971 , now in its sixth edition), curricula focused on developing the child's explicit knowledge about text structure, types of inferences, the varied relationships between author and reader, the varied goals of reading, how to monitor comprehension and repair errors, and so on.

On the science side, the story is the exact opposite. Basic skills are difficult to acquire (mainly because of the partial and abstract way that writing systems represent spoken language) and thus the area where instruction matters most. Comprehension, in contrast, depends on extended experience using spoken and written language for varied purposes. Environments and activities that provide such experience can therefore promote comprehension skill. Ironically, this aspect of becoming a skilled reader more closely resembles spoken language acquisition than does the acquisition of basic skills.

Reading comprehension initially depends on the child's knowledge of spoken language. Learning a first language involves encoding detailed information about the statistical structure of the utterances to which the child is exposed ( Seidenberg, 1997 ). This information is continually updated through the lifespan, via language use ( Haskell, Thornton, & MacDonald, 2010 ). Comprehension abilities vary among children because there are huge individual differences with respect to the sheer amount of spoken language to which the child is exposed, and the range of vocabulary items and sentence structures it includes. Thus, reading comprehension can be improved by enriching the child's knowledge of spoken language ( Hoff, 2013 ).

Children who are given the instruction and support to acquire basic skills can advance to reading varied texts for varied purposes, learning from feedback about whether they are succeeding rather than explicit instruction in how to comprehend. Promoting skill development through engagement and feedback is different from teaching the child a meta-theory of comprehension. Eventually the relationship between spoken and written language becomes reciprocal and interactive. Knowledge of spoken language facilitates learning to read; the child can then acquire vocabulary and familiarity with diverse grammatical structures from reading as well as speech. What is learned from reading also contributes to the further development of spoken language skills. For skilled readers, the systems become closely knit, even at the neural level ( Pattamadilok et al., 2010 ).

In short, theorists on the education side had the instructional vs. experiential demands of acquiring basic skills vs. comprehension backward . Generations of teachers were then taught that the skills come naturally and that comprehension requires explicit instruction. That reversal has made learning to read harder for many children.

Finally, because of this two-cultures problem, there is very little opportunity to focus on how to best integrate basic research with educational practices. The science of reading is highly advanced but it does not come with prescriptions about educational practice attached. It is one thing to know about how children learn to read and another to translate that knowledge into classrooms. The NRP report again offers a good illustration of the zeitgeist. The report did a good job describing the main elements involved in learning to read, and the supporting evidence. It was not within the panel's mandate to address the educational issue, how these components could be affectively addressed within an integrated, multi-year reading curriculum. Thus the report described the importance of elements such as phonemic awareness and vocabulary in early reading, but not the levels of competence that are developmentally appropriate or how to assess them, or the effectiveness of different instructional methods. This created an enormous loophole. It is very easy to design curricula that can be said to conform to the recommendations of the NRP, simply by touching on all of the components they listed, even if only for a day. There has not been a serious dialogue about the pedagogical implications of the science summarized in the report, one of the major factors contributing to the science's lack of impact.

The Impact of Language Variation in the Educational Context

There cannot be a serious discussion of literacy issues in the US without considering the “achievement gap.” The term refers to disparities in academic performance between groups of individuals. It is mainly used with reference to minority groups – African Americans, Hispanics, Native Americans – compared to whites, but there are many other “gaps.” There are huge “gaps” associated with income disparities ( Reardon, 2011 ), and there are gaps for other groups such as first generation children of immigrants to the US compared to later generation children. Such “gaps” are seen in reading, math, science and in other areas, and on a variety of indices, including grades, standardized test scores, the kind of classes students take, high school and college completion rates, and so on (Washington et al., in press). My focus is on the achievement gap in reading and, again, whether what we have learned from our research could be brought to bear on it. 8

I am also going to limit attention to the gap for African Americans, even though they exist for groups defined in many other ways. Why single out this group? First, because this gap is a major issue for a very large number of affected individuals. Second, because it is part of a long history of racial inequality in the US. Third, because it has been the focus of attention from politicians, educators, and economists for many years ( Jencks & Phillips, 1998 ; Equity and Excellence Commission, 2013 ). Fourth, because this gap has been persistent. It has existed for as long as relevant data have been collected, with little change despite government efforts dating from the War on Poverty through No Child Left Behind to Race to the Top. Fifth, it is an area in which I am conducting research (Washington et al., in press). Finally, the various achievement gaps in our society have varying causes. Conditions and circumstances that are highly relevant to one group may be moot for another. Although this focus is warranted it is also essential to recognize it as part of a much broader phenomenon affecting many diverse groups of people.

The causes of the achievement gap in reading for African Americans (and other groups) are obviously complex and cannot be covered fully here (see, e.g., Richardson, 2008 ; Magnuson & Duncan, 2006 ; Barton & Coley, 2009 ; Washington et al., in press). The topic is also a sensitive one, having to do with generalizations about groups, within which there is enormous variation. What is said here cannot be assumed to apply to all members of a group, or to any individual within the group. It can also be harmful to raise issues about group differences in contexts that do not permit serious exploration and exchange of ideas. 9 I also know from experience that anything that is said about this issue, however well-reasoned, backed by evidence, and carefully stated with necessary qualifications attached can be spun for political purposes that researchers cannot control. My goal here is limited: to establish the relevance of research on reading and language to understanding and potentially ameliorating this gap. My only personal agenda is to encourage others to conduct research in the area, for the same reasons I became involved: because the issues are scientifically interesting; because existing research on reading and language acquisition is relevant; because there is a research gap insofar as the factors and conditions specific to African American children's reading acquisition are understudied; because this gap could be addressed by researchers who study other aspects of language acquisition and reading; and because the consequences of reading failures are so devastating.

Econometric Analyses: What Is Missing from this Picture?

The basis of the Black-White achievement gap in reading has been extensively studied by econometricians and sociologists. Several important analyses have utilized a large publicly-available data set, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study ( http://nces.ed.gov/ecls ). These data were derived from extensive interviews with large numbers of individuals (about 20,000), supplemented with scores on assessment instruments. Consistent with other research, the ECLS data show that there is an achievement gap at the start of schooling: African American children are behind on measures of reading and pre-reading skills in kindergarten ( Duncan & Magnuson, 2005 ). Researchers have attempted to identify the bases of this gap by determining which factors in the data set account for the difference. Fryer and Levitt (2004) showed that six factors (out of over a hundred that were considered) accounted for the difference at the onset of schooling in this statistical sense: a composite measure of socioeconomic status, child's age at the start of kindergarten, birth weight, age of mother at time of first birth, whether the mother was a WIC (welfare) participant, and number of children's books in the home. These results are correlational, of course, and open to varied interpretation. This particular set of factors seems to be mainly tapping into SES and sequelae such as poorer health and health services, and fewer resources such as books in the home.

The results from additional waves of data collection yielded the surprising finding that the size of the gap increased through grade 3 ( Fryer & Levitt, 2006 ; see also Magnuson & Duncan, 2006 ). The causal interpretation is again unclear; schooling could either be exacerbating the gap, or the positive effects of schooling might be outpaced by the increasing impact of other factors. In either case, schooling was not acting as the “great equalizer”. 10 Moreover, the six factors that had accounted for the gap in kindergarten did not account for the gap in third grade, nor did any other factors in the data set. Fryer and Levitt wrote, “None of the explanations we examine[d], including systematic differences in school quality across races, convincingly explain the divergent trajectory of Black students.”

The fact that SES-related factors did not account for the increase in the gap is consistent with other findings indicating that the reading gap is not limited to lower income individuals. As noted above, performance on the NAEP is affected by SES, as indexed by the eligibility for subsidized lunch proxy. However, the black-white gap is highly consistent across the three levels of this measure (see Vanneman et al., 2009 , p. 33). Other studies have found that the gap exists for middle income blacks as well as low income ( Gosa & Alexander, 2007 ). Again it has to be emphasized that with overlapping distributions and imperfect correlations, there will be individuals who differ from these overall trends, including lower SES blacks whose reading achievement is on par with high-achieving, higher SES whites, a point that Magnuson and Duncun (2006 , p. 368) emphasized, noting that nearly a quarter of the black kindergarteners in the ECLS-K outscored the median for white students However, the group differences merit attention.

Here, then, is a puzzle. We are looking for a missing factor (or factors) with the following characteristics:

  • It contributes to the increasing deficit from K-3.
  • It affects individuals at different SES levels.
  • It is not captured by the measures included in data sets like the ECLS.

What is it?

One possibility is: language. There are two elements to consider. One is knowledge of spoken language, which varies across children, like many other skills and capacities. The other is the nature of the linguistic codes to which children are exposed. I will focus on different dialects of English, although similar issues arise regarding exposure to different languages. I will consider each of these elements—language-general and dialect-related—in turn.

There is very little data about children's language in large-scale data sets such as the ECLS, the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development or the Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 79 survey. 11 The ECLS-K survey studied by Fryer and Levitt includes an item about whether English is spoken in the home and a “cognitive assessment” consisting of items taken from a variety of standardized tests, but no direct assessments of the characteristics of a child's language and linguistic environment. Yet there is a substantial body of evidence about the impact of these factors on children's school achievement, particularly reading ( McCardle et al., 2001 ). Children vary considerably with respect to knowledge of components of spoken language, including vocabulary size, morphology, and syntax ( Bates et al., 1995 ). People often refer to differences in verbal “ability,” but the relative richness of children's language is affected by exogenous factors and so this term seems unsuitable. Some refer to spoken language “quality”, but in this context the term evokes the discredited idea that African American English is inherently inferior. Lacking a better term I will refer wherever possible to differences in general knowledge of spoken language. These differences could arise from constitutional, environmental, and socio-cultural factors.

The most widely studied such factor is SES. Differences in linguistic input associated with SES were documented in Risley and Hart's famous study (1995; see also Hansen & Joshi, 2010 ; Hoff, 2013 ). Children will have difficulty learning words, grammatical structures, and discourse conventions to which they are not exposed. Vocabulary in particular is strongly related to progress in learning to read ( NRP, 2000 ). In short, children's success in making the transition into reading depends heavily on their knowledge of spoken language, which varies across individuals and is associated with differences in SES ( Fernald & Marchman, 2011 ). African American children are overrepresented at the lower end of the SES distribution; therefore, they will be disproportionately subject to the effects of low SES on language.

This argument is inconclusive, however. The SES-related factors that Fryer and Levitt identified may be relevant, in part, because of their association with differences in language input, but this cannot be determined because the ECLS-K data set does not include measures of child or caregiver vocabulary (or other aspects of spoken language). The relations between their six factors and the child's knowledge of spoken language are indirect at best. Moreover, these factors accounted for differences at the onset of schooling, but not the growth in the gap through grade 3. Thus, whatever aspects of the linguistic environment they might be picking up are not sufficient to explain the increase. Finally, there is the fact that the achievement gap is not limited to the lower SES cohort. Disentangling the complex relationship between SES and the achievement gap continues to be the focus of research ( Duncan & Magnuson, 2012 ). A cautious reading of the existing literature suggests that there must be other factors involved and that the contributions of language variability need to be assessed more directly.

A further consideration not addressed by studies such as the ECLS is the nature of the linguistic code to which the child is exposed, in particular dialect. Dialects are variants of a language, spoken by individuals grouped by region, ethnicity, race, income, and other factors (Chambers & Trudgill, 1998). Every native speaker of a language learns a dialect of that language. English has many dialects, identifiable at different grain sizes (e.g., American vs. British English; regional dialects in these countries, etc.). In the US, one major division is between so-called “standard” or “mainstream” American English (SAE) and African American English (AAE). As in other countries, which dialect is treated as “standard” is not a linguistic issue but rather is determined by demographic, cultural and political considerations. AAE and SAE overlap—they are both versions of English—but also differ with respect to specific elements of phonology, morphology, lexicon, syntax, and discourse/pragmatics ( Rickford, 1999 ). Although like others I will refer to AAE, it is important to recognize that it has regional variants, as does SAE ( Green, 2002 ; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes, 2006). Moreover, speakers vary in the extent to which they use characteristic features of AAE (a dimension termed dialect density; Thompson et al., 2004 ); thus the extent to which AAE differs from SAE also varies. AAE is used by most African Americans, at varying densities, across SES levels ( Washington, 1996 ). The question then is whether use of AAE contributes to the reading gap.

