An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Psychological Meanings of Eating Disorders and Their Association With Symptoms, Motivation Toward Treatment, and Clinical Evolution Among Outpatients

Marie-pierre gagnon-girouard, marie-pier chenel-beaulieu, carole ratté, catherine bégin.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

École de psychologie, Pavillon Félix-Antoine-Savard, 2325, rue des Bibliothèques, Université Laval, Québec (Québec) G1V 0A6. [email protected]

Received 2018 Mar 22; Accepted 2018 Nov 12; Collection date 2019 Jun.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) 4.0 License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Unlike patients suffering from egodystonic disorders, people with eating disorders sometimes attribute positive meanings to their symptoms, and this attribution process contributes to the maintenance of the disorder. This study aims at exploring psychological meanings of eating disorders and their associations with symptoms, motivation toward treatment, and clinical evolution. Eighty-one adults with an eating disorder (anorexia nervosa, n = 46 and bulimia nervosa, n = 35) treated in a day-hospital program were asked, each week over an 8-week period, to identify the psychological meanings they ascribed to their eating disorder. Avoidance was the most frequently identified meaning, followed by mental strength, security, death, confidence, identity, care, and communication. Avoidance was more frequently mentioned by participants with bulimia than in cases of anorexia. Security and mental strength were associated with less motivation toward treatment. Death was associated with more depressive and anxious symptoms. An exploratory factor analysis showed that these meanings formed three main dimensions: Avoidance, Intrapsychic, and Relational. Findings suggest that psychological meanings associated with eating disorders can be assessed and used as a clinical tool to increase treatment acceptability and effectiveness.

Keywords: Bulimia, anorexia, depression, anxiety, motivation, maintenance factors, ambivalence toward treatment

Treating eating disorders (ED) is challenging, notably since a significant number of patients deny being sick, or show ambivalence toward change ( Abbate-Daga et al., 2013 ; Campbell, 2009 ; Zerbe, 2007 ). Chronicity and relapse rates have been suggested to be as high as 50% ( Keel & Hersog, 2004 ; Keel & Mitchell, 1997 ). Treatment dropout rates are worrisome, ranging from 20 to 51% within inpatient samples, and from 29 to 73% in outpatient care ( Fassino et al., 2009 ).

The mere description of eating symptoms severity and frequency do not entirely reflect the complexity of ED ( Andersson & Ghaderi, 2006 ). A review of 24 qualitative studies supports the idea that many patients suffering from anorexia nervosa (AN) value their problematic eating behaviors, viewing them as a way to face personal issues ( Espíndola & Blay, 2009 ). ED symptoms may compensate for low self-esteem, feelings of ineffectiveness, cognitive rigidity, and interpersonal mistrust ( Brusset, 1998 ; Bulik et al., 2005 ; Mitchell & Bulik, 2006 ; Palmer, 2008 ). Since the psychological positive meanings that patients attribute to their ED symptoms may contribute to the maintenance of their dysfunctional eating behaviors and to their reluctance to change, a growing number of authors have proposed a more subjective, patient-centered, approach to better understand the underlying complexity of ED symptoms ( Corstorphine et al., 2006 ; Deaver et al., 2003 ; Lee & Miltenberger, 1997 ; Marzola et al., 2016 ; Serpell et al., 1999 ; Vitousek, Watson, & Wilson, 1998 ). Understanding the subjective psychological meanings of ED symptoms may explain patients’ resistances to change, which influences therapeutic alliance and adherence to treatment ( Fox, Larkin, & Leung, 2011 ; McManus & Waller, 1995 ; Nordbø et al., 2006 ).

Serpell and colleagues (1999) were among the first to study the psychological meanings of ED. They asked 18 women undergoing treatment for AN to wrote two letters to their ED, one addressing it as a friend and the other as an enemy. Based on these letters, they identified ten themes describing different psychological meanings of AN: guardian (providing protection, security, and stability), control (giving structure), attractiveness (allowing to feel more attractive), confidence (increasing self-confidence), feeling special and different (allowing to see oneself as superior to others), ability (succeeding in something difficult to achieve), avoidance (avoiding emotions and distress), communication (communicating distress to others), appearance and amenorrhea . When replicated among patients suffering from bulimia nervosa (BN) ( Serpell & Treasure, 2002 ), two additional themes were identified: eating without gaining weight and coping with boredom . These results were coherent with the clinical reports demonstrating that, apart from their weight regulation function, bulimic behaviors contribute to the regulation of affect and internal states ( Corstorphine et al., 2006 ; Deaver et al., 2003 ; Lee & Miltenberger, 1997 ; Milligan & Waller, 2000 ; Stickney & Miltenberger, 1999 ; Waller, Quinton, & Watson, 1995 ).

In order to get a clearer understanding of patients’ perception of their life with AN, Nordbø and his colleagues (2006) used interactive semi-structured interviews with a focus on the subjective experience of patients. These 90- to 120-minute interviews allowed interviewers to clarify and deepen the perspective of each patient regarding the psychological meanings of their ED. Eighteen women suffering from AN were asked to describe “how it is to have AN?.” Text analyses were then completed based on interviews verbatim. Eight psychological themes were extensively described: security , avoidance , mental strength , self-confidence , identity , care , communication , and death. This typology overlaps with Serpell’s classification, and is relevant for both AN and BN ( Fox, Larkin, & Leung, 2011 ). Nordbø’s classification was also supported by a systematic review ( Espíndola & Blay, 2009 ).

To date, the efforts devoted to the exploration of the psychological meanings of ED symptoms were based on qualitative studies. Very few quantitative studies have empirically investigated the meanings of ED symptoms, and to our knowledge, only one study examined the association between these self-reported meanings and the clinical profile of patients ( Marzola et al., 2016 ). Based largely on Nordbø’s classification and the systematic review conducted by Espíndola and Blay (2009) , Marzola and colleagues (2016) developped a self-report questionnaire designed to assess triggers of AN onset as well as meanings and impact of AN. They reported that psychological meanings ascribed to AN can be regrouped in three independent factors (intrapsychic, relational and avoidance), and that these factors are all associated with eating psychopathology, depressive symptoms as well as personality traits. Factors also showed differential associations with age and motivation to treatment. Considering that the study conducted by Marzola and colleagues (2016) focused only on AN, the inclusion of both AN and BN should be prioritized as most ED treatments are now based on a transdiagnostic approach to ED. Finally, it is important to determine whether psychological meanings ascribed to ED symptoms predict treatment evolution and outcome because differential treatment responses might be expected depending on specific meanings attributed to ED.

In this context, the present study aimed to further study Nordbø and colleagues’ classification (2006) with an open-ended question, in a sample of individuals suffering from AN or BN, and to examine the link between self-reported ED meanings and 1) ED diagnosis (AN or BN), 2) ED symptoms severity and general symptoms (depression and anxiety), 3) motivation toward treatment, and 4) evolution of symptoms and motivation throughout treatment. Results from the present study will also be compared to those recently obtained by Marzola and colleagues (2016) to assess whether the three-factor structure (intrapsychic, relational and avoidance) is robust across clinical samples and if these factors predict symptoms severity, therapeutic motivation, and treatment response.

Participants and Procedure

Eighty-one adult women suffering from AN ( n = 46) or BN ( n = 35) participated in the study (aged from 18 to 65 years old). From 2006 to 2010, all women who were admitted to the day-hospital program of the Treatment Program for Eating Disorders at the Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Québec (Canada) were included. No woman refused to participate in the study, as it was integral part of the treatment program (see procedure for more details). They all received a diagnosis by a psychiatrist specialized in ED who applied DSM-IV criteria to diagnose AN or BN.

ED and Clinical Symptoms

Eating symptoms were measured using the French version ( Leichner et al., 1994 ) of the Eating Attitude Test (EAT-26), a 26-item self-reported questionnaire ( Garner et al., 1982 ). A higher score indicated greater symptoms and concerns regarding ED. The French version has demonstrated good internal consistency, which was comparable to the original version (Cronbach’s alpha = .90) ( Gauthier et al., 2014 ; Jonat & Birmingham, 2004 ). Its test-retest reliability in the long term is controversial, mostly because symptoms may vary massively with treatment, which was expected in our study ( Banasiak et al., 2001 ). Depressive symptoms were measured using a French version ( Bourque & Beaudette, 1982 ) of the 21-item Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) ( Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996 ), one of the most widely used psychometric tests for measuring depression. A higher score indicated more severe depressive symptoms. The French version has demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .92) and adequate stability over a 4-month period ( Bourque & Beaudette, 1982 ). Anxiety symptoms were measured by the French version ( Freeston et al., 1994 ) of the Beck Anxiety Inventory ( Beck & Steer, 1990 ), which is a well-known self-reported 21-item questionnaire that assesses physiological symptoms associated with anxiety. Again, a higher score indicated worst physiological symptoms related to anxiety. The French version has demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .93) and adequate test-retest reliability ( r = .63) ( Freeston et al., 1994 ).

Motivation Toward Treatment

Motivation toward treatment was measured with an adapted version of the Treatment Self-Regulation Questionnaire (TSRQ) ( Williams, Freedman, & Deci, 1998 ), an 8-item questionnaire, which was translated in French by our team (double translation process). The original version of the TSRQ assesses reasons for staying in a weight-loss program and this instrument was modified to measure reasons to stay in an ED program. A higher score indicated higher motivation toward ED program. The original version has demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.81 to 0.86).

Psychological Meanings of ED

The psychological meanings that participants attributed to their ED were documented through an open-ended question: What does my ED mean to me (what purpose does my ED fulfill)? Participants could write as many meanings as they wanted.