Research on this question extends back many decades (see Washington et al., in press, for more detailed review). One issue is whether an AAE speaker would be disadvantaged because the dialect is deficient in some manner. This issue was decisively resolved by the basic linguistic research on AAE conducted by Labov (1972) and subsequently many others ( Rickford et al., 2004 ). This research appropriately situated AAE in the context of dialectal variation as it occurs in languages around the world. One of the great achievements of this research was to establish how unexceptional AAE is as an example of linguistic variation (Chambers & Trudgill, 1998). Whether dialect use affects school achievement is not a question about the linguistic status of AAE. The unresolved question is whether use of the minority dialect has an impact because of the conditions surrounding its use. Specifically, AAE usage could affect the child's ability to benefit from educational experiences because of sociological and cultural factors (e.g., AAE is a “low status” dialect; books are written in SAE; acquiring skill in SAE is an educational goal in American schools). It is because of these conditions that differences between the dialects are relevant, not because they are linguistically significant.

Labov's research stimulated considerable research on the possible impact of AAE on the development of African American children's reading skills. Most of the early studies found that dialect usage had little effect on comprehension (Washington et al., in press). Thus it was concluded that what mattered was the quality of linguistic experience, independent of dialect.

These studies were thought to have put the issue of AAE's impact on reading acquisition to rest, but they did so prematurely in my view. The early studies do not hold up well by modern research standards and the strong conclusions that were based on them need to be re-examined. In recent years, researchers have begun to reopen the issue, using what has been learned about reading, language, development, and cognition over the past several decades to generate more specific hypotheses that can be tested using more powerful research methods. The topic is still understudied and many empirical questions are unresolved.

One major question is this: if language variation is a major element in the achievement gap, to what extent does it involve language-general vs. dialect-related aspects of language? Early reading achievement is closely related to knowledge of spoken language. This relationship is general, applying across languages and, within a language, across dialectal variants. Whether young children differ with respect to skills such as phonological awareness, the ability to analyze spoken words at the phonemic level, vocabulary size, and spoken language comprehension should matter, not which variant of a language they happen to speak ( Terry, in press ; Terry & Scarborough, 2010).

It is also possible that there could be dialect-related effects on reading outcomes. Such effects could arise from a variety of factors that emerge when linguistic differences between dialects converge with extralinguistic factors related to the educational and socio-cultural conditions under which dialects are used. The language-general and dialect-related possibilities are not mutually exclusive. They also may not be independent: for example, the acquisition of a language-general skill could be affected by dialect-related factors. General and dialect-related influences may also be difficult to differentiate because a child's general language ability is manifested in the use of a particular dialect. Finally, there may be advantages to exposure to multiple dialects, analogous to those associated with exposure to different languages ( Bialystok et al., 2009 ), although this possibility is rarely considered.

These issues can be illustrated with respect to vocabulary knowledge, a factor that has been the focus of much research and is known to have a major impact on reading acquisition. It is a fact about languages that they consist of inventories of words (among other elements). Vocabulary size, however, is a characteristic of a child—what he/she knows about this component of spoken language—not the dialect that is spoken. This observation suggests that research should focus on the child's knowledge of properties of spoken language, such as vocabulary, irrespective of dialect.

A potential complicating factor is that differences in language background—e.g., use of a minority dialect, varying exposure to and knowledge of the mainstream dialect, dialect usage in the school context—could affect the child's acquisition of “general” elements of spoken language. A factor such as vocabulary size needs to be considered not just as a quantitative predictor of reading outcomes, but also with respect to a theory of how this knowledge is acquired. It could then be determined whether or to what extent specific aspects of dialect experience matter. Is vocabulary acquisition affected by properties of a dialect, or the need to accommodate two dialects? Are such effects positive or negative or both? Do the effects differ depending on the child's point in development? Are they modulated by individual differences in cognitive capacities such as executive function? These issues are not well understood. The bilingual literature suggests they are worth addressing, however. Many studies have shown that preschool-aged bilingual children have smaller vocabularies in each language than comparable monolingual speakers ( Bialystok et al., 2005 ), which then has an impact on learning to read in one of the languages. These effects also arise from conditions relevant to children's learning rather than properties of the languages. They also occur across SES levels. The bilingual burden is by no means insuperable, but the developmental time course may be affected, creating another “gap”, but also the emergence of bilingual advantages ( Bialystok et al., 2009 ). Analogous issues may arise for AAE speaking children who are learning to read in SAE.

As an illustration, consider the optional deletion of final consonants in pronouncing some words in AAE. One consequence is that a given word (such as COLD) can be pronounced differently, at the phonemic level, in the two dialects. Deletion of final consonants also creates additional homophony: words such as COLD and COAL have different pronunciations (at the phonemic level) in SAE but they can be homophonic in AAE. Do these differences between the dialects have any impact on language learning or reading? Or are the differences inconsequential? I don't think we know. The existence of alternative pronunciations across dialects could create a more complex word learning problem, or it might be no more difficult than assimilating differences in pronunciation arising from pitch, speech rate, and so on. An increase in the number of homophones might facilitate vocabulary acquisition (fewer distinct phonological word forms to learn) or make it harder (because the child has to use other mechanisms to disambiguate homophones). Or the functional impact could be trivial. These unanswered questions suggest that it would be premature to treat vocabulary size as a general linguistic factor independent of dialect experience.

Given the limited evidence that is available, I believe that it would be a recapitulation of an earlier mistake to conclude at this point that dialect experience has no significant impact on reading or other aspects of school achievement. As Snow et al. (1991) noted, differences between home and school language could affect children's learning. For speakers of the mainstream dialect, the home and school dialects are the same. For speakers of the minority dialect AAE, the home and school dialects differ in varying degrees. Thus, dialect use is consistent across contexts for one group but not the other. AAE speakers have to learn about the mainstream dialect and use both dialects at the same time they are learning to read, write, and do arithmetic. SAE speakers do not have the additional language-related demands. Speakers of the minority dialect clearly have to do more work in order to succeed. They are nonetheless assessed against the same achievement milestones as SAE speakers. If this analysis is correct, it means that the achievement gap has been intractable because it is built in, guaranteed by prevailing circumstances unrelated to the linguistic validity of the dialect or the capacities of the child. 12 Stated another way, if by analogy to the Fryer and Levitt analysis the overhead associated with accommodating dialect differences were somehow factored out of the natural experiment, the gap in the first years of schooling would greatly narrow rather than grow.

Differences between dialects could potentially affect children's ability to benefit from classroom experience in numerous ways. For example, an SAE-speaking teacher will pronounce many words differently from the child and use different morphological and syntactic constructions. The additional processing and attentional demands associated with comprehending utterances in the less familiar dialect and switching between dialects could be expected to interfere with the child's opportunity to learn from what is being said. The impact would be exacerbated by the fact that the classroom context is a noisy one (literally, and in the information theoretic sense), degrading the quality of linguistic signals. The effect would be similar to imposing a delay on the on-line processing of spoken utterances. These demands could at the same time promote the development of other capacities such as executive function. There are opportunities for communication failures from the opposite side as well: a teacher who speaks SAE and does not know AAE may have more difficulty understanding the AAE speaking child. The teacher may also use discourse conventions (e.g., indirect speech acts) with which the child is unfamiliar, or misinterpret the child's own discourse conventions.

Then there are specific ways in which dialect differences could affect learning to read. The child speaks one dialect but books and other materials are written in the other dialect, again imposing additional learning and processing demands compared to the child for whom the same dialect is used for both. A useful comparison is to hearing-impaired children who are fluent, native signers of ASL, who have high verbal skills, with large vocabularies and language that exhibits a high degree of syntactic variety and complexity—who are nonetheless poor readers. There is an “achievement gap” in reading for the hearing-impaired, due in part to the fact that they are learning to read in English, a different language. There are important differences between these two situations (e.g., the ASL-English differences are greater than the differences between English dialects; the use of different dialects is not associated with presence or absence of a perceptual deficit such as hearing loss), but the analogy is apposite.

And what about acquiring basic reading skills? The beginning reader's initial challenge is to learn how the spoken language they know relates to the written code they are learning. Making this connection is difficult for many children, for reasons that have been investigated in great detail ( NRP, 2000 ). Reading an alphabet involves learning to treat spoken words as if they consist of discrete phonemes. Units in the written code (letters and digraphs) can then be mapped onto units in the spoken code (phonemes). Making this abstraction is difficult for many children. The task is further complicated by inconsistencies in the mappings between spelling and sound in English. The task becomes even more complex when a substantial number of words are pronounced differently at the phonemic level in the two dialects. Consider just the subset of words in AAE in which the final consonant can be dropped (e.g., GOLD→/goυl /, BEST→/bes/). A teacher explains that the word “gold” is spelled G-O-L-D. For an SAE speaking child this is a lesson about the alphabetic principle and the correspondences between four letters and four phonemes. What is being taught an AAE speaking child who pronounces the word /goυl/? That there are different ways to pronounce the word? That if the spelling maps onto one pronunciation, the final letter is pronounced /d/, whereas for the other it is silent? The alternative pronunciations create additional inconsistencies in the mappings between spelling and sound. The learning problem is further complicated by the fact that this deletion is not obligatory, and thus may be used for a given word only some of the time, or for only some words in a similarly spelled neighborhood (e.g., -OLD words).

In short, the need to accommodate both dialects may place a variety of additional burdens on young learners. The potential for dialect-related factors to affect learning—and the need to determine where differences between the dialects do and do not have a significant impact, positive or negative—does not invalidate the relevance of dialect-independent variability in spoken language skills. Plainly, both could exert influence, creating in the worst case a debilitating double-whammy. However, the extent to which such effects occur and how much impact they have are not well understood.

What is striking about dialect is how poorly it is addressed in America compared to other countries. Dialect variation is not specific to African Americans or English, but dialect differences seem to have greater prominence in this country than elsewhere. There are major dialect variants in countries such as Finland and Germany where literacy levels are higher than in the US. Although each situation is different in detail, the main challenges are the same. It appears that these countries do a better job of acknowledging and accommodating dialect differences. Are educators more familiar with dialect issues and their potential impact? Does teacher training include dialect-related issues? Is there less dialect-related prejudice in these countries? Is there greater exposure to alternative dialects prior to the onset of schooling? We know that what works in one country cannot simply be ported to another where relevant circumstances are different. Nonetheless, there is information to be gained from examining how such issues play out in other countries and languages. This effort might suggest ways of changing the culturally-determined conditions that contribute to the achievement gap.

Looking to the future, research on the achievement gap could take a page from research on reading and language disorders. Research on dyslexia, for example, has shifted from a focus on single causes (e.g., a visual or phonological deficit) to the view that outcomes arise from the aggregate effects of a set of risk factors ( Snowling & Hulme, 2011 ; Morris et al., 2010). Each factor is probabilistic in the sense that it does not itself guarantee a particular outcome. The factors also vary in degree of severity and relative impact. Together these factors yield a range of behavioral outcomes. The crucial linkage between the risk factors and outcomes is provided by theories of reading that specify major subskills and how they are learned. By analogy, the poor reading outcomes seen in the “achievement gap” arise from a variety of risk factors that are also probabilistic, vary in severity and impact, interact in complex ways, are differentially amenable to intervention, and yield a broad range of individual outcomes. Risk factors relevant to African American children in American schools include knowledge of spoken language (“general” language skills), language experience (e.g., dialect usage, exposure to and knowledge of the alternative dialect, the cognitive demands of dialect switching), adequacy of educational responses to language variation, and poverty, among others. There is a further need to link these risk factors to specific components of reading and how they are acquired.

Conclusions

Reading failures arise from multiple causes. My goal has been to suggest that this serious societal issue can further benefit from the kinds of research that we conduct as scientists who study reading and language. Reading is often viewed as secondary to spoken language and of very little linguistic interest. Reading did not evolve in the species, it is true, but once the technology became available to many people, it became as central an expression of our capacity for language as speech, greatly changing the ways that language can be used. There are scientists who study reading qua reading, and we have made considerable progress in understanding this skill. But questions about how reading works and the determinants of reading skill invariably come back to issues about spoken language. I've tried to suggest that our basic research is highly relevant to a societal problem of enormous importance. The challenges are daunting, and the need is great.

Acknowledgments

I am greatly indebted to Julie Washington, who stimulated my interest in dialect issues and the achievement gap, took me to school, and gave me the opportunity to become involved in research on the topic. She should not be held responsible for the contents of the article and especially any mistakes, but she has greatly influenced my thinking. I also thank Katherine Magnuson for guidance in navigating the socio-economic literature; she also provided very helpful feedback on a previous version, as did Heidi Feldman, Richard Aslin, and Maryellen MacDonald. I am grateful to Jon Willits for collecting the data in Figure 1 . Finally I thank the journal editor, Cindy Fisher, for her patience in dealing with a manuscript that was always almost finished. Preparation of this article was supported by NICHD grant R24D075454 (Julie Washington, Nicole Patton Terry, and Mark Seidenberg, PIs).

1 See the PISA summary document available here: http://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisaproducts/pisa2009/pisa2009keyfindings.htm .

2 See, for example, the summary report for the 2011 reading assessment, available here: http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pubs/main2011/2012457.asp

3 Basic facts about Finnish elementary education are available here: http://www.oecd.org/pisa/pisaproducts/46581035.pdf .

4 Is Hebrew the outlier writing system? Seemingly contrary to my analysis, it is morphologically complex but also orthographically deep in the default, unpointed form. Note, however that children learn to read using the shallow form in which vowels are indicated by diacritics ( niqqud ). Learning to read using the unpointed form would be vastly more difficult (though perhaps it was achieved by the ancient scribal elite prior to the development of the diacritic system).

5 The Mann quote (which I first encountered in Adams, 1990 ) is from an 1844 report he prepared as Secretary (head) of the Massachusetts Board of Education in which he was highly critical of the local schools, comparing them unfavorably to the classrooms he had observed in Prussia and Scotland (shades our modern-day envy of educational practices in Finland and Shanghai!). Greatly offended, schoolmasters from the Boston public schools published a rejoinder in which they remarked that “Our dissent from [Mann's] views arises from an honest conviction that, if adopted, they would retard the progress of sound learning.” Mann was advocating what later became known as the whole-word or “look and say” method, which involves memorizing words as patterns, without regard to the functions of the component letters. The Boston educators favored a “phonetic” teaching method. Their take-down of Mann's “new method” was thorough and incisive but settled nothing. The arguments on both sides will be easily recognizable to anyone familiar with the “Reading Wars” of the past 30 years. All the documents (the sides went back and forth a few times) are available as Ebooks on Google Play and highly recommended.

6 See Allington and Woodside-Jiron (1999) , who believe that many of the research findings that contradict their own views were the product of research funded by Reid Lyon, an official at NICHD, as part of an anti-education political agenda. The founding document for this political movement, they claim, is Grossen (1997) , an obscure 22 page review of 30 years of reading research funded by NICHD. Allington and Woodside-Jiron's paranoia is so keenly focused on NICHD that they ignore the mass of similar findings from research conducted in many other countries. The same conclusions about learning to read are found in both American reports such as the NRP (2000) and the British Rose Report ( Rose, 2006 ). It would be easier to dismiss Allington's campaign against reading science (see also Allington, 2002 ) were he not a leading figure in reading education, former president of the International Reading Association, former president of the National Reading Conference, and a member of the “Reading Hall of Fame”, http://www.readinghalloffame.org ).

7 In the current climate, everyone has to favor a “balanced” approach to reading instruction, acknowledging the importance of both skills and literacy. Having seen and comprehended the writing on the wall, organizations that had gone to the mat in support of “literacy” approaches such as Whole Language have turned out guidelines for “balanced literacy” instruction (see, e.g., Cowen, 2003 , for an example, and Moats, 2007 , for a critique of such efforts).

8 Basic data on achievement gaps in reading, as measured on the National Assessment of Academic Progress (NAEP), can be found in the Executive Summary of the 2011 results, pp. 15 and 44 ( http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pubs/main2011/2012457.asp ).

9 As exemplified by then-Harvard President Lawrence Summers' conjectures about possible sex-linked genetic differences in mathematical aptitude ( http://www.harvard.edu/president/speeches/summers_2005/nber.php ). I believe that an article in a journal such as this one provides an appropriate context.

10 These data give the lie to a cherished belief. Then: “Education then, beyond all other devices of human origin, is a great equalizer of the conditions of men — the balance wheel of the social machinery.” Horace Mann, 1848 . And now: “In America, education is still the great equalizer.” Arne Duncan, U.S. Secretary of Education, 2011 ( http://www.ed.gov/news/speeches/remarks-us-secretary-education-arne-duncan-he-education-trust-conference ). We should also be considering whether education, as it occurs in American schools and as it is funded, exacerbates differences between groups.

11 NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, http://www.nichd.nih.gov/research/supported/Pages/seccyd.aspx ; Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 79 survey, http://www.bls.gov/nls/nlsy79.htm .

12 I owe this observation to Julie Washington, who made the point with great clarity and impact. The finish line may be in the same location, but paths to getting there are not of equal length or difficulty.