The detailed study protocol was reviewed and approved beforehand by the ethic committee of the hospital where the study took place. On the first day of the program, a doctoral student informed the patients about the purpose and procedure of the study. All participants were invited to participate in the study and were informed that, during the treatment program, time would be reserved to complete the questionnaires. If they agreed to participate, they signed an informed consent form approved by the ethic committee of the hospital. Then, questionnaires were filled in by participants every Friday morning for the entire length of the 8-week treatment program. All questionnaires were completed in the same order as presented in the measures section. Accordingly, the question regarding psychological meanings of ED was completed at the end of the protocol each week.

Data Analysis

Classification procedure.

Based on Nordbø and colleagues’ classification ( Nordbø et al., 2006 ), we categorized participants’ statements into ED psychological meanings. Classification procedure was based on the model proposed by Taylor-Powell and Renner (2006) and consisted of four steps: 1) a priori description of themes, 2) preliminary reading of the data, 3) improvement of code definitions, and 4) coding. This procedure allowed preserving the nature of the data generated by participants while allowing the replication of previous structures ( Nordbø et al., 2006 ). Based on Nordbø and colleagues’ denominations, we first defined an a priori list of eight themes representing proposed psychological meanings. Two independent raters with ED clinical experience made a preliminary reading of all the available data to ensure that no additional theme was needed, after which the definitions of themes were refined, leading to a detailed description of all meanings (summarized in Table 1 ). For example, at this step, it was proposed to add the theme Death, to include statements related to self-destruction in general. The two raters used the final version of the typology to code all participants’ statements. Inter-raters agreement was tested on a sub-sample of 15 participants at the beginning of classification and at mid-process.

Table 1. Brief Description of Meanings Based on the Classification From Nordbø and Colleagues ( Nordbø et al., 2006 ).

Statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS Statistical software (version 19.0). For each participant, a score from 0 to 8 was assigned for each meaning, based on the weekly identification of the meaning. A score of 0 was attributed if the meaning was never mentioned by the participant whereas a score of 8 was attributed if the meaning was mentioned every week of the program. For example, a participant who identified only avoidance from week 1 to 8 and security from week 2 to week 8 obtained a score of 8 on the avoidance meaning, 7 on the security meaning, and 0 for all the other meanings. Consequently, for each participant, each meaning had an endorsement score (from 0 to 8) that represented the frequency of its adoption in the participant’s responses.

t -tests were computed to compare ED diagnosis groups (AN vs. BN) on the eight meanings endorsement’s scores. Pearson’s coefficients were computed to assess the relationship between meanings endorsement scores on one hand, and the clinical symptoms, motivation toward treatment and evolution throughout treatment, on the other hand. The evolution of symptoms and motivation throughout treatment was calculated as the difference between the mean score at the end of treatment and the mean score at baseline.

Pearson’s correlation analyses were also computed to assess the relationships between the eight meanings endorsement scores. An exploratory factorial analysis was also performed with the Varimax rotation method in order to obtain a more easily interpretable structure. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) indicator was used to measure the sampling adequacy, with a KMO of 0.7 considered as acceptable. A confirmatory factorial analysis was computed to verify the fit of the factorial structure in the data from the sample. Indices of adjustment were the chi-square test, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). A chi-square value close to zero and a chi-square p-value greater than 0.05 indicate a good fit, as well as a CFI value of 0.90 or greater and a RMSEA of 0.06 or less.

Classification

Inter-raters agreement for classification of statements was excellent, with a kappa coefficient of 0.91. Most statements (79.68%) were classified within Nordbø and colleagues’ classification (2006) . Statements that did not fit into any category were labelled “others.” These statements were either non-psychologically oriented or very vague (for example, I don’t know why). No consistent new theme emerged from these statements.

Mean scores for each meaning are presented in Table 2 . A voidance was the most frequently identified meaning, followed by mental strength , security , death , confidence , identity , care , and communication.

ED Diagnosis

Mean scores for psychological meanings according to ED diagnosis are presented in Table 2 . Participants suffering from BN endorsed significantly more often the avoidance meaning than participants suffering from AN ( d  = 0.66). Participants suffering from AN mentioned more often the mental strength and identity meanings than participants with BN, and even if the differences did not reach significance, the effect size were moderate ( d = 0.34 and 0.32). No difference was noted for other meanings. For participants with BN, avoidance was endorsed 3.5 times more than the second most frequent meaning which was mental strength , whereas for participants with AN, the difference between avoidance and mental strength was of lesser magnitude ( avoidance 1.5 times more frequent than mental strength ).

Short-Term and Long-Term Links With Clinical Profile

Correlations between meanings and symptoms, motivation, and their evolution are presented in Table 3 . Death was positively associated with depressive and anxiety symptoms, meaning that the more a participant endorsed the death meaning, the more this participant reported being depressed and anxious at the beginning of treatment. Security and mental strength were negatively associated with motivation toward treatment, meaning that the more a participant endorsed these meanings, the less she reported being motivated at the beginning of treatment. There was no other significant correlation between meanings and ED symptoms. There was no significant association between meanings and the evolution of symptoms and motivation throughout treatment.

Table 4. Pearson’ Coefficients for Correlations Between Meanings.

An exploratory factor analysis yielded three factors, based on the presence of three eigenvalues greater than 1, for a percentage of explained variance of 62% ( Table 5 ). The KMO for the factorial structure was 0.62, which shows a poorly fitted model. Based on the structure proposed by Marzola and colleagues (2016) , the three factors were named Avoidant ( avoidance ), Relational ( confidence, care , and communication ), and Intrapsychic ( mental strength, identity , and death ). Security loaded significantly on both the Avoidant and Intrapsychic factors, and it was considered part of the Intrapsychic factor, to be coherent with Marzola and colleagues’ (2016) typology. Moreover, the magnitude of the correlation coefficients was more important between security and the subscales of the intrapsychic factor ( r = .129 to .175) than the avoidant one ( r = .028). The confirmatory factor analysis revealed a better adjustment to the data (χ² = 21.79, p = .295; CFI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.042). This confirmatory factor analysis suggests that the replication of this analysis in a larger sample could validate it, notably with a sample of at least 10 subjects per variable, as previously suggested ( Hair, 1998 ). Correlations between factors and symptoms, motivation, and their evolution were also performed. There was no significant association ( r ranging from −0.04 to −0.18).

Table 5. Meanings’ Loadings Following the Exploratory Factor Analysis.

The present study aimed at exploring the subjective psychological meanings that ED patients attribute to their ED, based on a previous typology by Nordbø and colleagues (2006) , using a simple self-reported statement instead of interviews. This study adds to existing literature by exploring the links between self-reported ED meanings and ED diagnosis (AN or BN), ED and general symptoms severity, and motivation toward treatment, as well as evolution of symptoms, and motivation throughout treatment, in a sample of women suffering from either AN or BN.

First, the very high inter-rater agreement ( K = 0.91) as well as the high percentage of statements that could be categorized (79.68%) showed that the classification from Nordbø and colleagues (2006) has clinical significance among patients suffering from AN and BN, and can be used to classify simple self-reported statements. In previous studies, the meanings were identified through carefully conducted interactive semi-structured interviews, which greatly deepened the understanding of underlying meanings attributed to ED by participants, but which were costly and required a lot of time and resources. The present study replicates this typology with a simple self-reported question repeated throughout treatment, which is significantly less costly and complex. Findings give empirical support to this method of assessing psychological meanings of ED in clinical settings.

Participants in this study identified mainly avoidance, mental strength, and security as the primary meanings of their disorder, which is similar to previous studies’ findings ( Nordbø et al., 2006 ; Serpell & Treasure, 2002 ; Serpell et al., 1999 ). When ED diagnosis was taken into consideration, it was found that the adoption of disturbed eating behaviors to soothe or avoid aversive internal states, as described by the avoidance meaning, was more frequently reported by participants suffering from BN than by those with AN. These results are in concordance with the literature, suggesting that binge eating may serve to escape or temporarily reduce negative affects such as anxiety, anger, irritation, depression, and boredom ( Deaver et al., 2003 ; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991 ; Kaye et al., 1986 ; McManus & Waller, 1995 ; Milligan & Waller, 2000 ; Stickney & Miltenberger, 1999 ; Waller, Quinton, & Watson, 1995 ), by shifting the attention focus on food, while purging behaviors can serve to reduce guilt and worry associated with binge eating ( Corstorphine et al., 2006 ; Kaye et al., 1986 ; Milligan & Waller, 2000 ).

Among participants with AN, the security and mental strength meanings were the most frequently endorsed meanings, although they did not appear to be as common as in previous studies, avoidance being the most frequent meaning ( Nordbø et al., 2006 ; Serpell et al., 1999 ; Vitousek, Watson, & Wilson, 1998 ). Such dissimilarity might be related to our sample composition, in which both subtypes of AN patients (restricting and binge-eating/purging) were included. Security and mental strength are possibility more prevalent in purely restrictive AN patients, who are known to show high self-control. Control over one’s body and food could artificially compensates for feelings of ineffectiveness and low self-esteem, as an attempt to internally control external reality ( Chassler, 1994 ; Gabbard, 2000 ; Vitousek, Watson, & Wilson, 1998 ).

Considering the aggregation of meanings into three main dimensions, results from our study not only replicate the three-factor structure proposed by Marzola and colleagues (2016) but the theoretical content of our factors is similar to the one obtained by this research team. The avoidance factor represents a strategy for escaping negative emotions and experience. The relational factor represents a way of communicating patient’s own messages via problematic symptoms (ex. a way of communicating difficulties to other people, of eliciting care from other people, of feeling acknowledged). The intrapsychic factor is more oriented toward patients own motives and internal world (ex. a way of obtaining a sense of stability and security, of getting an inner sense of mastery and strength, of creating a different identity or personality). In the present study, the three factors were not related to ED symptoms. Nevertheless, subscales from the intrapsychic factor were associated with greater psychopathology ( death ) and less motivation toward treatment ( mental strength and security ). These results are similar to those obtained by Marzola and colleagues (2016) , who found more depression and less motivation in relation with the intrapsychic factor. Intrapsychic motives may be related to less favorable prognostic ( Marzola et al., 2016 ). It is however important to note that death being part of our intrapsychic factor was a novel finding, and this should be replicated. The absence of difference within the three factors regarding ED symptoms in the present study might be explained by the fact that we used a rather general measure of eating problems, instead of a more specific one, as Marzola and colleagues (2016) did . As such, they found different associations between the factors and ED problems depending on the facets measured. Still, they did not find associations between their factors and BMI, duration of illness as well as duration of treatment/psychotherapy. According to them, the independence of ED meanings from duration of illness and treatment supported their results since it ensured that psychological meanings were not contaminated by illness or over-rationalization following treatment.