  • Adams M. Beginning to read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allington RL. Big brother and the national reading curriculum: how ideology trumped evidence. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Allington RL, Woodside-Jiron H. The politics of literacy teaching: How “research” shaped educational policy. Educational Researcher. 1999; 28 :4–13. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aro M, Wimmer H. Learning to read: English compared to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2003; 24 :621–635. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Association Of Masters Of The Boston Public Schools. Remarks on the seventh annual report of the Hon Horace Mann, secretary of the Massachusetts Board of Education. Boston, MA: Charles C. Little and James Brown; 1844. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Au KH. Social constructivism and the school literacy learning of students of diverse backgrounds. Journal of Literacy Research. 1998; 30 :297–319. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barton PE, Coley RJ. Parsing the achievement gap II. Policy Information Center, Educational Testing Service 2009 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bates E, Dale PS, Thal D. Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In: Fletcher P, MacWhinney B, editors. Handbook of child language. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell; 1995. pp. 96–151. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bialystok E, Craik FI, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2009; 10 :89–129. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bialystok E, Luk G, Kwan E. Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific studies of reading. 2005; 9 :43–61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Boring EG. A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin. 1953; 50 :169–189. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Charity AH, Scarborough HS, Griffin DM. Familiarity with school English in African American children and its relation to early reading achievement. Child Development. 2004; 75 :1340–1356. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coles G. Misreading Reading: The Bad Science That Hurts Children. Portsmouth, NJ: Heinemann; 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cowen JE. A Balanced Approach to Beginning Reading Instruction: A Synthesis of Six Major U S Research Studies. International Reading Association; 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cox D, Meyers E, Sinha P. Contextually evoked object-specific responses in human visual cortex. Science. 2004; 304 :115–117. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cremin LA. American education: the metropolitan experience, 1876-1980. Harper Collins; 1988. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daniels PT, Bright W. The world's writing systems. Oxford University Press; 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duke NK, Martin NM. 10 things every literacy educator should know about research. The Reading Teacher. 2011; 65 :9–22. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duncan GJ, Magnuson KA. Can family socioeconomic resources account for racial and ethnic test score gaps? The future of children. 2005; 15 :35–54. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2012; 3 :377–386. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durgunoğlu AY. How the language's characteristics influence Turkish literacy development. In: Joshi M, Aaron PG, editors. Handbook of orthography and literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. pp. 219–230. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Elliott JG, Gibbs S. Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Philosophy of Education. 2008; 42 :475–491. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Equity and Excellence Commission. For each and every child—A strategy for equity and excellence. 2013 http://www.foreachandeverychild.org/60565_EEC_(508)_rev.pdf .
  • Fernald A, Marchman VA. Experience, Variation, and Generalization: Learning a First Language (Trends in Language Acquisition Research) Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 2011. Causes and consequences of variability in early language learning; pp. 181–202. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Finn CE. (2010). A Sputnik moment for U.S. Education. Wall St Journal. 2010 Dec 8; [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fryer RG, Levitt SD. Understanding the black-white test score gap in the first two years of school. Review of Economics and Statistics. 2004; 86 :447–464. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fryer RG, Levitt SD. The black-white test score gap through third grade. American Law and Economics Review. 2006; 8 :249–281. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gabrieli JDE. Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science. 2009; 325 :280–283. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gee JP. Thinking, learning, and reading: The situated sociocultural mind. In: Kirshner DI, Whitson JA, editors. Situated cognition: Social, semiotic, and psychological perspectives. 1997. pp. 235–259. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gibson E, Levin H. The psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1978. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldin-Meadow S, Mayberry R. How Do Profoundly Deaf Children Learn to Read? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2001; 16 :222–229. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gosa TL, Alexander KL. Family Disadvantage and the Educational Prospects of Better Off African American Youth. Teachers College Record. 2007; 109 (2):285–321. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gough PB, Hillinger ML. Learning to read: an unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society. 1980; 30 :179–196. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Government Accounting Office. Poverty in America. 2007 Report GAO-07-344. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Green LJ. African American English: a linguistic introduction. Cambridge University Press; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grossen B. 30 years of research: What we now know about how children learn to read. 1997 http://www.eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED415492.pdf .
  • Hanley JR, Masterson J, Spencer L, Evans D. How long do the advantages of learning to read a transparent orthography last? An investigation of the reading skills and reading impairment of Welsh children at 10 years of age. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2004; 57 :1393–1410. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hansen K, Jones E, Joshi H, Budge D. (2010) Millennium Cohort Study Fourth Survey: A User's Guide to Initial Findings. 2nd. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies; Dec, 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hanushek EA, Rivkin SG. Teacher Quality. In: Hanushek E, Welch F, editors. Handbook of the Economics of Education. Elsevier; 2006. pp. 1051–1078. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Company; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Haskell TR, Thornton R, MacDonald MC. Experience and grammatical agreement: Statistical learning shapes number agreement production. Cognition. 2010; 114 :151. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoff E. Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 :4–14. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 1990; 2 :127–160. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoxhallari L, van Daal VHP, Ellis NC. Learning to read words in Albanian: A skill easily acquired. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2004; 8 :153–66. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hulme C, Snowling M. Children's Reading Comprehension Difficulties: Nature, Causes, and Treatments. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011; 20 :139–142. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jencks C, Phillips M. America's next achievement test: Closing the black-white test score gap. American Prospect. 1998; 9 :44–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jorm AF, Share DL. Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1983; 4 :103–147. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux; 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Katz L, Frost R. The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In: Frost R, Katz L, editors. Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning. Amsterdam: Elsevier North Holland Press; 1992. pp. 67–84. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krashen S. More smoke and mirrors: A critique of the National Reading Panel report on fluency. The Phi Delta Kappan. 2001; 83 :119–123. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lonigan CJ, Shanahan T. Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel Executive Summary A Scientific Synthesis of Early Literacy Development and Implications for Intervention. National Institute for Literacy; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lesgold A, Welch-Ross M, editors. Improving adult literacy: Options for practice and research. National Research Council; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liberman IY, Shankweiler D, Liberman AM, Fowler C, Fischer FW. Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In: Reber AS, Scarborough DL, editors. Toward a psychology of reading: the proceedings of the CUNY conference. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977. pp. 207–225. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lindgren SD, deRenzi E, Richman LC. Cross-national comparisons of developmental dyslexia in Italy and the United States. Child Development. 1985; 56 :1404–1417. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Magnuson KA, Duncan GJ. The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children. Developmental Review. 2006; 26 :365–399. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mann H. Seventh annual report. Boston: Dulton & Wentworth; 1844. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mann H. Twelfth annual report The republic and the school: Horace Mann on the education of free men. 1848:79–112. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McCardle P, Scarborough HS, Catts HW. Predicting, explaining, and preventing children's reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2001; 16 :230–239. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mirković J, MacDonald MC, Seidenberg MS. Where does gender come from? Evidence from a complex inflectional system. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2004; 20 :139–168. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mirković J, Seidenberg MS, Joanisse MF. Rules vs. statistics: Insights from a highly inflected language. Cognitive Science. 2011; 35 :638–681. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moats L. Whole language hijinks. Thomas B. Fordham Institute; 2007. http://www.edexcellence.net/publications/wholelanguage.html . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moje EB, Luke A. Literacy and identity: Examining the metaphors in history and contemporary research. Reading Research Quarterly. 2009; 44 (4):415–437. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Morris RD, Lovett MW, Wolf M, Sevcik RA, Steinbach KA, Frijters JC, Shapiro MB. Multiple-component remediation for developmental reading disabilities: IQ, socioeconomic status, and race as factors in remedial outcome. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2012; 45 :99–127. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nation K, Cocksey J. The relationship between knowing a word and reading it aloud in children's word reading development. J Experimental Child Psychology. 2009; 103 :296–308. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • National Assessment of Educational Progress. Reading 2011, Executive Summary. 2011 http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pubs/main2011/2012457.asp .
  • National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read. Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; 2000. http://www.nationalreadingpanel.org . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pattamadilok C, Knierim IN, Duncan KJK, Devlin JT. How does learning to read affect speech perception? The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010; 30 :8435–8444. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pugh KR, Landi Nicole, Preston Jonathan L, Mencl W Einar, Austin Alison C, Sibley Daragh, Fulbright Robert K, Seidenberg Mark S, Grigorenko Elena L, Constable R Todd, Molfese Peter, Frost Stephen J. The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain & Language. 2012 Available online 7 May 2012. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006; 101 :385–413. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rasinski T, Yildirim K, Nageldinger J. Building fluency through the phrased text lesson. The Reading Teacher. 2011; 65 :252–255. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ravitch D. Left behind: A century of battles over school reform. NY: Simon and Schuster; 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ravitch D. The death and life of the great American school system: How testing and choice are undermining education. Basic Books; 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reardon S. The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the poor: New evidence and possible explanations. In: Duncan GJ, Murnane RJ, editors. Whither Opportunity? Rising Inequality, Schools, and Children's Life Chances. Russell Sage Foundation; 2011. pp. 91–116. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richardson Elaine. African American literacies. In: Street B, Hornberger NH, editors. Encyclopedia of Language and Education. 2nd. Vol. 2. Springer; 2008. pp. 335–346. Literacy. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rickford JR. African American vernacular English: Features, evolution, educational implications. Oxford, UK: Blackwell; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rickford JR, Sweetland J, Rickford AE. African American English and other Vernaculars in Education: A Topic-Coded Bibliography. Journal of English Linguistics. 2004; 32 :230–320. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rose J. Independent review of the teaching of early reading: Final report. London: Department for Education and Skills; 2006. Available at www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/phonics/rosereview . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scribner S, Cole M. The psychology of literacy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1981. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seidenberg MS. Language acquisition and use: Learing and applying probabilistic constraints. Science. 1997; 275 :1599–1604. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seidenberg MS. Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In: McCardle P, Ren J, Tzeng O, editors. Dyslexia Across languages: Orthography and the Gene-Brain-Behavior Link. Paul Brooke Publishing; 2011. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seidenberg MS. What happened to reading. New York: Basic Books; forthcoming. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seidenberg MS, McClelland JL. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review. 1989; 96 :523–568. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith Frank. Understanding reading: A psycholinguistic analysis of reading and learning to read. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; 1971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snow C. Reading for Understanding: Toward an R & D Program in Reading Comprehension. RAND Corporation report; 2002. http://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/monograph_reports/2005/MR1465.pdf . [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffin P, editors. Preventing reading difficulties in young children. National Academies Press; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snow C, Baines W, Chandler J, Goodman I, Hemphill L. Unfulfilled expectations: Home and school influences on literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Snowling M, Hulme C. The nature and classification of reading disorders: a commentary on proposals for DSM-5. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011 pre-published on-line. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stanovich KE. Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly. 1980; 16 :32–71. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Terry NP. Dialect variation and phonological knowledge: Phonological representations and metalinguistic awareness among beginning readers who speak nonmainstream American English. Applied Psycholinguistics in press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Terry NP, Scarborough HS. The phonological hypothesis as a valuable framework for studying the relation of dialect variation to early reading skills. In: Brady S, Braze D, Fowler C, editors. Explaining Individual Differences in Reading: Theory and Evidence. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group; 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson CA. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Michigan; 2003. The oral vocabulary abilities of skilled and unskilled African American readers. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson CA, Craig HK, Washington JA. Variable production of African American English across oracy and literacy contexts. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2004; 35 :269. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tobias S, Duffy TM, editors. Constructivist Theory Applied to Instruction: Success or Failure? New York: Routledge, Taylor and Francis; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trudgill P, Chambers JK. Dialects of English: Studies in grammatical variation. London & New York: Longman; 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vanneman A, Hamilton L, Anderson JB, Rahman T. Statistical Analysis Report NCES 2009-455. National Center for Education Statistics; 2009. Achievement Gaps: How Black and White Students in Public Schools Perform in Mathematics and Reading on the National Assessment of Educational Progress. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Washington J. Issues in assessing the language abilities of African-American children. In: Kahmi A, Pollack K, Harris J, editors. Communication development and disorders in African American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Willis J. Engaging the whole child: The neuroscience of joyful education. educational leadership. 2007 On-line only: http://www.ascd.org/publications/educational-leadership/summer07/vol64/num09/The-Neuroscience-of-Joyful-Education.aspx .
  • Wolfram W, Schilling-Estes N. American English: dialects and variation. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]

https://www.high-endrolex.com/29

IMSE Journal

What is the Science of Reading?

  • #Expert Insight

Our Commitment to Evidence-Based Reading Instruction: Aligning Instruction with the Science of Reading Using Structured Literacy

Being a proficient reader has an impact on a student’s entire education. Teaching students to read is the work of educators. While some educators are well-equipped to take on such a significant task, others wish they were better prepared and supported to make an impact that could change a child’s life. IMSE makes it easy for teachers to access and understand the core principles of Structured Literacy and the Science of Reading (SoR) through their professional development trainings and programs. IMSE programs are delivered by experienced teachers, all with a master’s degree, who know how to ensure that teachers feel knowledgeable and supported in their ability to teach literacy.

Notably, over the past years, the Science of Reading has compiled information that holds important implications for empowering teachers with the knowledge to help students succeed in reading. In addition, intervention research estimates that the use of evidence-based prevention and intervention approaches in literacy, such as what IMSE offers, could decrease the number of below-level readers from 30% to 5%.

The Science of Reading: What it is

The Science of Reading is a comprehensive body of research that encompasses years of scientific knowledge, spans across many languages, and shares the contributions of experts from relevant disciplines such as education, special education, literacy, psychology, neurology, and more. The Science of Reading has evolved from a wide span of research designs, experimental methods, participants, and statistical analyses. This conclusive, empirically supported research provides us with the information we need to gain a deeper understanding of how we learn to read, what skills are involved, how they work together, and which parts of the brain are responsible for reading development. From this research, we can identify an evidence-based best practice approach for teaching foundational literacy skills called Structured Literacy. 

David Kilpatrick stated, “We teach reading in different ways; they [students] learn to read proficiently in only one way.” The Science of Reading (SoR) has demystified any wonder of how we learn to read and offers evidence backed by science to confirm that there is one right way to teach reading. In addition to this, the SoR provides vital information to drive IMSE’s approach to instruction and to reach all learners.

The Science of Reading: What we know

  • The SoR helps us to understand the cognitive processes that are essential for reading proficiency. It describes the development of reading skills for both typical and atypical readers.
  • The SoR has debunked various methods used over the years to teach reading that were not based on scientific evidence. 
  • Most reading difficulties can be prevented in young, at-risk students. In other grades, studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of intensive phonemic awareness training, intensive phonic decoding training, and opportunities for repeated practice with reading controlled text. Intervention in these skills leads to efficient orthographic mapping and the highest degree of success.
  • 350 three-sound words
  • 4,320 four-sound words
  • 21,650 five-sound words
  • Letters and sounds: Letter-sound knowledge is essential for both phonic decoding and sight-word learning. 
  • Phonic decoding: Early phonological awareness skills enable the development of letter-sound knowledge and should be targeted for direct instruction through first grade. Advanced phonological awareness skills should continue to be assessed and practiced through third grade to ensure that a solid orthographic lexicon is established. 
  • Orthographic mapping: Understanding orthographic mapping allows for teachers to support students who struggle to read. Orthographic mapping is the process that occurs when unfamiliar words become automatic sight words. The research on orthographic mapping explains how students develop this vast sight word bank for accurate and automatic word retrieval and also why students with reading problems struggle to develop this skill. 
  • Phonics and phonemic manipulation must be proficient to allow for students to build a sight word bank or orthographic lexicon. To support this, students need sufficient practice and review in decoding and encoding, knowledge and application of concept skills, and exposure to decodable text. 
  • Comprehension is the ultimate goal for reading. It is driven by two broad skill sets that are identified in the Simple View of Reading (SVoR).

Simple View of Reading

  • The Simple View of Reading (SVoR) is further detailed in Scarborough’s Reading Rope, which highlights the essential components of reading.

Skilled Reading Language Comprehension Word Recognition

  • Phonics is an important component in early, effective literacy-based instruction.
  • Learning to spell is far more complex than just memorizing words. Encoding (spelling) is a developmental process that impacts fluency, writing, pronunciation, and vocabulary. 
  • Most teachers have received little knowledge about language structures that are used in reading, speaking, and writing. The SoR has compounded information but has yet to make it into the professional development of all teachers.
  • Students with reading difficulties present on a continuum of severity and require highly skilled teachers who have the knowledge and expertise to provide intervention based on the SoR.
  • The findings of the SoR translate into practices called Structured Literacy™. Structured Literacy is an approach to teach reading that is based on the Science of Reading.

Structured Literacy:  How we teach

  • Through Structured Literacy (SL), teachers implement methods that are appropriate for all students and particularly necessary for students with learning differences.
  • Orton-Gillingham is an evidence-based SL approach that uses research from the SoR and incorporates recommended multi-sensory instructional techniques. SL supports instruction that is explicit, sequential, systematic, prescriptive, diagnostic, and cumulative.
  • Instruction is assessment-driven. The diagnostic aspect of SL requires continued progress monitoring to measure outcomes and guide differentiation. 
  • Students are provided repeated opportunities with decodable text that have ample representations of the phonetic elements for code-emphasis.
  • Through regular dictation of words and sentences containing the phonetic concept, students become skilled in spelling words within and outside of the text.

Structured Literacy: What we teach

  • Phonemic Awareness
  • Comprehension
  • Students who have difficulty decoding need a focus on phoneme-grapheme and blending automaticity for both real and nonsense words. Teachers are skilled at differentiating instruction based on assessment results.
  • Orthography helps students to understand why words are spelled the way they are spelled. Students learn to identify the overlapping features of words including word origin, phoneme-grapheme correspondence, position constraints, and patterns and conventions. This helps them to acquire the alphabetic principle.
  • Phonemic awareness is emphasized as a necessary pre-reading skill and teachers recognize and target the sequence of skills to build phonological awareness from early to advanced skill levels. 
  • Regular words are taught according to phonetic patterns and irregular words are analyzed for their irregularities. When proficient readers encounter new words, they phonemically analyze the word for the regular grapheme-phoneme patterns and are able to identify the irregular element(s) with ease. Teaching weak readers to activate this process allows them to align the letters to the phonemes in their memory. 
  • Morphology is the study of meaningful units within words. Students are able to expand their vocabulary when they are directly exposed to the study of root words, prefixes, and suffixes.