From a clinical point of view, our study underlines the relevance of considering the subjective reality of ED patients, rather than only describing EDs as observable and discrete behaviors. As Nordbø and colleagues (2006) mentioned, patients describe the psychological meanings of ED as precipitating, contributing and maintaining their disorder. Adding interventions specifically targeting psychological meanings of EDs to existing treatment could increase treatment acceptability and effectiveness, especially if these interventions address ED’s benefits such as a greater sense of security, identity and mental strength. For the patient, it could carry a feeling of being more fully understood (as the therapist would understand the meaning of problematic behaviors) and could give sense to the healing process, which helps developing the therapeutic alliance, as well as ensuring motivation and adherence to treatment (Fox, Larkin, & Leung, 2010; Nordbø et al., 2006 ). Furthermore, by targeting the prominent ED meanings for each patient, interventions would be more personalized and adapted to the patient discourse, which may reduce the heterogeneity of the clinical response. Finally, considering that ED develops at a relatively young age, it would be very interesting for future studies to investigate ED meanings among teenagers to validate the different themes that we have found and their frequency among those patients. This will support future interventions on ED meanings with adolescents.

Although the present study has a number of strengths (a single question to assess ED psychological meanings, distinction between AN and BN, reference to a theoretical framework), results must be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, considering that all patients were recruited at their admission session of a day hospital program in a specialised ED unit, we cannot ensure that their ED diagnosis (AN or BN) remained stable throughout the study as diagnosis crossover may have happened. Second, although the sample size was larger than previous studies, it was still not large enough to examine subtypes of AN. Further studies should test these associations within sub-samples of individuals with AN (restrictive or binge-eating/purging), BN and BED separately, with more refined measures of eating symptoms. Finally, given the correlational nature of the present study, the direction of causality between ED psychological meanings and clinical symptoms, motivation, and their evolution cannot be assumed.

In sum, the present study adds to previous empirical investigations by showing that ED psychological meanings can be assessed rigorously by a simple self-reported statement, which is less expensive and time-consuming than semi-structured interviews. In addition, findings suggest that the inclusion of explicit interventions specifically targeting psychological meanings of ED could increase treatment acceptability and effectiveness.

Acknowledgments

The authors have no support to report.

Biographies

Marie-Pierre Gagnon-Girouard , Ph.D., is a professor of psychology at the Department of psychology in Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières. Her research interests converge around binge eating disorder, body image and weight bias. She is also a clinical psychologist who works with individuals suffering from eating disorders and body image difficulties.

Marie-Pier Chenel Beaulieu is a psychologist at the Integrated Health and Social Services Centres (CISSS) affiliated to Laval University. She is working with adults suffering from mental health disorders, including eating disorders. Her doctoral research focussed on the psychological meanings underlying eating disorders.

Annie Aimé is a professor at the Université du Québec en Outaouais. Her research and clinical interests mainly focus on eating disorders, body satisfaction, weight bullying and weight-based stigmatization, and emotional well-being. She focuses on youth, adults and parents who suffer from weight, shape and eating problems.

Carole Ratté is a retired professor of psychiatry at the Faculty of Medicine of Laval University. She founded the Intervention Program for Eating Disorders provided in Quebec CIUSS. Her research interests include the etiology and treatment of eating disorders.

Catherine Bégin is a professor of psychology at the School of psychology of Laval University. She is the director of a multidisciplinary clinic specialized in the treatment of eating and weight disorders. Her main research interests stands on profiling people suffering from eating and weight issues. She is also interested in the etiology and treatment of eating and weight disorders.

The authors have no funding to report.

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

  • Abbate-Daga G., Amianto F., Delsedime N., De-Bacco C., Fassino S. (2013). Resistance to treatment and change in anorexia nervosa: A clinical overview. BMC Psychiatry , 13, 294 10.1186/1471-244X-13-294 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Andersson G., Ghaderi A. (2006). Overview and analysis of the behaviourist criticism of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM). Clinical Psychologist , 10(2), 67–77. 10.1080/13284200600690461 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Banasiak S. J., Wertheim E. H., Koerner J., Voudouris N. J. (2001). Test-retest reliability and internal consistency of a variety of measures of dietary restraint and body concerns in a sample of adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 29(1), 85–89 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Beck Anxiety Inventory: Manual . San Antonio, TX, USA: The Psychological Corporation. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Inventaire de Dépression de Beck: Manuel (2nd ed.). Toronto, Canada: Pearson Canada Assessment. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bourque P., Beaudette D. (1982). Étude psychometrique du questionnaire de dépression de Beck auprès d'un échantillon d'étudiants universitaires francophones. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des Sciences du comportement , 14(3), 211–218. 10.1037/h0081254 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brusset, B. (1998). Psychopathologie de l'anorexie mentale . Paris, France: Dunod. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bulik C. M., Reba L., Siega-Riz A.-M., Reichborn-Kjennerud T. (2005). Anorexia nervosa: Definition, epidemiology, and cycle of risk. The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 37 Suppl, S2–S9. 10.1002/eat.20107 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell M. (2009). Drop-out from treatment for the eating disorders: A problem for clinicians and researchers. European Eating Disorders Review , 17(4), 239–242. 10.1002/erv.934 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chassler L. (1994). “In hunger I am king”—Understanding anorexia nervosa from a psychoanalytic perspective: Theoretical and clinical implications. Clinical Social Work Journal , 22(4), 397–415. 10.1007/BF02190330 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Corstorphine E., Waller G., Ohanian V., Baker M. (2006). Changes in internal states across the Binge-Vomit Cycle in Bulimia Nervosa. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease , 194(6), 446–449. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000221303.64098.23 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Deaver C. M., Miltenberger R. G., Smyth J., Meidinger A., Crosby R. (2003). An evaluation of affect and binge eating. Behavior Modification , 27(4), 578–599. 10.1177/0145445503255571 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Espíndola C. R., Blay S. L. (2009). Anorexia nervosa’s meaning to patients: A qualitative synthesis. Psychopathology , 42(2), 69–80. 10.1159/000203339 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fassino S., Piero A., Tomba E., Abbate-Daga G. (2009). Factors associated with dropout from treatment for eating disorders: A comprehensive literature review. BMC Psychiatry, 9(1), 67 10.1186/1471-244X-9-67 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fox A. P., Larkin M., Leung N. (2011). The personal meaning of eating disorder symptoms: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Journal of Health Psychology , 16(1), 116–125 10.1177/1359105310368449 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Freeston M. H., Ladouceur R., Thibodeau N., Gagnon F., Rhéaume J. (1994). [The Beck Anxiety Inventory. Psychometric properties of a French translation]. L'Encephale , 20(1), 47–55. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gabbard, G. O. (2000). Psychodynamic psychiatry in clinical practice (3rd ed.). Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Garner D. M., Olmsted M. P., Bohr Y., Garfinkel P. E. (1982). The eating attitudes test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine , 12(4), 871–878. 10.1017/S0033291700049163 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gauthier C., Hassler C., Mattar L., Launay J.-M., Callebert J., Steiger H., et al. Godart N. (2014). Symptoms of depression and anxiety in anorexia nervosa: Links with plasma tryptophan and serotonin metabolism. Psychoneuroendocrinology , 39(1), 170–178. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.009 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hair, J. F. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA: Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heatherton T. F., Baumeister R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin , 110(1), 86–108. 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jonat L., Birmingham C. (2004). Disordered eating attitudes and behaviours in the high-school students of a rural Canadian community. Eating and Weight Disorders—Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity , 9(4), 285–289. 10.1007/BF03325083 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaye W. H., Gwirtsman H. E., George D. T., Weiss S. R., Jimerson D. C. (1986). Relationship of mood alterations to bingeing behaviour in bulimia. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science , 149, 479–485. 10.1192/bjp.149.4.479 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keel, P. K., & Hersog, D. B. (2004). Long-term outcome, course of illness, and mortality in anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and binge eating disorder. In T. D. B. (Ed.), Clinical handbook of eating disorders, an integrated approach (pp. 97–116). New York, NY, USA: Marcel Dekker, Inc. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keel P. K., Mitchell J. E. (1997). Outcome in bulimia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry , 154(3), 313–321. 10.1176/ajp.154.3.313 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lee M., Miltenberger R. G. (1997). Functional assessment and binge eating—A review of the literature and suggestions for future research. Behavior Modification , 21(2), 159–171. 10.1177/01454455970212002 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leichner P., Steiger H., Puentes-Neuman G., Perreault M., Gottheil N. (1994). [Validation of an eating attitude scale in a French-speaking Quebec population]. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie , 39(1), 49–54. 10.1177/070674379403900110 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marzola E., Panepinto C., Delsedime N., Amianto F., Fassino S., Abbate-Daga G. (2016). A factor analysis of the meanings of anorexia nervosa: Intrapsychic, relational, and avoidant dimensions and their clinical correlates. BMC Psychiatry , 16(1), 190 10.1186/s12888-016-0894-6 [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McManus F., Waller G. (1995). A functional analysis of binge-eating. Clinical Psychology Review , 15(8), 845–863. 10.1016/0272-7358(95)00042-9 [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Milligan R. J., Waller G. (2000). Anger and bulimic psychopathology among nonclinical women. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 28(4), 446–450. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mitchell A. M., Bulik C. M. (2006). Eating disorders and women’s health: An update. The Journal of Midwifery & Women’s Health , 51(3), 193–201. 10.1016/j.jmwh.2006.01.005 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nordbø R. H. S., Espeset E. M. S., Gulliksen K. S., Skårderud F., Holte A. (2006). The meaning of self-starvation: Qualitative study of patients’ perception of anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 39(7), 556–564. 10.1002/eat.20276 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palmer, B. (2008). Causes of eating disorders. In J. Morris (Ed.), ABC of eating disorders (pp. 5–8). Oxford, United Kingdom: Wiley-Blackwell. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Serpell L., Treasure J. (2002). Bulimia nervosa: Friend or foe? The pros and cons of bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 32(2), 164–170. 10.1002/eat.10076 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Serpell L., Treasure J., Teasdale J., Sullivan V. (1999). Anorexia nervosa: Friend or foe? The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 25(2), 177–186. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stickney M. I., Miltenberger R. G. (1999). Evaluating direct and indirect measures for the functional assessment of binge eating. The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 26(2), 195–204. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor-Powell, E., & Renner, M. (2006). Analyzing qualitative data. In Y. A. Unrau, P. A. Gabor, & R. M. Grinnell (Eds.), Evaluation in social work: The art and science of practice (4th ed., pp. 341–360). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vitousek K., Watson S., Wilson G. T. (1998). Enhancing motivation for change in treatment-resistant eating disorders. Clinical Psychology Review , 18(4), 391–420. 10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00012-9 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Waller G., Quinton S., Watson D. (1995). Processing of threat-related information by women with bulimic eating attitudes. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 18(2), 189–193. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Williams G., Freedman Z., Deci E. (1998). Supporting autonomy to motivate patients with diabetes for glucose control. Diabetes Care , 21(10), 1644–1651. 10.2337/diacare.21.10.1644 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zerbe K. J. (2007). Eating disorders in the 21st century: Identification, management, and prevention in obstetrics and gynecology. Best Practice & Research Clinical Obstetrics & Gynaecology , 21(2), 331–343. 10.1016/j.bpobgyn.2006.12.001 [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (252.0 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Anorexia nervosa and a lost emotional self: a psychological formulation of the development, maintenance, and treatment of anorexia nervosa.