The Science of Reading has proven that a Structured Literacy approach is a necessary foundation for reading success. As teachers, we make a commitment to continue our education to support the learning of every student. IMSE’s Orton-Gillingham training and programs make it possible for teachers to integrate evidence-based and research-based reading instruction and intervention strategies into Multi-Tiered Systems of Support (MTSS) to empower all students in every classroom. 

Sign up for our LIVE virtual Orton-Gillingham training! We are now offering half-day, evening, and weekend options to best fit your schedule. 

To learn more about the Science of Reading and Structured Literacy, check out the following great reads:

  • Essentials of Assessing, Preventing, and Overcoming Reading Difficulties by David Kilpatrick
  • Equipped for Reading Success by David Kilpatrick
  • Reading in the Brain: The New Science of How We Read by Stanislas Dehaene
  • Phonics from A to Z: A Practical Guide by Wiley Blevins
  • Unlocking Literacy: Effective Decoding and Spelling Instruction by Marcia Henry
  • Reader, Come Home: The Reading Brain in a Digital World by Maryanne Wolf

IMSE’s approach allows teachers to incorporate the five components essential to an effective reading program into their daily lessons: phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension. 

The approach is based on the Orton-Gillingham methodology and focuses on explicit, direct instruction that is sequential, structured, and multi-sensory.

It is IMSE’s mission that all children must have the ability to read to fully realize their potential. We are committed to providing teachers with the knowledge and tools to deliver proven literacy education to students of all ages and abilities.

About The Author

science of reading case study

Please connect with us on  Facebook ,  Twitter , and  Pinterest  to get tips and tricks from your peers and us. Read the  IMSE Journal  to hear success stories from other schools and districts, and be sure to read the OG Weekly email series for refreshers and tips.

Accessibility Report

[Enter personal and organization information through the Preferences > Identity dialog.]

The checker found no problems in this document.

  • Needs manual check: 0
  • Passed manually: 2
  • Failed manually: 0

Detailed Report

Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedAccessibility permission flag must be set
PassedDocument is not image-only PDF
PassedDocument is tagged PDF
Passed manuallyDocument structure provides a logical reading order
PassedText language is specified
PassedDocument title is showing in title bar
PassedBookmarks are present in large documents
Passed manuallyDocument has appropriate color contrast
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedAll page content is tagged
PassedAll annotations are tagged
PassedTab order is consistent with structure order
PassedReliable character encoding is provided
PassedAll multimedia objects are tagged
PassedPage will not cause screen flicker
PassedNo inaccessible scripts
PassedPage does not require timed responses
PassedNavigation links are not repetitive
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedAll form fields are tagged
PassedAll form fields have description
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedFigures require alternate text
PassedAlternate text that will never be read
PassedAlternate text must be associated with some content
PassedAlternate text should not hide annotation
SkippedOther elements that require alternate text
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedTR must be a child of Table, THead, TBody, or TFoot
PassedTH and TD must be children of TR
PassedTables should have headers
PassedTables must contain the same number of columns in each row and rows in each column
SkippedTables must have a summary
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedLI must be a child of L
PassedLbl and LBody must be children of LI
Rule NameStatusDescription
PassedAppropriate nesting

science of reading case study

  • Member Login
  • JOIN NOW/RENEW
  • LRA All Academic

The Science of Reading and the Media: Does the Media Draw on High-Quality Reading Research?

The science of reading and the media:, does the media draw on high-quality reading research, maren aukerman, university of calgary.

Recent stories about the “science of reading” in the popular press have often distorted how children learn reading. Reporting can be biased, as my last Critical Conversations piece discussed.  But there is another problem, equally grave: reporters telling these stories may have only a partial grasp of reading research. To be fair, developing deep proficiency in a research domain can be elusive and take years of intense study. 1 For example, to report well on astrophysics news, science reporters need more than interest and a grade-school understanding of the scientific method; they require meaningful grounding in astrophysics and its research. The field of reading is no less specialized, involving thousands of scholars using a variety of research methods in robust dialogue with one another. Thus, to report well on developments in reading education, reporters must understand more than just one vocal corner of this research landscape. They should know how the information they communicate connects to a large body of scholarship that frequently includes conflicting findings and scholarly disagreement. 

Error of Insufficient Understanding 1: Weak Connection to Actual Research

The Time and NYT articles both made claims about reading research, but neither appeared substantively grounded in it. Links provided in both articles mostly take the reader not to peer-reviewed research, an expected standard in scholarship, but to other news articles, podcasts, and/or popular (but not always scholarly) books. Both journalists often relied on the word of “science of reading” advocates, failing to verify those statements against research.  For example, The NYT cited a Units of Study critic claiming that the program’s author was “disconnected from research” without clearly situating this as opinion. 

By drawing mostly on vociferous advocates of one approach and bolstering their claims primarily with other journalism, journalists create an echo chamber which itself is disconnected from reading research. Indeed, Time cited no individual research studies, even where those would clearly be relevant. For example, it discussed widespread adoption of LETRS training, a professional development program supposedly aligned with the “science of reading;” yet it never acknowledged a rigorous study indicating that LETRS had no positive effect on student achievement, a critical detail from a research-based perspective. 4

When it is present, coverage of reading research also often fails to pass muster. Both Time and the NYT heavily rely on a research synthesis over twenty years old, the National Reading Panel Report . 5 Indeed, Time laments the ostensible fact that “the National Reading Panel’s recommendations and all the research were so blithely ignored” (again citing a journalist as proof of the claim). The NRP Report is an extensive, important research analysis, but it is far from complete (given research available at that time), up to date (given research conducted since then), or irreproachable (given limitations in methodology and scope). It has been the subject of numerous critiques, including scathing criticism by a participating panel member as well as reanalysis of its findings on phonics that calls those into question. 6   7 8 9 10 It is fair to call it influential, but false to equate the whole of reading research with this document, to dismiss scholars who fail to fully embrace it as ignoring research, or even to assume “science of reading” advocates and the NRP Report are themselves fully aligned.

Troublingly, Time treated the NRP Report and books from the popular press as the whole research story; it never mentioned individual research studies. The NYT did discuss two studies relevant to its topic – one that observed substantial student gains with a treatment sample of 40 schools using Units of Study 11 and one showing no effects from its use at a single school. 12 The article treated the studies as though they were of equal import and thus inconclusive, with no acknowledgment that the first, far larger in scale, was more robust. Such reporting is not research-aligned.

Recommendation: Ensure that “science of reading” news meshes with up-to-date, varied, peer-reviewed research before accepting its claims. 

Error of insufficient understanding 2: inaccurate, distorted use of terminology.

In fact, educators can use it to identify when a child over-relies on guessing. After identifying the primary nature of children’s errors, teachers can guide them into attending to new information so they no longer need to guess. If a child guesses “mother” in place of “mommy,” the child might be asked to slide their finger under the word to sound it out. A different child, who pronounces “said” so it rhymes with “raid,” might be asked if it makes sense in the sentence – not to replace the child’s phonics-based strategy, but to supplement it. This instructional guidance is not usually called “cueing” by those who use it. Rather, it may be called an interactive strategies approach. 14  

Well-designed instruction based on a three-cueing system analysis of student errors emphasizes tailored, progressively more refined strategies so that, for example, a child over-relying on pictures to decode would not be encouraged to keep using pictures. If a teacher or instructional program nonetheless does that, it is inconsistent with the whole basis of the approach. Some researchers argue that a subset of children need more systematic exposure to phonics than may happen with an interactive strategies approach. 15 But even those critics generally do not characterize the approach as “guessing.” That characterization is unhelpful, polarizing, and false.

This is not the only journalistic misuse of literacy terminology. For example, “balanced literacy” is frequently caricatured as involving little phonics, in contrast with what gets called the “science of reading.” Time explained it as offering “instruction in the link between sounds and letters… sprinkled in with other methods teachers thought worked.” The article wrongly suggested that the defining feature of balanced literacy is teaching idiosyncratic, non-research-based ways of decoding outside of phonics . In reality, advocates of balanced literacy see phonics as integral but also recognize that it should not displace attention to other important things like comprehension and language development, even in the early grades – a position firmly rooted in what reading research suggests. 16 17 In fact, some educators ascribing to balanced literacy may exclusively teach decoding via explicit phonics instruction (not interactive strategies).

Recommendation: Be aware that “science of reading” news may contain distorted information and wrong use of terminology: do not take accuracy for granted.

Error of Insufficient Understanding 3: Spurious Claims that One Approach is Settled Science

The NYT article argues that one particular approach to teaching decoding, called systematic phonics, “is the most effective way to teach reading,” and the Time article conveys a similar message. One can certainly make a strong case for such instruction 18 , but the research terrain is complex: some analyses of phonics-intensive approaches, including studies of the much-touted Orton-Gillingham approach and the aforementioned LETRS program , have found no positive effects. 19 21 22 Moreover, little research examines whether systematic phonics ultimately improves comprehension, which is arguably the gold standard; studies that do examine this have failed to find clear benefits. 23 In short, there is insufficient evidence to conclude that any single approach, including the particular systematic phonics approach often elided with “the science of reading,” is most effective. 24 25  

The NYT not only insists that the type of phonics pushed by many “science of reading” advocates is the “most effective;” it also maintains that interactive strategies approaches – wrongly called “three-cueing” –  don’t work. 26 The actual evidence there is also more nuanced: for example, while one study suggests that gains associated with interactive strategies fade over time 27 , another found positive effects even 10 years post treatment. 28 Moreover, journalists often have a double standard, pointing out the current lack of evidence for the long-term efficacy of interactive strategies approaches but staying mum about a corresponding dearth of evidence regarding the longitudinal efficacy of more exclusively phonics-based approaches. 29 30  

Worth noting, too, is that what gets called the “science of reading” is in no way analogous to the science of climate change, where 97% of scientists agree. 31 Nearly 57% of education professors favored balanced literacy in a recent survey, with only 22% favoring systematic phonics. 32  The press should be asking why buy-in by researchers is so low rather than accepting on faith that the minority position is the research-based one; few journalists appear to be doing so.

But beyond that, a genuinely research-based science of reading must be rooted in a scientific process – ways of engaging with the known, posing questions, and reconciling complexities – more than it is about adhering to fixed knowns. Disagreement is vital. This does not mean anything goes: reading researchers examine goals, what can be extrapolated from different measures, how research should be designed, whether research is generalizable to a larger sample of children, and what implications are valid. But it is a misrepresentation of research to treat it as fixed knowledge handed down by irrefutable researchers: this is perhaps what “science of reading” advocates within and outside the press get most wrong. Indeed, someone who denies the role of research-based dialogue is not a scientist at heart, but rather an ideologue.

It could be different. Early literacy researchers investigate many things. For example, there is likely a limit to how much phonics instruction children should receive. Phonics skills instruction alone produces less benefit than splitting time between skills instruction and practice in applying those skills to real texts; 25 or 30 minutes of targeted skills daily may be the sweet spot 33 34 , which is pretty close to the average of what teachers already report doing. 35 Other scholars argue against one-size-fits-all solutions: stronger initial decoders in first grade benefit from less phonics instruction even as peers who begin the year with fewer of such skills benefit from more. 36

And other things similarly remain contested: how to make phonics instruction more engaging and effective 37 ; whether to teach phonics beyond first grade, given diminishing returns 38 ; what kinds of texts should be used with emergent readers 39 ; how much phonics should be “pushed down” into kindergarten when earlier does not mean better in developing reading 40 ; and so on. Researchers do have varied opinions on whether interactive strategies instruction contributes enough to warrant teaching it. 41 42 But that disagreement is not the only or even arguably the most important debate. 

Intelligent, informed people can disagree about these issues. Reading researchers regularly seek to persuade other researchers without dismissing those who see things differently as not listening to science. And adjusting one’s approach as one becomes persuaded of the need, as was done when the Units of Study update incorporated more phonics, is a mark of thoughtful engagement with research, not a “major retreat” (as per the NYT article).  

Recommendation: Be skeptical of “science of reading” news that touts “settled science,” especially if such claims are used to silence disagreement.

Error of insufficient understanding 4: lack of context about previous phonics implementation attempts.

The NYT notes that Units of Study is being dropped in Oakland, apparently because critics believe that its less phonics-intensive approach has not seen enough success. The article does not mention that Oakland previously used a phonics-heavy program, Open Court Reading 43 ,  introduced there in 1998 with similar media fanfare to the current hoopla. 44 The Time article does bring in a historical perspective after a fashion, quoting an Oakland-based “science of reading” advocate who claimed that the previous Open Court implementation there had led to strong reading gains before it was abandoned. But Time failed to discuss actual research on Open Court . In point of fact, there is evidence that Open Court actually had a negative effect on many kids’ reading, particularly as kids aged up and encountered complex, harder-to-understand text. 45   

The idea that phonics can fix children’s reading ills is at least 70 years old, yet results from other large-scale phonics reforms have also yielded disappointing results, including during the Reading First era in the U.S. and as England’s recent national curriculum mandates have played out. 46 47 48   And Canada, which has relied mostly on a holistic, less phonics-intensive approach, has generally had excellent reading scores in international test comparisons. 49 Both historical and international comparisons unravel the narrative that systematic phonics is a do-all fix. 

Recommendation: Consider whether “science of reading” news acknowledges the uninspiring track record of real-world phonics-intensive initiatives.

These four kinds of errors show up in numerous “science of reading” articles in the popular press beyond the two analyzed here. Particularly in combination with media bias errors, they do real harm. In my final contribution to this Critical Conversations series, to be released in the coming weeks, I will discuss some of these consequences. 

Please cite this work: Aukerman, M. (2022) The Science of Reading and the Media: Does the Media Draw on High-Quality Reading Research? Literacy Research Association Critical Conversations . CC BY 4.0 license .

Maren Aukerman is a Werklund Research Professor at the University of Calgary who focuses on literacy education and democratic citizenship. She studies educational ethics, how youth engage with information in the media, and the preparation of students for responsible citizenship and democratic dialogue, particularly in the context of literacy education. Aukerman previously was on the faculty at Stanford and the University of Pennsylvania and is the recipient of a National Academy of Education/Spencer Postdoctoral Fellowship as well as the 2009 Albert J. Harris Award and the 2018 Dina Feitelson Research Award from the International Literacy Research Association. In her current work, she is studying how young people make sense of COVID-19 information that they encounter, with a special emphasis on what they do and do not find trustworthy as information sources.

Photo by  Jessica Ruscello  on  Unsplash

  • Alexander, P. A. (2004). A model of domain learning: Reinterpreting expertise as a multidimensional, multistage process. In D. Y. Dai & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 273–298). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Luscombe, B. ( 2022 ,  August 11 ). Inside the Massive Effort to Change the Way Kids Are Taught to Read. Time Magazine . https://time.com/6205084/phonics-science-of-reading-teachers/
  • Goldstein, D. (2022, May 22). In the fight over how to teach reading, this guru makes a major retreat. The New York Times . https://www.nytimes.com/2022/05/22/us/reading-teaching-curriculum-phonics.html
  • Garet, M. S., Cronen, S., Eaton, M., Kurki, A., Ludwig, M., Jones, W., Uekawa, K., Falk, A., Bloom, H., Doolittle, F., Zhu, P., & Sztenjnberg, L. (2008). The impact of two professional development interventions on early reading instruction and achievement (NCEE 2008-4030). National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pdf/20084030.pdf
  • National Reading Panel. (2000). The report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature . Washington, D.C. https://www.nichd.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/pubs/nrp/Documents/report.pdf
  • Bowers, J. S. (2020). Reconsidering the evidence that systematic phonics is more effective than alternative methods of reading instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 32 (3), 681-705. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09515-y
  • Camilli, G., Wolfe, P. M., & Smith, M. L. (2006). Meta‐analysis and reading policy: Perspectives on teaching children to read. The Elementary School Journal, 107 (1), 27-36. https://doi.org/10.1086/509525
  • Garan, E. M. (2001). Beyond the smoke and mirrors: A critique of the National Reading Panel report on phonics. Phi Delta Kappan, 82 (7), 500-506. https://doi.org/10.1177/003172170108200705
  • Krashen, S. (2001). More smoke and mirrors: A critique of the National Reading Panel report on fluency. Phi Delta Kappan, 83 (2), 119-123. https://doi.org/10.1177/003172170108300208
  • Yatvin, J. (2002). Babes in the woods: The wanderings of the National Reading Panel. The Phi Delta Kappan, 83 (5), 364-369. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20440142
  • American Institutes for Research. (2021). Teachers College Reading and Writing Project study .  https://www.air.org/sites/default/files/Teachers-College-Reading-and-Writing-Project-Jan-2021.pdf
  • Figge, B., & Sass, T. (2021). Impact evaluation of the Lucy Calkins Units of Study program, 20. https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/gpl_reports/20/
  • Scanlon, D. M., & Anderson, K. L. (2020). Using context as an assist in word solving: The contributions of 25 years of research on the interactive strategies approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 55 (S1), S19-S34. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.335
  • Adams, M. J., Fillmore, L. W., Goldenberg, C., Oakhill, J., Paige, D. D., Rasinski, T., & Shanahan, T. (2020). Comparing reading research to program design: An examination of Teachers College Units of Study. Student Achievement Partners , 1-64.
  • Pearson, P. D. (2004). The reading wars. Educational Policy, 18 (1), 216-252. https://doi.org/10.1177/0895904803260041
  • Hoffman, E. B., Paciga, K. A., & Whittingham, C. E. (2021). The problem with pigeons in research and practice: Communicating early literacy essentials and foundations in curriculum and instruction. Literacy , 55 (3), 159-171. https://doi.org/10.1111/lit.12257
  • Ehri, L. C. (2020). The science of learning to read words: A case for systematic phonics instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 55 (S1), S45-S60. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.334
  • Garet, M. S., Cronen, S., Eaton, M., Kurki, A., Ludwig, M., Jones, W., Uekawa, K., Falk, A., Bloom, H., Doolittle, F., Zhu, P., & Sztenjnberg, L. (2008). The impact of two professional development interventions on early reading instruction and achievement (NCEE 2008-4030). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pdf/20084030.pdf [\efn_note]   20 Thompson, G. B., McKay, M. F., Fletcher-Flinn, C. M., Connelly, V., Kaa, R. T., & Ewing, J. (2007). Do children who acquire word reading without explicit phonics employ compensatory learning? Issues of phonological recoding, lexical orthography, and fluency. Reading and Writing, 21 (5), 505-537. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9075-9
  • Wyse, D., & Bradbury, A. (2022). Reading wars or reading reconciliation? A critical examination of robust research evidence, curriculum policy and teachers’ practices for teaching phonics and reading. Review of Education, 10 (1), e3314.  https://doi.org/10.1002/rev3.3314
  • Torgerson, C., Brooks, G., Gascoine, L., & Higgins, S. (2019). Phonics: Reading policy and the evidence of effectiveness from a systematic ‘tertiary’ review. Research Papers in Education, 34 (2), 208-238. https://doi.org/10.1080/02671522.2017.1420816
  • Cook, P., Rodes, D. R., & Lipsitz, K. L. (2017). The reading wars and Reading Recovery: What educators, families, and taxpayers should know. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 22 (2), 12-23. https://doi.org/10.18666/LDMJ-2017-V22-I2-8391
  • Hurry, J., Fridkin, L., & Holliman, A. J. (2022). Reading intervention at age 6: Long-term effects of Reading Recovery in the UK on qualifications and support at age 16. British Educational Research Journal, 48 (1), 5-21. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3752
  • Whittingham, C. E., Hoffman, E. B., & Paciga, K. A. (2021). Assessment, accountability, and access: Constrained skill mastery as instructional gatekeeper. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy . https://doi.org/10.1177/14687984211042056
  • NASA. (n.d.). Scientific consensus: Earth’s climate is warming. Retrieved from: https://climate.nasa.gov/scientific-consensus/
  • Kurtz, H., Lloyd, S., Harwin, A., Chen, V., & Furuya, Y. (2020). Early reading instruction: Results of a national survey . EdWeek Research Center. https://epe.brightspotcdn.com/1b/80/706eba6246599174b0199ac1f3b5/ed-week-reading-instruction-survey-report-final-1.24.20.pdf
  • Taylor, B. M., Pearson, P. D., Clark, K., & Walpole, S. (2000). Effective schools and accomplished teachers: Lessons about primary-grade reading instruction in low-income schools. The Elementary School Journal, 101 (2), 121-165. https://doi.org/10.1086/499662
  • Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., & Katch, L. E. (2004). Beyond the reading wars: Exploring the effect of child-instruction interactions on growth in early reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8 (4), 305-336. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0804_1
  • Palmer, J. L., & Invernizzi, M. (2015). No more phonics and spelling worksheets : Heinemann.
  • Ivey, G., & Baker, M. I. (2004). Phonics instruction for older students? Just say no. Educational Leadership, 61 (6), 35-39. https://www.ascd.org/el/articles/phonics-instruction-for-older-students-just-say-no
  • Mesmer, H. A. E. (2010). Textual scaffolds for developing fluency in beginning readers: Accuracy and reading rate in qualitatively leveled and decodable text. Literacy Research & Instruction, 49 (1), 20-39. https://doi.org/10.1080/19388070802613450
  • Suggate, S. P. (2009). School entry age and reading achievement in the 2006 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA). International Journal of Educational Research, 48 (3), 151-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2009.05.001
  • Shanahan, T. (2020). What constitutes a science of reading instruction? Reading Research Quarterly, 55 (S1), S235-S247. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.349
  • Adams, M. J., Bereiter, C., Carruthers, I., Case, R., Hirshberg, J., McKeough, A., et al. (2000). Open court reading. Columbus, OH: SRA/McGraw-Hill
  • Bowler, M. (1999, February 6). Phonics experiment has sound of success; California: In the second year the Open Court program has been used in some high-poverty schools, youngsters are excited about reading. Baltimore Sun . https://www.baltimoresun.com/news/bs-xpm-1999-02-07-9902060309-story.html
  • Vaden-Kiernan, M., Borman, G., Caverly, S., Bell, N., Sullivan, K., Ruiz de Castilla, V., Hughes Jones, D. (2018). Findings from a multiyear scale-up effectiveness trial of Open Court Reading. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 11 (1), 109-132. https://doi.org/10.1080/19345747.2017.1342886
  • Durán, L., & Hikida, M. (2022). Making sense of reading’s forever wars. Phi Delta Kappan, 103 (8). https://kappanonline.org/readings-forever-wars-duran-hikida/?fbclid=IwAR2nK4U-vhFc2PJicToK2MYYyaDDztJSUg928OcsYa8Tj83gPj6M8qdPJf0
  • Gamse, B. C., Jacob, R.T., Horst, M., Boulay, B., & Unlu, F. (2008). Reading First impact study final report (NCEE 2009-4038) . https://ies.ed.gov/ncee/pdf/20094038.pdf
  • Ian O'Byrne
  • December 7, 2022

Recent Post

Sorry, no posts were found.

Email Newsletter

Purchase: the science of reading and the media: does the media draw on high-quality reading research.