\r\nAnna Oldershaw,*

  • 1 Salmons Centre for Applied Psychology, Canterbury Christ Church University, Canterbury, United Kingdom
  • 2 Kent and Medway All Age Eating Disorder Service, North East London NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
  • 3 Sussex Eating Disorders Service and Research and Development Department, Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Sussex, United Kingdom

In this paper, we argue that Anorexia Nervosa (AN) can be explained as arising from a ‘lost sense of emotional self.’ We begin by briefly reviewing evidence accumulated to date supporting the consensus that a complex range of genetic, biological, psychological, and socio-environmental risk and maintenance factors contribute to the development and maintenance of AN. We consider how current interventions seek to tackle these factors in psychotherapy and potential limitations. We then propose our theory that many risk and maintenance factors may be unified by an underpinning explanation of emotional processing difficulties leading to a lost sense of ‘emotional self.’ Further, we discuss how, once established, AN becomes ‘self-perpetuating’ and the ‘lost sense of emotional self’ relentlessly deepens. We outline these arguments in detail, drawing on empirical and neuroscientific data, before discussing the implications of this model for understanding AN and informing clinical intervention. We argue that experiential models of therapy (e.g., emotion-focused therapy; schema therapy) be employed to achieve emergence and integration of an ‘emotional self’ which can be flexibly and adaptively used to direct an individual’s needs and relationships. Furthermore, we assert that this should be a primary goal of therapy for adults with established AN.

Introduction

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is an eating disorder (ED) characterized by self-starvation driven by weight, shape and eating concerns and extreme dread of food, eating and normal body weight ( American Psychological Association [APA], 2013 ; Walsh, 2013 ; Treasure et al., 2015b ). The annual United Kingdom female incidence of AN is approximately 14 cases per 100,000 ( Micali et al., 2013 ), with up to 4% of women and 0.24% of men meeting the broad definition of AN in their lifetime ( Smink et al., 2013 ). The peak age of onset for girls is 15–25 years and for boys is 10–14 years ( Micali et al., 2013 ). AN is associated with poor prognosis and the highest mortality rates of all psychiatric disorders ( Smink et al., 2013 ).

The treatment of choice for AN is talking therapy ( National Institute for Health and Care Excellence [NICE], 2017 ). Yet the disorder has poor rates of remission and high levels of relapse. Current psychological interventions facilitate small change, with better interventions needed ( Bulik, 2014 ; Startup et al., 2015 ). Although early intervention is key to recovery, there is an average delay of 18 months from symptoms emerging to treatment, followed by multiple relapses even following treatment, each lasting 6 years ( PricewaterhouseCoopers, 2015 ). Costs of AN and other EDs to the individual, family and carers, and society are therefore substantial. Time spent caregiving for somebody with severe AN is almost twice that for somebody with a physical health disorder (e.g., cancer) or other mental health difficulty (e.g., psychosis; ( Viana et al., 2013 ). The annual cost to the United Kingdom economy is estimated to be £17.9 billion, offering a “compelling case for change” in services and treatment ( PricewaterhouseCoopers, 2015 , p. 9). There is now significant work underway in this vein; for example, the First Episode and Rapid Early Intervention for Eating Disorders (FREED; Brown et al., 2018 ).

Risk and Maintenance Factors Associated With AN

Anorexia Nervosa has been associated with numerous broad ranging risk and maintenance factors. Risk factors are variables which predict subsequent development of later pathology, in an individual currently disorder and symptom-free ( Stice, 2002 ). Risk factors can have effects mitigated by protective factors or amplified by potentiating factors . Maintenance factors predict symptom persistence versus remission over time in individuals already symptomatic for a disorder ( Stice, 2002 ). Clinical implications of risk and maintenance factors differ; risk factors are relevant to the development of preventative programs and maintenance factors to treatment interventions ( Stice, 2002 ). While we do not fully review evidence for risk and maintenance factors for AN herein; we briefly indicate those most well-accepted and summarize them diagrammatically in Figure 1 . This reveals complex interactions between genetic, biological, psychological, and socio-environmental factors in the development and maintenance of AN, with some factors proposed to represent both risk and maintenance factors.

www.frontiersin.org

Current Clinical Perspectives, Models, and Treatment for AN to Date

Psychological models to date use existing psychological theory to address relevant risk or maintaining factors within clinical treatment for AN. Recent guidance published by National Institute for Health and Care Excellence [NICE] (2017) recommends the following psychological interventions be considered for adults with AN: Eating Disorder Focused Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT-ED); Maudsley Anorexia Nervosa Treatment for Adults (MANTRA); Specialist Supportive Clinical Management (SSCM); and Focal Psychodynamic Therapy (FPT).

CBT-ED traditionally focuses on symptom-based accounts suggesting both control and overvaluation of weight/shape maintain AN. Later revisions include: clinical perfectionism; low self-esteem; mood intolerance; and, interpersonal difficulties as additional treatment foci ( Fairburn et al., 1999 , 2003 , 2009 , 2015 ). Using cognitive and behavioral techniques, it seeks to increase motivation to change, directly enhance weight gain while tackling concerns about weight and shape and prepare for set-backs to maintain gains made ( Fairburn et al., 2009 ).

MANTRA outlines a broader range of putative maintenance factors as treatment targets ( Schmidt and Treasure, 2006 ; Wade et al., 2011 ; Schmidt et al., 2012 , 2015 ; Treasure and Schmidt, 2013 ). Four core factors are included: (1) Rigid, detail-focused, and perfectionist information processing style; (2) Socioemotional difficulties (e.g., avoiding experience and expression of emotions within close relationships); (3) Positive beliefs about the value of AN; and (4) Close others exhibiting high expressed emotion or accommodating/enabling AN behaviors. These factors intensify once in a starved state, further maintaining them. The authors argue that CBT models miss important factors because key difficulties underlying EDs rarely concern ED-related themes (only 1%); with issues of interpersonal difficulties being more significant, including rejection and abandonment (42%); negative self-perception (22%); and emotional experience (20%; Sternheim et al., 2012 ).

Focal Psychodynamic Therapy employs three phases of treatment focussing upon relationships and breaking pro-anorexic belief and behavior patterns ( Zipfel et al., 2014 ). Firstly, it concentrates upon relationship building, therapeutic alliance, identifying pro-anorectic behavior/beliefs and self-esteem. Secondly, relevant relationships are examined and links with AN beliefs/behaviors made. Finally, this is transferred to everyday life and a therapeutic ending.

Specialist SSCM ( McIntosh et al., 2006 ) was originally developed as a comparison ‘treatment as usual’ for use in clinical trials. It combines clinical management and supportive psychotherapy to provide practical support and is not formulation-based; rather it focuses on psycho-education, resumption of eating and normalization of weight.

These treatment developments since the publication of previous NICE guidelines ( National Institute for Health, and Care Excellence [NICE], 2004 ) represent obvious innovation in the application of empirical research to treatment models and clinical practice. However, of these, there remains no clear front runner and it is uncertain which treatment best suits which sufferer of AN. Results from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) indicate that these speciality out-patient treatments do not out-perform each other or control comparisons post-therapy or at follow-up ( Carter et al., 2011 ; Schmidt et al., 2012 , 2015 ; Zipfel et al., 2014 ; Fairburn et al., 2015 ). They demonstrate only small non-significant effect sizes of change ( Watson and Bulik, 2013 ), with around 20% of people weight-restored after 1 year ( Schmidt et al., 2012 , 2015 ; Zipfel et al., 2014 ). Therefore, non-specific control interventions seeking to manage clinical symptoms (e.g., SSCM) appear as effective as complex, empirically driven models, prompting the inclusion of SSCM in NICE guidelines. This falls short of advances made in outcomes from interventions developed for other Axis I disorders, including CBT for depression, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, and Bulimia Nervosa ( Butler et al., 2006 ).

Anorexia Nervosa is notoriously considered ‘difficult to treat’ and, as described, treatment outcomes indicate an unexplained discrepancy between theoretical models based on empirical data findings and clinical application. It may be that even where causal models are available and appear robust, it cannot be assumed that derived interventions effectively manipulate targets. Evidence of the impact of interventions upon proposed maintenance factors is absent in the field and not understanding how change is facilitated is a barrier to developing evidence-based interventions for AN ( Pennesi and Wade, 2016 ). Furthermore, speciality interventions developed to date tend to have complex hypotheses with many diverse target variables. This potentially falls into the trap of an unhelpful ‘everything is relevant’ approach common in mental health research and results in the inclusion of many possible risk or maintenance factors into a causal model ( Kendler and Campbell, 2009 ). It produces heterogeneity across the delivery of an intervention creating difficulty in drawing links between outcomes and causal processes. In addition, while earlier models have sought to describe and address the current clinical presentation of AN, it is imperative that models offer strong theoretical bases and robust consideration of how AN has arisen; a paradigm that the ED field has not optimally employed ( Pennesi and Wade, 2016 ). Previous models may fail to sufficiently consider and adequately account for etiology and phenomenology of EDs ( Fox, 2009 ; Waller et al., 2010 ). Like others, we therefore propose that any explanation for AN must include reference to phenomenological and interpretive aspects of the presentation ( Amianto et al., 2016 ). Moreover, the definition and extrication of risk and maintenance factors is complex, especially for AN which is compounded due to its mixed psychological and physical presentation, and there is a paucity of research examining risk and maintenance factors by differential ED diagnoses (cf. Stice, 2002 ; Jacobi et al., 2004 ; Jacobi et al., 2011 ). Risk factors can be potentiated once in the ill state, or mediated by other variables and maintenance factors can be generated as a consequence of AN, perpetuating the disorder ( Wonderlich et al., 2005 ). Thus, key foci for clinical interventions are difficult to discern and may alter as the disorder progresses.

Building an explanatory developmental framework attempting to understand how factors link together to cause and maintain AN makes it unnecessary to distinguish between risk and maintenance factors and is therefore desirable. We propose that an integrative account of the emergence of risk and maintenance factors and their interplay (including how this is potentiated once AN is established) is required to gain the necessary depth of understanding of the development and presentation of AN to develop and inform interventions.

Aims of the Current Paper

The current paper offers an account of AN integrating risk and maintenance factors by proposing their influence upon development and course of AN may be explained and potentiated by an underpinning unifying explanatory variable of emotional difficulties, giving rise to AN as a disorder of a ‘lost sense of emotional self.’

First, we describe the difficulties with emotion experienced by people with AN forming the basis of our argument of AN as a ‘lost sense of emotional self’ (see the section “Emotional Difficulties in AN”). We present how emotional difficulties link several known risk and maintenance factors for AN throughout development (see the section “Development of AN”). We describe how, once developed, AN becomes self-perpetuating within this context adding to persistence and complexity of disorder and treatment (see the section “Perpetuation of AN: The Ever Decreasing ‘Self”’). Once this core presentation to treatment is outlined (a ‘lost sense of emotional self’), we offer considerations for clinical conceptualization and practice and hypothesis testing (see the sections “Clinical Conceptualization and Practice” and “Future Directions in Testing the ‘Lost Emotional Self’ Hypothesis”).

Emotional Difficulties in AN

Emotions evolved as adaptive processes: they are learnt or instinctive responses to external or internal stimuli, informing about immediate environments, relationships and our needs, with critical impact upon physiology, behavior and cognition, including memory and decision-making capabilities ( Dolan, 2002 ). They evolved to organize and direct human cognitions and behavior ( Cosmides and Tooby, 2000 ). Emotions act as a super-ordinate system; we describe them as the conductor of an orchestra comprised of cognitive, behavioral, physiological and social functions ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ).

Dysfunctional emotional processing and regulation are proposed to underpin many psychological disorders ( Aldao et al., 2010 ), including EDs ( Aldao et al., 2010 ; Hatch et al., 2010b ; Arcelus et al., 2013 ; Lavender et al., 2015 ; Oldershaw et al., 2015 ; Mallorqui-Bague et al., 2018 ). They play a significant role in the development and maintenance of AN ( Schmidt and Treasure, 2006 ; Haynos and Fruzzetti, 2011 ; Treasure and Schmidt, 2013 ; Wildes et al., 2014 ; Racine and Wildes, 2015 ). Indeed, even earliest descriptions recognize emotional experience as a factor in AN development and maintenance. In 1871, a women with AN was reported to “suffer from some emotions she avows or conceals” (Charles Lasègue cited in Vandereycken and Van Deth, 1990 ). A century later, women with AN were described as having underlying deficiencies in the identification of emotional states and responses ( Bruch, 1962 ). A link between emotional experience and the behavioral expression of AN is clear: potential emotion is avoided by eliciting predictable and controllable behavioral patterns from others ( Treasure et al., 2016 ); focus on food, eating, weight, and shape, as well as cognitive processes such as worry and rumination, affords cognitive distraction from negative thoughts/emotion ( Sternheim et al., 2011 ; Startup et al., 2013 ). Once starved, suppressed physiological experience numbs emotions and is valued ( Miller et al., 2003 , 2009 ; Serpell et al., 2004 ); while starvation and emaciation enables a maladaptive expression of distress ( Serpell et al., 2004 ) and ever-narrowing interpersonal life fuels greater reliance on AN ( Schmidt and Treasure, 2006 ).

Systematic reviews of emotional experience in AN expand on this understanding by providing summaries of experimental and self- report data ( Oldershaw et al., 2011 , 2015 ; Lavender et al., 2015 ), integrating pre-existing theory from the field of emotion regulation ( Aldao et al., 2010 ; Gross, 2013 ). The reviews indicate that, relative to healthy controls, people with AN experience difficulties in emotional awareness (including alexithymia and poor emotional clarity), alongside high self-reported levels of disgust and shame specifically. Difficulties in meta-level processes are also evident, such as elevated maladaptive schema, particularly around defectiveness, dependence/incompetence, social isolation and subjugation, and negative beliefs about having or expressing emotion.

Engagement in many emotion regulation strategies is evident, indicative of a pattern of emotion over-regulation ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ) ( Figure 2 ). Adaptive emotion regulation strategies are absent, and unhelpful strategies dominant, including: avoiding emotion triggers, such as situations or modifying social interaction (e.g., submissiveness); worry and rumination processes; and emotion suppression, particularly to avoid conflict. It is argued that people with AN are disproportionately reliant upon the feedback of others for reassurance and regulation of emotion (e.g., via social comparison). These findings support the notion that emotional avoidance and unhelpful over-regulation strategies play a central role in AN, including those achieved via anorexic behaviors ( Dolhanty and Greenberg, 2009 ; Wildes et al., 2010 ; Brockmeyer et al., 2012 ; Arcelus et al., 2013 ; Treasure and Schmidt, 2013 ). Indeed, emotional avoidance and submissive behaviors (and not social cognition or neurocognition) are more promising predictors of clinical outcomes following treatment ( Oldershaw et al., 2018 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Emotional Avoidance as a Maintenance Factor for AN

In summary, following many other authors highlighting the key role of emotional avoidance in AN (e.g., Wildes et al., 2010 , 2014 ; Treasure and Schmidt, 2013 ), experimental and self-report findings point to a maintenance model of AN as a disorder underpinned by difficulties with emotional experience promoting emotional avoidance and over-regulation ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ). This model posits that early life factors develop schemata and beliefs that leave somebody vulnerable to experiencing emotion as overwhelming and confusing. Emotion regulation strategies, including ED behaviors, develop in this context as a means to control and prevent triggering emotion. Strategies developed are perceived as useful in the first instance, generating an initial reduction in emotion. Ultimately, however, they are maladaptive methods of controlling and regulating emotion serving only to trigger further negative emotional experience and reinforce negative beliefs and schemata, thereby increasing reliance upon (maladaptive) emotion regulation strategies: hence a vicious cycle develops ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ).

Emotional Avoidance and the Impact on Development of Self in AN

In the current paper, we consider the implications of this persistent emotional avoidance cycle and seek to further this as an explanation by considering both how it arises and its consequences. We will argue that this picture of emotional avoidance and over-regulation is shaped by and further impacts development of self. As such, we will propose that it is not simply a difficulty with emotional experience, interpersonal experience or emotion regulation that is observed and should be tackled in treatment, but the consequential impact of this on development and awareness of a core ‘emotional self.’

We posit that difficulties with emotion are so pervasive due to their integral part of our existence: the basis of the self and emotion are shared and inextricably linked ( Damasio, 2003 ). Primary emotional experience emerges from physical arousal, including interoceptive awareness, in addition to complex processes such as neural activation ( Barrett et al., 2007 ) and emotional memory networks ( Greenberg, 2004 ). Interoceptive emotion signals afford us a mental representation of selfhood experienced within the body – “a material me” ( Seth, 2013 ). Through such processes, emotion becomes fundamental to the construction and organization of ‘self’ with largely bottom-up interoceptive hierarchies of emotional experience interacting to motivate and direct behavior ( Damasio, 2003 ; Greenberg, 2004 ). Thus, there is a process of non-conscious emotional experience, giving rise to conscious thinking and feeling states, which are self-regulated ( Hatch et al., 2010b ).