Written by: ROOTEDIN

Price: $50 / Year

Personal information

Student Member – Initial Payment

$50 / Year

$50.00
Total $50.00

Stripe

Pay with your Credit Card via Stripe Checkout

Javascript is disabled in your browser. You will not be able to complete your purchase until you either enable JavaScript in your browser, or switch to a browser that supports it.

Price: $100 / Year

2nd Family Member – Initial Payment

$100 / Year

$100.00
Total $100.00

Price: $115 / Year

Emeritus Member – Initial Payment

$115 / Year

$115.00
Total $115.00

Price: $150 / Year

Professional Member – Initial Payment

$150 / Year

$150.00
Total $150.00

Professional Learning & Support | Literacy, Math, Curriculum

The Science of Reading

CORE has a long-standing history of prioritizing evidence-based practices rooted in the Science of Reading , and we believe it’s imperative to equip educators with instructional practices aligned with the Science of Reading so that all children learn to read. 

science of reading case study

What is the Science of Reading?

The Science of Reading is a vast, interdisciplinary body of scientifically-based research on reading and writing developed by the Defining Movement Coalition .

This research has been conducted worldwide over the last five decades and derives from thousands of studies in multiple languages. The science of reading explains:

  • How proficient reading and writing develop.
  • Why some learners have difficulty with reading.
  • How to assess and improve student outcomes through solutions for students with reading difficulties.
“The Science of Reading is a vast, interdisciplinary body of scientifically-based research on reading and writing.”

How the Brain Processes Written Language

The Science of Reading reveals how reading is processed by the brain:

  • The temporal and occipital lobes are responsible for letter and word recognition and language comprehension.
  • The parietal and temporal lobes process word analysis and sound-symbol connection.
  • The frontal lobe translates written language into speech sounds.

science of reading case study

Scarborough’s Rope

Scarborough’s Rope is a visual metaphor for the development of skills over time (represented by the strands of the rope) that lead to skilled reading.

When a student is confronted with a text, there are two processes that make skilled reading possible. The student must understand what they’re reading and combine that understanding with a broader knowledge of language. The first is called word recognition; the second, language comprehension. Each of these competencies is composed of different “strands” that intertwine to form the rope of skilled reading.

“Scarborough’s Rope is a visual metaphor for the development of the skills that lead to skilled reading.”

science of reading case study

Science of Reading Resources

  • CORE’s Science of Reading Resource Library
  • In this white paper , the Center for the Collaborative Classroom engages with research, offers informed guidance, and explores instructional considerations related to the Science of Reading.
  • The Reading League’s Science of Reading: Defining Guide provides a definition of what the science of reading is, what it is not, and how all stakeholders can understand its potential to transform reading instruction.

Literacy Services

For districts, curriculum adoption.

CORE Learning partners with district leaders on the adoption of high-quality K-12 math and English Language Arts (ELA) instructional materials through a data-driven process grounded in student outcomes.

Curriculum Implementation

CORE Learning builds the capacity of district and school leaders to implement high-quality instructional materials (HQIM) so that educators can ensure equitable outcomes for all students.

For Educators

Online language & literacy academy.

OL&LA (Online Language & Literacy Academy) is an asynchronous online course that gives today’s educators the tools they need to unlock the full potential of every student.

Minnesota + CORE Learning

California + core learning, publisher-approved professional learning.

CORE Learning partners with publishers to provide curriculum-aligned professional learning.

Elementary Reading Academy

CORE’s Elementary Reading Academy offers educators fundamental knowledge in effective standards-aligned and evidence-based reading practices based on the science of reading.

Adolescent Literacy Solutions

Our two-part workshop series equips educators with the knowledge of the science of reading and the instructional skills necessary to accelerate underperforming students.

For Schools

Literacy coaching & site implementation support.

CORE provides coaching and embedded support to help advance literacy and learning goals.

Early Literacy Planning & Implementation Collaboration

Our Early Literacy Planning and Implementation Collaborative (ELPIC) provides school teams with the tools and skills they need to develop powerful plans to improve early literacy.

See Our Work in Action

making sense of fractions

A Commitment to Equity Drives Curriculum Implementation Success

science of reading case study

UnboundEd | CORE Effective Implementation Cohort Grant Years 1 and 2 Initial Report

science of reading case study

Building District and School Systems to Promote Equitable Instruction

science of reading case study

Improving Student Achievement in Math Through Coherent Instructional Systems

science of reading case study

Setting a New Vision for California’s Literacy Instruction

science of reading case study

Our OERA Best Taken as a Team

Get in touch.

Contact CORE to learn more about improving literacy instructional practices using the Science of Reading.

  • Future Students
  • How to Apply
  • Admitted Students
  • Tuition, Costs and Aid
  • Degrees and Programs
  • Contact Admissions
  • Current Students
  • Class Schedule
  • Academic Calendar
  • Academic Resources and Support
  • Student Services and Resources
  • Lifetime Membership
  • Alumni Events
  • Update Your information
  • Awards and Recognitions
  • Give to UHCL
  • Faculty & Staff
  • Faculty Highlights
  • Administrative Offices
  • Course Development
  • Maps and Directions
  • News and Events
  • go.uhcl.edu
  • UHCL at Pearland
  • Mental Health
  • Academic Resources and Offices
  • First Year Seminar
  • Centers and Institutes
  • Extended and Professional Education
  • Commencement
  • Global Learning
  • Military and Veteran Services

Academics

  • Apply for Admission
  • Visit Campus
  • International
  • Non-Degree Seeking
  • Former Students

Admissions

  • Cost of Attendance
  • Apply for Aid
  • Types of Aid
  • Scholarships
  • Forms and Resources
  • Frequently Asked Questions

Costs and Aid

  • Student Affairs
  • Orientation and New Student Programs
  • Health and Wellness
  • Student Involvement and Leadership
  • Student Advocacy and Community
  • Career Services
  • Campus Community
  • Arts and Culture

Campus Life

  • Events Calendar
  • Office of Special Events
  • Emergency Communications

Events at UHCL

  • Administrative Leadership
  • Administrative Offices and Resources
  • 50th Anniversary
  • Facts and Statistics
  • Map and Directions
  • Strategic Partnerships
  • Working at UHCL
  • Accreditations
  • Hispanic Serving Institution (HSI)
  • Impact 2025 and Beyond

About UHCL

  • Career Outcomes
  • Student Success
  • Institutional Success
  • Notable Alumni

Success and Outcomes

  • Request Info
  • Science of Teaching Reading Study Aids

Overview of the STR Exam Requirement

House Bill 3, passed during the 86th Texas Legislature, established the requirement that all teacher candidates who teach students in grades Pre-K-6 demonstrate proficiency in the science of teaching reading on a new, standalone certification exam. This requirement takes effect on January 1, 2021.   

The STR exam will be required for the issuance of five certification fields:

  • Early Childhood: EC – Grade 3
  • Core Subjects with Science of Teaching Reading: Early Childhood–Grade 6
  • Core Subjects with Science of Teaching Reading: Grades 4–8
  • English Language Arts and Reading with Science of Teaching Reading: Grades 4–8​
  • English Language Arts and Reading/Social Studies with Science of Teaching Reading: Grades 4–8​

The STR exam will be required for the issuance of intern, probationary , and standard certifications for these certification fields beginning January 1, 2021.   

Teachers who are currently standard certified in Core Subjects: EC-6, Core Subjects: 4-8, English Language Arts and Reading: 4-8, or English Language Arts and Social Studies: 4-8 will not be required to pass the STR exam for renewal of their certificate. The STR exam requirement only impacts candidates seeking certification after Jan. 1, 2021.   

Content of The STR Exam

The Science of Teaching Reading Exam Content and Design document  provides information on the structure of the exam.

STR Preparation Manual

The Preparation Manual for Science of Teaching Reading (293) is now available on the Texas Educator Certification Examination Program website.  This preparation manual includes:

  • The STR examination framework
  • Approaches to answering different question types
  • Sample selected-response questions
  • In-depth rationales for correct and incorrect responses
  • A sample constructed response question, including the video exhibit
  • Sample strong and weak constructed-responses and rationales
  • A list of preparation resources 

STR Webinar Resources 

  • STR Exam Timelines and Requirements Webinar: A short informational webinar that outlines the STR exam requirement and the timelines associated with the STR exam. This webinar is designed as a tool to support educator preparation programs and teacher candidates understand critical requirements and deadlines ahead of the January 1, 2021 STR exam mandate.  
  • Watch the TEA Science of Teaching Reading Webinar (video).
  • Review the TEA Science of Teaching Reading Webinar Presentation (pdf).
  • To request additional topics for upcoming STR exam related webinars, please complete the following survey: https://bit.ly/STRExamTopics 

Constructed Response Examples

The Literacy, Language Arts, and Literature Studies faculty provide guided examples of how to complete the constructed response question.

  • Review the Constructed Response Example and Practice.

College of Education State Assessments Office

Phone: 281-283-3623 [email protected] Bayou Building 1231-16 2700 Bay Area Blvd, Box 188 Houston, TX 77058-1002

  • DOI: 10.21009/stairs.5.1.7
  • Corpus ID: 271991452

Vocational High School Teachers’ Perception of Project-based Learning Method in English Language Teaching: A Case Study

  • Melvira Tanila , Sri Sulastini , Imas Wahyu Agustina
  • Published in Starting AI Researchers… 16 August 2024

Related Papers

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

  • Mobile Site
  • Staff Directory
  • Advertise with Ars

Filter by topic

  • Biz & IT
  • Gaming & Culture

Front page layout

Unknowns —

More unidentified illnesses linked to unexplained bird flu case in missouri, the update raises questions about how the health investigation is going..

Beth Mole - Sep 27, 2024 8:15 pm UTC

A warning sign outside a laboratory testing the H5N1 bird flu virus at The Pirbright Institute in Woking, UK, on Monday, March 13, 2023.

More than a month after a person in Missouri mysteriously fell ill with H5-type bird flu, investigators in the state are still identifying people who became ill after contact with the patient, raising questions about the diligence of the ongoing health investigation.

Further Reading

Given the unexplained source of infection, health investigators in the state have been working to track the virus both backward in time—to try to identify the source—and forward—to identify any potential onward spread. The bird flu patient was initially hospitalized on August 22 but recovered and had been released by the time the state publicly reported the case.

In an update Friday , September 27, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention relayed that Missouri officials have now identified four more health care workers who experienced mild respiratory illnesses after caring for the person with bird flu. None of the four workers were tested for flu at the time of their illnesses and all have since recovered.

Testing new cases for antibodies to H5N1

The four newly identified cases bring the total number of health care workers who fell ill after contact to six. Missouri investigators had previously identified two other health care workers who developed mild respiratory symptoms. One of those workers was tested for flu around the time of their illness—and tested negative. But the other, like the four newly identified cases, was not tested. That person has since submitted a blood sample to test for bird flu antibodies, which would indicate a previous infection.

In addition, a household contact of the bird flu patient also fell ill at the same time as the patient, suggesting a possible common source of the infection.

But, the illnesses do clearly raise concern about the health investigation, which is being conducted by Missouri officials. "The slow trickle of info is the most concerning part," infectious disease expert Krutika Kuppalli wrote on social media Friday. The CDC can get involved at the request of a state, but such a request has not been made. For now, the CDC is only providing technical assistance from Atlanta.

In its update today, the CDC emphasized that "to date, only one case of influenza A(H5N1) has been detected in Missouri. No contacts of that case have tested positive for influenza A(H5N1)." The agency added that blood testing results for H5 antibodies are pending.

Currently, 239 dairy herds in 14 states have been infected with H5N1.

reader comments

Channel ars technica.

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Robotics

Title: gait switching and enhanced stabilization of walking robots with deep learning-based reachability: a case study on two-link walker.