‘Self’ is the overarching concept used to describe the organization and integration of our many identities; who I am within each relationship and every social interaction ( Haworth-Hoeppner and Manies, 2005 ). It is argued that our identity is ‘rooted in emotion, emerging in relationships, developing as a dynamic self-organizing system’ ( Bosma and Kunnen, 2001 , p. 5). If we cannot access those bottom-up emotional hierarchies (“a material me”), we are without the conductor of the orchestra (our emotional sense of self). If the conductor is not functioning or appears absent, the orchestra descends into disorder. It must muddle through as best it can, desperate to conceal in-fighting, misattunement and confusion, while persistently reliant upon and sensitive to audience feedback to ascertain if it is doing an adequate job.

People with AN lack interoceptive perception of their own body and the internal bodily sensations which give rise to the basic form of self-awareness and emotion perception ( Gaudio et al., 2014 ; Stanghellini et al., 2015 ). It is suggested that AN emerges in this context of vague and overwhelming emotional experience both to regulate emotion ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ) and to regain a sense of bodily self, defined by the other’s gaze and external appearance ( Monteleone et al., 2017 ). Indeed, shape and weight concerns are thought to arise as the result of a disturbance in experience of one’s own body (embodiment) and are influential in how personal identity is determined ( Stanghellini et al., 2012 ). People with AN have described the disorder as a means of forging a new identity ( Nordbo et al., 2006 ) which becomes inexplicably linked with, or replaces, the ‘true’ self ( Williams et al., 2016 ). As such, AN has been referred to as a “false self” ( Bruch, 1988 ), arising due to an otherwise unstable and fragile self ( Karwautz et al., 2001b ) unable to integrate the body ( Amianto et al., 2016 ). In this paper, we further define this difficulty with self by arguing that AN arises specifically from a lost emotional sense of self . That is, a person struggling to navigate the world, themselves and others, without an emotional conductor to guide them, increasingly reliant upon feedback of others.

Development of AN

Here, we build a picture of the development of AN, moving from infancy through to adolescence, seeking to offer an explanatory developmental framework for the emergence of AN in the context of a ‘lost emotional self,’ drawing on identified risk and maintenance factors, and highlighting how they are unified by being both influential upon and potentiated by emotional difficulties.

Infancy and Early Life

Attunement and attachment.

Difficulties with emotional awareness and regulation in EDs are hypothesized to have their origins in childhood attachment ( Arcelus et al., 2013 ). At the earliest stage of infancy, maternal reflective functioning and attunement are critical to emotional development. Reflective functioning is caregiver capacity to hold her child’s emotional and mental states in mind. Attunement is caregiver responsiveness and ability to identify, model and name emotional experiences for a child. They are core to attachment, which enables the capacity to identify the feelings of self and others and to integrate them into a felt emotional experience, forming a template for self and others in close relationships (‘internal working models’; Bowlby, 1969 ). The mother acts as container for her child’s unbearable feelings, such as anger and distress, with her responses shaping the child’s future responses to emotions ( Bion, 1962 ). Through this process, emerging emotional awareness enables a gradual articulation of self-experience ( de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ) and differentiation ( Mahler, 1963 ). This requires a “goodness-of-fit” between maternal and infant temperament ( Belsky, 1984 ). When present, adequate mutual regulation of interactions and communication of needs and attention are possible. Yet this is an extremely sensitive process; even incongruence as early as 8 months old can impair neural discrimination of emotional facial expressions ( Rajhans et al., 2016 ).

Over 35 years ago, Bruch advocated a role of attachment and early social experiences in the development of AN ( Treasure and Cardi, 2017 ). Insecure patterns of attachment of people with AN to primary caregivers have been reported in adults ( Zachrisson and Skarderud, 2010 ; Tasca and Balfour, 2014 ) and adolescents ( Gander et al., 2015 ; Jewell et al., 2016 ; Pace et al., 2016 ), of large effect ( Caglar-Nazali et al., 2014 ). Further, people with AN have particularly low levels of reflective functioning ( Tasca and Balfour, 2014 ). This matches a similar pattern of attachment in the mothers of those with AN ( Ward et al., 2001 ; Pace et al., 2015 ). Higher attachment anxiety is significantly related to greater ED severity and poorer treatment outcomes ( Illing et al., 2010 ). A discrepancy in the fit between environment and the temperament of the individual who develops AN is proposed to lead to an experience of social environments as invalidating or demanding ( Karwautz et al., 2001a , b ; Corstorphine et al., 2007 ; Oldershaw et al., 2015 ). Chronic misattunement (“empathic failure”) is argued to affect the development of any or all sense of self and relate to ED development ( de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ).

AN is more prevalent for females than for males with ratio of 1:8 ( Steinhausen and Jensen, 2015 ). Female preponderance of EDs is often hypothesized to reflect cultural ideals, particularly internalization of the thin ideal and objectification of women. Yet this fails to adequately address the complexity of gender and gender role socialization. Gender differences in emotion expression arise as a complex combination of biological determinants, socialization, social context and cultural expectations ( Chaplin, 2015 ). We argue that gender socialization and social construction of emotion expectations from infancy plays a role in women becoming particularly at risk of developing an ED.

From an early age, parents socialize emotional expression based on gender stereotypes or expectations; encouraging internalizing emotions such as sadness, fear, shame and guilt and discouraging anger expression in girls, while the opposite is observed for boys ( Zeman et al., 2012 ). Girls are expected to display a greater array of emotions than boys, including positive emotions ( Brody and Hall, 2008 ), yet, perhaps due to biological differences, need more encouragement to learn to express their emotion than boys ( Chaplin, 2015 ). Consequently, girls have been observed to display more internalizing emotions than boys, with the strongest differences found for fear and shame ( Chaplin and Aldao, 2013 ). High levels of internalizing difficulties and emotions (particularly shame) are prevalent for people with AN ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ).

The ‘self-in-relation’ (capacity for identification with and relatedness to the mother) is considered more important to female identity than male ( Gilligan, 1982 ). Miscoordinated interactions with the caregiver leads to a sense of ineffectiveness more prone with mother–daughter than with mother–son dyads ( Tronick and Cohn, 1989 ). Mothers see daughters as a continuation of themselves more than they do sons, thus affecting the extent to which girls are experienced as separate and individual ( de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ). Further, girls in particular may base their emotional relatedness on parental feelings rather than their own emotion; facilitating intimacy with the mother, but interfering with girls’ experience of their own unique emotional world ( de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ). Indeed, women can find it difficult to define themselves without first contextualizing ‘self’ within the mother–daughter relationship ( Miller-Day, 2012 ). In general, girls feel less powerful in parental relationships than boys ( Buhl, 2008 ). Thus, females value interconnected social and emotional experience more than males, but at the expense of developing their own unique emotional self-awareness and experience. Such patterns are observed in higher levels with people with AN. They report lower perceived individual autonomy and higher perceived cohesion in their relationship with their mother than unaffected sisters, although have similar perceived emotional connectedness ( Karwautz et al., 2003 ).

Females have greater innate empathy than males, linked not only to socialization, but resulting from evolutionary processes creating neurobiological differences ( Christov-Moore et al., 2014 ). Girls are expected to offer more empathy and sympathy than boys, and are socialized to this end ( Knafo et al., 2008 ). Girls are more sensitive than boys to the responses of others, including both approval and rejection, with girls’ need for approval superseding their striving for autonomy ( McClure, 2000 ). As such, females may consider their own subjective experience or evaluations as less valid than those of others. Indeed, it has been argued that mothers (consciously and unconsciously) direct their daughters in “gender appropriate ways” including offering the message their own “needs” and certain emotions are unacceptable ( Orbach, 1993 cited in Richmond, 2002 ). This can include modeling for girls “feminine” emotion expression such as appearing cheerful, even where this is not felt ( Chaplin, 2015 ). Girls demonstrate greater suppression of emotions, particularly in relation to their own goals or needs (e.g., disappointment; Saarni, 1984 ).

This presents an image of girls as sensitive to the feelings of others, likely to suppress their own emotions and needs, and to experience the self via the other (mother). These features may be heightened for people who go on to develop AN. People with AN report low levels of their own emotional awareness, alexithymia (inability to identify and describe their own feelings), limited expression of their own emotion and submissiveness, especially to avoid conflict ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ). They suppress facial expression of emotion and use fewer words when describing their emotional experiences as compared with healthy people or those recovered from AN ( Davies et al., 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ). They thus inhibit the expression of their own emotion ( Gramaglia et al., 2016 ) and autonomy ( Karwautz et al., 2003 ) while seeming able to acknowledge others’ emotions. Individuals with AN during both starvation and weight restoration report greater empathy than healthy controls ( Hambrook et al., 2008 ), including in the domain of personal distress (vicarious negative arousal to others’ suffering; Beadle et al., 2013 ). They appear highly motivated to understand the feelings of others, ‘hyperscanning’ stimuli relating to others while avoiding visually attending to salient features of their own facial image ( Phillipou et al., 2015 ). They have low self-directedness, scoring highly on ‘Other-Directedness’ ( Bachner-Melman et al., 2009 ). People currently with AN have significantly less self-focussed attention than those who are weight restored ( Zucker et al., 2015 ), while tending to self-attribute negative over positive social interactions ( McAdams et al., 2018 ). Perhaps unsurprisingly then, adolescents with disordered eating devalue personal subjective experience, and favor socially accepted externally validated ideals (e.g., “thin ideal”) as their own ( Steiner-Adair, 1990 , cited in de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ).

It is of note that the male incidence of AN has increased in recent decades, in the absence of any such increase in bulimia nervosa ( Steinhausen and Jensen, 2015 ). Attitudes toward gender roles have shifted substantially since the 1980s, including toward roles in relationships, parenting, and women’s participation in the labor market ( Park et al., 2013 ). It is possible that changes in gender role norms, as well as in emotion socialization may have ensued, including changes in mother–son dyad relationships; further research is required to investigate this. We propose that any such shifts could in part relate to this increase in male AN presentations.

Food and Communication

Effectively feeding one’s children is of innate importance, essential for well-being and survival. Up to 52% of infants and toddlers are viewed by caregivers as experiencing feeding problems ( Reau et al., 1996 ). Even feeding healthy children can be time-consuming, tedious and exasperating: up to 15 exposures of a new food can be required before it is trusted enough to even be tasted ( Wardle et al., 2005 ). Moreover, around age two, children undergo a developmental change, causing them to reject foods previously liked and accepted ( Cooke et al., 2003 ) triggering potential frustration and anxiety for parents. Emotions and mealtimes are strongly linked, and although largely positive in valence, negative emotions such as disgust and guilt are aroused in subgroups described as “indifferent restrictives” ( den Uijl et al., 2014 ). Indeed, adults’ food preferences and emotions can be traced back to early negative experiences such as pressure to eat ( Batsell et al., 2002 ), with less pressure predicting food enjoyment ( Webber et al., 2010 ). Moreover, EDs have been retrospectively linked to greater use of food for communication and emotion regulatory purposes, such as rewarding, comforting, or punishing ( Mazzeo and Bulik, 2009 ). Thus, even preverbally, children learn to associate food with feeling misunderstood, stress and anxiety. Further, using food and eating as communication and for emotion regulation may be fostered and is observed in girls as young as five ( Carper et al., 2000 ).

People who develop AN have increased early life experiences generating sensitivity to and preoccupation with somatic experience, including early gastrointestinal events and eating difficulties, with evidence pointing to digestive problems as a risk factor for AN ( Marchi and Cohen, 1990 ; Jacobi et al., 2004 ). Once people develop AN and caregivers become concerned, food offers a way to communicate needs valued by the person with AN ( Serpell et al., 2004 ). Emotion expressed around eating, and engagement with the ED by parents, may represent an emotional engagement that is reinforcing, but ultimately leaves the sufferer feeling overwhelmed and misunderstood in the moment serving only to remove them further from developing an awareness and acceptance of authentic internal experiences.

Infancy and Early Life: Summary

People who develop AN may be those with poor fit in their environment, leading to poor attunement and insecure attachment. Consequently they may feel invalidated as a separate, unique person, and be left with a sense that their own emotions and needs are less important than those of others and should be suppressed. It follows that this could result in poor emotional awareness and regulation, in the context of attempts at pleasing or meeting the emotional needs of others. Even from an early age, food and mealtimes may represent periods of heightened emotion and emotional communication for both parent and child.

Adolescence

Anorexia Nervosa can affect people of any age, gender, culture; however, adolescent and young adult females are most at risk ( Zipfel et al., 2014 ). AN typically emerges in early- to mid-adolescence ( Herpertz-Dahlmann, 2009 ), highlighting adolescence as a risk factor in its development. Adolescence is a challenging period emotionally and socially with consequences for emotion regulation and psychological adjustment ( Miller-Slough and Dunsmore, 2016 ). It is a critical period for developing identity ( Pfeifer and Berkman, 2018 ) associated with emergence of self-concept and enhanced self-awareness ( Weil et al., 2013 ). The ‘self’ develops driven by a key intersection of social, cognitive, affective, motivational, and regulatory processes ( Pfeifer et al., 2013 ). As such, adolescence is a phase of life representing an opportunity, yet also a vulnerability ( Fuhrmann et al., 2015 ).

Parents and Identity

Identity development during adolescence has roots in the parent–child relationship. Separation–individuation factors such as autonomy-supportive parenting, separateness from parents, and personal autonomy are crucial ( Luyckx et al., 2006 , 2007 ; Beyers and Goossens, 2008 ). Early developmental interactions with parents, and whether they met identity needs of parent or child, can have implications for identity development and separation–individuation in adolescence ( Koepke and Denissen, 2012 ). Autonomy supporting parents promote a belief in personal agency by enabling expression of identity and opinion without fear of parental rejection or engulfment ( Luyckx et al., 2007 ). To fully engage in adult-to-adult relationships with grown up children, parents need to let go of the part of their own identity of “omnipresent caretaker” ( Koepke and Denissen, 2012 ). And yet, emotion socialization continues to rely on parents. The need to offer appropriate emotional guidance is enhanced by new social demands and risks ( Garcia and Scherf, 2015 ). Thus, adolescence is a challenging time of adaptation for parents, alongside their child.

Separation–individuation processes during adolescence may be particularly problematic for people who develop AN, and may follow from interactions during early life described above. Women with AN identify boundary violations by their parents within the family ( Rowa et al., 2001 ). Adolescents with AN rate their families as less communicative, flexible, cohesive, and more disengaged, compared to control participants ( Laghi et al., 2017 ). Maternal criticism and emotional over-involvement links to ED severity ( Kyriacou et al., 2008 ; Duclos et al., 2014 ). High levels of expressed emotion is included in maintenance models of AN and reflects a description of families as critical, hostile and/or emotionally over involved and overprotective, particularly toward the person with AN ( Schmidt and Treasure, 2006 ; Treasure and Schmidt, 2013 ). Thus, there is a traditional view that families of people with AN are enmeshed and rigid in their style ( Minuchin et al., 1978 ). This fits with the presentation described: a person with AN unsure of their own internal world, seeking to meet the needs of others over their own. Indeed, adolescents with EDs have lower levels of self-differentiation, indicating high emotional reactivity, emotional cut-off, and greater fusion with others, causing confusion between one’s own emotional and mental states and those of others ( Doba et al., 2018 ). Consequently, people with AN report a longing for independence seen as a reaction to helplessness ( Karwautz et al., 2001b ). Taking such an oppositional stance by refusing to eat in the face of desperate parental persuasion may reflect a need for adversarial transference ( de Groot and Rodin, 1994 ), thus, enabling growth of the person with AN and a sense of separateness through assertion of opposition. The instinct for individuation may also be a factor in seeking external validity of their worth via social reinforcement from others.

Although, some of these relational patterns can be observed in earlier childhood, there may be increased consequences during the adolescent period, when identity is struggling to emerge, and there are also shifts in the most adaptive response to emotion. Parents therefore need to adjust to this developmental phase, shifting in their socialization of emotion and demonstrating flexibility in emotional responding. In infancy and early childhood, focussing on the child’s emotional experience functions to encourage recognition and labeling of emotion supporting basic emotion knowledge and self-regulation ( Sanders et al., 2015 ). By contrast, during adolescence, encouraging excessive focus on emotions, particularly via increased expression of parental reactive emotion or parental matching of emotion, can prolong a negative emotional state resulting in emotion dysregulation and psychopathological risk ( Brand and Klimes-Dougan, 2010 ; Moed et al., 2015 ). Reassurance or distraction to a positive activity is a more adaptive response better alleviating distress. Yet, magnifying is more commonly used for girls than boys and there is a more robust link between magnifying responses and adolescent psychological problems for girls over boys ( Klimes-Dougan et al., 2014 ). Indeed, girls show higher rates of depression and anxiety than boys, starting in adolescence, and which is associated with internalizing negative emotions of sadness, guilt, and fear ( Chaplin, 2015 ). High expressivity of parental negative emotion relates to internalizing symptoms, depression and anxiety in adolescents ( Suveg et al., 2008 ; Luebbe and Bell, 2014 ), all of which are associated with development of AN ( Adambegan et al., 2012 ). High levels of parental emotion dysregulation are associated with invalidation of adolescent’s emotional expression, and in turn results in adolescent’s emotion dysregulation ( Buckholdt et al., 2014 ). Equally, by being dismissive of emotion, parents can transmit beliefs that emotions are dangerous or invalid and need to be suppressed ( Morris et al., 2007 ). These beliefs are reported by adults with AN ( Hambrook et al., 2011 ). Therefore, parental emotion socialization is found to be crucial to emotion regulation and adolescent psychological outcomes ( Miller-Slough and Dunsmore, 2016 ), and appears to have a higher cost for female adolescents, and with direct relevance to the development of AN.

Parents enabling the emergence of a child’s own narrative voice is fundamental to development of healthy identity ( Fivush and Zaman, 2015 ). The stories adolescents know about their parents (intergenerational narratives) are critical for understanding self ( Fivush and Zaman, 2015 ). Story telling narratives offer a coherent sense of temporal, autobiographical self, within which discrete experiences can be embedded and understood ( Habermas and Bluck, 2000 ). This is scaffolded by story-telling with parents during early years ( McLean and Jennings, 2012 ). Development of narrative identity is also subject to gender differences. Mothers are especially important in helping their adolescent child to construct narratives around emotion and vulnerability (being overpowered by negative emotion) ( McLean and Mansfield, 2012 ). Mothers employ more emotion words and discuss the causes of emotion when reminiscing with their children than fathers ( Fivush et al., 2000 ). Moreover, both mothers and fathers use more emotion words when constructing narratives with preschool daughters than sons, focusing on elaboration of emotions such as sadness and social-relational themes, with mothers–daughter dyads being most emotionally expressive ( Reese and Newcombe, 2007 ). Meanwhile, adolescent boys receive more supportive scaffolding from mothers than girls do ( McLean and Mansfield, 2012 ). The ability of children and their parents to tell detailed stories about negative emotional past events, cognitive-processing and emotion words, are related to adolescent well-being ( Fivush et al., 2008 ).

It is possible that there is diminished parental scaffolding of emotional narratives and narrative identity is for those who develop AN. It is known that people with AN use fewer words when describing their emotional experiences compared with healthy people or those recovered from AN ( Davies et al., 2011 , 2012 , 2013 ). People with AN recall over-generalized autobiographical memories, reflecting lack in ability to integrate positive and negative emotional experiences, and which worsens with disorder duration ( Nandrino et al., 2006 ). Memories are associated with less negative emotion expression than healthy people, especially at a lower weight ( Brockmeyer et al., 2013 ). This may reflect poorer narrative identity within which emotions and emotional self cannot be embedded, further impacting difficulties in individuation and self-differentiation.