Abstract: Learning-based approaches have recently shown notable success in legged locomotion. However, these approaches often lack accountability, necessitating empirical tests to determine their effectiveness. In this work, we are interested in designing a learning-based locomotion controller whose stability can be examined and guaranteed. This can be achieved by verifying regions of attraction (RoAs) of legged robots to their stable walking gaits. This is a non-trivial problem for legged robots due to their hybrid dynamics. Although previous work has shown the utility of Hamilton-Jacobi (HJ) reachability to solve this problem, its practicality was limited by its poor scalability. The core contribution of our work is the employment of a deep learning-based HJ reachability solution to the hybrid legged robot dynamics, which overcomes the previous work's limitation. With the learned reachability solution, first, we can estimate a library of RoAs for various gaits. Second, we can design a one-step predictive controller that effectively stabilizes to an individual gait within the verified RoA. Finally, we can devise a strategy that switches gaits, in response to external perturbations, whose feasibility is guided by the RoA analysis. We demonstrate our method in a two-link walker simulation, whose mathematical model is well established. Our method achieves improved stability than previous model-based methods, while ensuring transparency that was not present in the existing learning-based approaches.
Comments: The first two authors contributed equally. This work is supported in part by the NSF Grant CMMI-1944722, the NSF CAREER Program under award 2240163, the NASA ULI on Safe Aviation Autonomy, and the DARPA Assured Autonomy and Assured Neuro Symbolic Learning and Reasoning (ANSR) programs. The work of Jason J. Choi received the support of a fellowship from Kwanjeong Educational Foundation, Korea
Subjects: Robotics (cs.RO); Machine Learning (cs.LG); Systems and Control (eess.SY)
Cite as: [cs.RO]
  (or [cs.RO] for this version)
  Focus to learn more arXiv-issued DOI via DataCite

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • HTML (experimental)
  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

BibTeX formatted citation

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

IMAGES

  1. PPT

    science of reading case study

  2. SOLUTION: Case Study Improving the Student's Reading Comprehension Case

    science of reading case study

  3. The Science of Reading: A Guide to Structured Literacy

    science of reading case study

  4. How to Support Struggling Readers Based on the Science of Reading

    science of reading case study

  5. The Science of Reading Explained

    science of reading case study

  6. 25+ Science of Reading, Language, Literacy & Learning Resources

    science of reading case study

VIDEO

  1. PALM READING CASE STUDY:A woman with the potential to make a big change #shorts #palmistry

  2. PALM READING CASE STUDY:Typical auspicious signs for someone with extraordinary talent #palmistry

  3. PALM READING CASE STUDY:A very rare palm reading of a 60 year old woman #shorts #palmistry

  4. The Educator's Science of Reading Toolbox: Intensifying Reading Instruction for Students

  5. PALM READING CASE STUDY:A person with strong luck who can obtain unusual luck #shorts #palmistry

  6. PALM READING CASE STUDY:A 50-year-old man with potential for success #shorts #palmistry

COMMENTS

  1. Evidence-Based: Does the Science of Reading Really Work?

    The Science of Reading Works for Students and Teachers. The science of reading is a gold-standard body of research, and instruction based on the science of reading has shown to be more effective at teaching all students how to read. It's in the name—evidence-based instruction is proven to work. Decades worth of research has shown that ...

  2. How the Science of Reading Informs 21st‐Century Education

    Abstract. The science of reading should be informed by an evolving evidence base built upon the scientific method. Decades of basic research and randomized controlled trials of interventions and instructional routines have formed a substantial evidence base to guide best practices in reading instruction, reading intervention, and the early ...

  3. PDF The Science of Reading

    Suggested citation: Trane, F. E., Rivas, M. J., & Jones, M. R. (2022). The Science of Reading: What research says about setting young readers up for success. WestEd. WestEd is a nonpartisan, nonprofit agency that conducts and applies research, develops evidence-based solutions, and provides services and resources in the realms of education ...

  4. The Latest in Science of Reading Research

    The Science of Reading (SoR) is a comprehensive body of research that encompasses years of scientific knowledge and spans across many languages. The research shares the contributions of experts from relevant disciplines such as education, special education, literacy, psychology, neurology, and more. There have been thousands of studies and ...

  5. The science of reading: research and resources

    The science of reading is the current body of evidence on how children learn to read. A structured literacy approach, based in the science of reading, is the best approach for nearly all children. The evidence we have is consistent and overwhelming: we know that explicit and systematic phonics instruction typically leads to better outcomes in ...

  6. PDF How the Science of Reading Informs 21st-Century Education

    Scientific research, whether in education, physics, anthropology, molecular biology, or economics, is a con-tinual process of rigorous reasoning supported by a dynamic interplay among methods, theories, and findings. It builds understandings in the form of models or theories that can be tested.

  7. The Science of Reading: Supports, Critiques, and Questions

    "The science of reading" is a phrase representing the accumulated knowledge about reading, reading development, and best practices for reading instruction obtained by the use of the scientific method.…Collectively, research studies with a focus on reading have yielded a substantial knowledge base of stable findings based on the science of reading.

  8. The Reading League

    Learn the science of reading, transform reading instruction, and improve student outcomes. The science of reading is a vast, interdisciplinary body of scientifically-based research about reading and issues related to reading and writing. The Science of Reading: Defining Guide is available as a free resource for download or as a hard copy for ...

  9. Critical Issues in the Science of Reading: Striving for a Wide-Angle

    National Reading Panel (U.S.) and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U.S.). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services ...

  10. The Science of Reading: Supports, Critiques, and Questions: Reading

    Reading Research Quarterly is the leading global journal offering multidisciplinary scholarship on literacy among learners of all ages, including the latest research studies.

  11. Translational science in the science of reading: A case study

    INTRODUCTION. Translational science refers to the critical process of turning basic scientific research into real-world applications (Rubio et al., 2010).In the world of education, translational science serves as a critical bridge connecting empirical research findings to their practical application in educational settings, particularly in the context of teaching reading to students with ...

  12. PDF Design and Methodology in the Science of Reading

    studies, group-based designs, and single-case studies, to ethnographic studies, and obser-vational studies, amongst other approaches. While all contribute to the science of reading, design and methodology have implications for the evidentiary value one should place upon a study's results, or more plainly, how the results of a study should be ...

  13. A Full Breakdown of the Science of Reading Components

    8/8/2024. A Full Breakdown of the Science of Reading Components. Every day in the U.S., school district leaders are coming to the realization that the way districts have been teaching reading needs to change. According to the Nation's Report Card (NAEP), only 33% of fourth grade students and 31% of eighth grade students performed at or above ...

  14. The Science of Reading: The Basics

    Teaching Based on the 5 Big Ideas. Phonemic Awareness - The ability to identify and play with individual sounds in spoken words. Phonics - Reading instruction on understanding how letters and groups of letters link to sounds to form letter- sound relationships and spelling patterns. Fluency - The ability to read words, phrases, sentences, and ...

  15. The Science of Reading and Its Educational Implications

    The Two Cultures of Science and Education. Learning to read is an educational issue, historically the purview of educators, specifically schools of education. The history of education in the US has been extensively documented, mainly from the perspective of educators themselves (e.g., Ravitch, 2000; Cremin, 1988).

  16. PDF Science of Reading

    Science of Reading Although the scientific evidence base for e†ective reading has existed for decades, the term "the science of reading" has gained traction in the last few years, potentially leading to misunderstandings. As a result, we believe that a common definition is useful for the field. 4 Science of Reading: Defining Guide

  17. What is the Science of Reading?

    The Science of Reading is a comprehensive body of research that encompasses years of scientific knowledge, spans across many languages, and shares the contributions of experts from relevant disciplines such as education, special education, literacy, psychology, neurology, and more. The Science of Reading has evolved from a wide span of research ...

  18. PDF Science of Reading: Leading for Literacy (Brief 7 of 7)

    The term Science of Reading refers to a body of 50+ years of interdisciplinary research that describes literacy development and provides guidance for instruction and interventions from early childhood through adolescence. See Briefs 1 and 2 for Science Of Reading Explained: Key Ideas And Myths. A high-quality instructional architecture reflects ...

  19. PDF Science of Reading Solution: Foundational Literacy Professional

    The Strategy. th the CDE, PCG's experts partnered with national literacy leaders to create customizedfoundational literacy professional development conten. based on science of reading research—and designed for immediate use by K-3 teachers. Starting in 2020, to align with Colorado's READ Act, K-3 classroom teachers across state districts ...

  20. The Science of Reading and the Media: Does the Media Draw on High

    Recommendation: Consider whether "science of reading" news acknowledges the uninspiring track record of real-world phonics-intensive initiatives. These four kinds of errors show up in numerous "science of reading" articles in the popular press beyond the two analyzed here. Particularly in combination with media bias errors, they do real ...

  21. PDF CASE STUDY Science of Reading Literacy Program is the Best 'Bang for

    Science of Reading Literacy Program is the Best "Bang for the Buck" lexialearning.com Finding Support and Resources When it comes to reading skills, third grade is a crucial time—students should be transitioning from learning to read, to reading to learn. At Northeast Elementary School in Meridian, Mississippi, those skills are measured ...

  22. The Science of Reading

    The Science of Reading is a vast, interdisciplinary body of scientifically-based research on reading and writing developed by the Defining Movement Coalition. This research has been conducted worldwide over the last five decades and derives from thousands of studies in multiple languages. The science of reading explains:

  23. Science of Teaching Reading Study Aids

    English Language Arts and Reading/Social Studies with Science of Teaching Reading: Grades 4-8 . The STR exam will be required for the issuance of intern, probationary, and standard certifications for these certification fields beginning January 1, 2021. Teachers who are currently standard certified in Core Subjects: EC-6, Core Subjects: 4-8 ...

  24. Vocational High School Teachers' Perception of ...

    As Vocational High School (VHS) students are prepared to work in certain fields, project-based learning (PjBL)becomes one of the teaching and learning methods that teachers can use to meet the expectations in the work field. Even though the effectiveness of PjBL implementation is related to how the teacher perceives the method, less attention is given to studies that focus on teachers ...

  25. Factors Affecting Household Evacuation Decision Making in Response to

    Sociodemographic factors found in this study to reduce the probability of evacuating include being a part of a larger household, being in an urban setting and not being owner-occupied. This is consistent with previous case studies in both the global north (Hasan et al., 2012; Ng et al., 2014) and global south (Lim et al., 2016, 2019).

  26. More unidentified illnesses linked to unexplained bird flu case in

    No contacts of that case have tested positive for influenza A(H5N1)." The agency added that blood testing results for H5 antibodies are pending. Currently, 239 dairy herds in 14 states have been ...

  27. [2409.16301] Gait Switching and Enhanced Stabilization of Walking

    Computer Science > Robotics. arXiv:2409.16301 (cs) [Submitted on 10 Sep 2024] ... A Case Study on Two-link Walker, by Xingpeng Xia and 5 other authors. View PDF HTML (experimental) Abstract: Learning-based approaches have recently shown notable success in legged locomotion. However, these approaches often lack accountability, necessitating ...

  28. Electrochemically assisted remediation of a highly chlorinated organic

    A long-standing aim of this landfill has been the removal of buried waste and the treatment of the leachate produced by rainfall, which has been confined in ponds, such as the one presented in this study [12].The pond covers an area of 50×25 m 2, where 600 m 3 of sludge has been collected, covered by a water film on the surface, the height of which depends on climatic conditions such as ...

  29. Is Pyrolysis Treatment a Viable Solution to Detoxify Metal(loid)s in

    Please note: If you switch to a different device, you may be asked to login again with only your ACS ID.

  30. Dynamic Simulation of Street-Level Carbon Emissions in Megacities: A

    To address the aforementioned knowledge gap, this study uses Wuhan City, China, as a case study and integrates multi-source data to develop a comprehensive simulation model for high-precision street-level CE dynamic simulation from 2015 to 2030 ... Science of The Total Environment, 906 (2024), Article 167692, 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2023.167692.