Peers and Identity

During adolescence, emotional and social pressures significantly increase, alongside volume and complexity of social experience. Friendships grow in salience as young people seek out peers for emotional support and explore their identity outside of the family context ( Jobe-Shields et al., 2014 ). Peer interactions become more influential and their quality reflects that of early attachments ( Lieberman et al., 1999 ). Adolescence is therefore considered a time of ‘social reorientation’ ( Meeus et al., 2005 ; Nelson et al., 2016 ). Influence of peers disproportionately affects behaviors such as risk-taking and relational reasoning during adolescence ( Wolf et al., 2015 ). Adolescents are highly attuned to peer evaluation ( Somerville, 2013 ). They are hypersensitive to social exclusion, particularly in younger adolescence ( Sebastian et al., 2010 ), with older adolescents especially fearful of peer evaluation ( Westenberg et al., 2004 ). There is also acceleration of gender-differentiation and an intensification of gender role expectations ( Hill and Lynch, 1983 ).

Early emotional difficulties outlined above, such as alexithymia, emotion acceptance and regulation, alongside suppression of own emotion and needs, may make an individual particularly vulnerable during adolescence when peer relationships and social acceptance become so vital. Social cognition (mental processes underlying human social behavior and interaction; ( Adolphs, 2001 ) is crucial in successfully negotiating complex social interactions and decisions ( Crone, 2013 ). However, the social brain network undergoes protracted development throughout adolescence before stabilizing in the mid-twenties ( Giedd et al., 1999 ; Gogtay et al., 2004 ; Sowell et al., 2004 ; Barnea-Goraly et al., 2005 ; Shaw et al., 2008 ). Capacity for introspection or ‘metacognition’ (reflection on our thoughts and behaviors) begins to slowly emerge and gradually improve throughout adolescence ( Weil et al., 2013 ). Younger adolescents are relatively focused on self-oriented choices; impulse control and perspective taking afford increased consideration of consequences for others later in adolescence and early adulthood ( Crone, 2013 ). Further, change in neural and hormonal activity impacts social cognitive abilities, with attachment and mentalization (identifying or inferring mental states of self and others) appearing to enter a state of flux ( Jewell et al., 2016 ). At puberty (around age 11), performance dips on some social cognitive abilities, such as facial emotion recognition and perspective taking, before gradually recovering; a process thought reflect the sudden proliferation of synapses at puberty which are pruned during adolescence ( Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006 ).

People with AN may have impaired emotion recognition and theory of mind (of small to medium effect), that precede AN development, and are further exacerbated by secondary consequences of starvation ( Oldershaw et al., 2011 ; Caglar-Nazali et al., 2014 ; Bora and Kose, 2016 ). Difficulties recognizing emotion from blended facial expressions ( Dapelo et al., 2016 ), tone of voice ( Kucharska-Pietura et al., 2004 ; Oldershaw et al., 2010 ), body movement ( Lang et al., 2015 ) and affective touch ( Crucianelli et al., 2016 ) are reported. A drop in these already potential (trait) difficulties during adolescence is likely to cause distress and increase reliance on external feedback, while simultaneously decreasing ability to define self as a ‘self-in-relation,’ increasing an already impoverished sense of self. Further, identity and other associated self-related processes become a source of information with which to shape decision-making and intrinsic motivations across adolescence ( Pfeifer and Berkman, 2018 ), in the context of strong extrinsic social forces ( Wolf et al., 2015 ). In the absence of a clear developing self, it is possible that extrinsic motivations (e.g., thin ideal; others’ needs) become further reinforced as the key driving force of decisions and behaviors. Thus eating, weight and shape cognitions begin to emerge and drive behavior.

Adolescence and Body Image

During adolescence physical shape changes, and awareness of and focus on the body heightens. Adolescents are influenced by and fearful of peer exclusion and evaluation, and this likely includes body and image valuations. EDs are associated with higher family standards on physical appearance ( Gunnard et al., 2012 ). Perfectionism, eating and weight concern are observed at higher levels in mothers of people with AN than in comparison groups ( Woodside et al., 2002 ). The link between insecure (both anxious and avoidant) attachment and EDs is largely mediated by emotion dysregulation, including social comparison ( Dias et al., 2011 ; Ty and Francis, 2013 ). High externalized self-perceptions and attributions about the importance of weight and shape for popularity and dating by adolescent girls predicts body esteem and eating behavior ( Lieberman et al., 2001 ).

As shape changes, an adolescent vulnerable to AN may receive comments from family or peers that are perceived as external validation. They may seek to manipulate their body through dieting resulting in its conditioned positive reinforcement ( Walsh, 2013 ). Adolescents are particularly sensitive to reward ( Steinberg, 2004 ), with sensitivity to reward and punishment even greater for those at risk of AN ( Harrison et al., 2010 ; Jappe et al., 2011 ). Initial weight loss is often met with compliments (extrinsic reinforcement); a validation that for a person with the early life experiences described above may finally feel like an achievable goal that is self-motivated (perceived intrinsic achievement).

Adolescence: Summary

Adolescence is a time of increased sensitivity to peer rejection and evaluation. It is expected that emotion socialization in infancy and early-mid childhood will afford a child with the foundational skills in emotion recognition, regulation, and expression with which to meet the social and emotional challenges of adolescence. Self-reflection and identity emerging during adolescence is built upon experience and abilities that have been developing since early childhood ( Reese et al., 2010 ). For somebody entering this life stage with poor emotion awareness and a need to please others, such challenges become magnified and may be further exacerbated by familial patterns. The emergence of identity within this context is especially likely to hinge on sources of external evaluation and validation. Thus, poorly integrated identity is built, which, in light of the pervasive ‘thin ideal’ within Western society and increased bodily attention due to puberty, may make somebody particularly drawn to a concrete externally validated sense of self. AN becomes a means to better oneself ( Bates, 2015 ) and to find validity and direction in the face of expectations of what is perceived others want and which is seen as functional and valued.

Perpetuation of AN: the Ever Decreasing ‘Self’

One of the challenges in working with people with AN is that, once established, there appears an ever-tightening vicious cycle. Increased severity links to increased positive egosyntonic beliefs about the value of AN ( Serpell et al., 2004 ). In many ways, AN becomes self-perpetuating due to its reinforcement of social, emotional and behavioral patterns and the impacts of starvation itself. We argue that these factors further distance an individual from an ‘emotional self’ and thus from recovery; hence AN becomes ‘self-perpetuating’ in and of itself, beyond risk and maintenance factors.

Emotion Over-Regulation and Perpetuation of Interpersonal Patterns

As discussed throughout the Section “Development of AN,” low levels of expression and high emotion suppression are observed amongst people with AN, including in the context of avoiding conflict ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ). The impact of emotion suppression in healthy people is increased negative feeling, decreased positive feeling, and decreased emotion expressivity, cardiovascular activity, and oxygenation ( Gross and Levenson, 1997 ; Dan-Glauser and Gross, 2011 ). Thus, emotion suppression paradoxically leaves somebody with greater negative emotion to regulate, reinforcing their need for emotion regulation strategies. Suppression elicited in experiments with healthy participants reduces interpersonal responsiveness during face-to-face interaction, increasing negative partner-perceptions and hostile behavior ( Butler et al., 2007 ). When watching film clips, people with AN report stimuli incongruent emotions, with limited facial expression ( Lang et al., 2016 ). Inaccurate signaling predicts higher levels of depression and lower levels of well-being, mediated by social connectedness, since accurately signaling emotion states enhances social connectedness ( Mauss et al., 2011 ). Therefore, suppressing emotions, reducing emotion expression and displaying emotions incongruent with those felt, impedes the congruent responding of others. This could perpetuate ‘attunement’ difficulties, echoing those proposed in childhood, further invalidating a true sense of self. Indeed, even when others actively seek to attune to the person with AN, it may paradoxically decrease social connectedness. AN is associated with high levels of internal shame ( Grabhorn et al., 2006 ). People with high levels of internalized shame display shame even following positive task feedback intended to elicit positive emotion ( Claesson et al., 2007 ). Thus, people with AN may experience negative emotions in response to situations more commonly viewed as positive. Further, they have difficulty in distinguishing between positive and negative feedback and report social anhedonia that relates to disorder severity and alexithymia, even following recovery ( Wagner et al., 2007 ; Tchanturia et al., 2012 ).

There may therefore be numerous ways in which the expected response from others (on both sides) is misinterpreted; social interaction and connection becomes constantly further misattuned. This increases negative emotional experience, particularly shame, and only makes the need for emotional regulation strategies, including social reinforcement, feel more urgent: unhelpful cycles are strengthened, AN becomes embedded and helpful regulation strategies cannot be developed. This fits with reports that people with AN feel they have insufficient emotion regulation strategies available ( Harrison et al., 2009 ; Oldershaw et al., 2015 ). Actual success in emotion regulation follows expected success ( Bigman et al., 2016 ) further limiting potential emotion regulation abilities, stifling validation of felt emotional experience and cues.

Starvation Effects

Once AN has developed, starvation effects contribute to self-perpetuation of AN. Starvation reinforces existing psychological difficulties and underpins development of new difficulties. Starvation alone can trigger a reinforcing feedback loop arousing evolved physiological mechanisms, triggering urges to binge eat, increased metabolic efficiency and fat storage, reinforcing fear of fatness and generating renewed attempts at restriction ( Nesse, 2017 ).

Interoception

Starvation perpetuates difficulties via its influence on physiological feedback from the body. People with AN struggle to discriminate between bodily sensations ( Skǎrderud, 2007 ), with limited access to physiological experience of emotion when underweight ( Miller et al., 2003 ). Interoceptive cues are abnormally interpreted, resulting in erroneous judgments about internal bodily states ( Kaye et al., 2009 ) and an imbalance between external and internal perception of body relating to ED symptomatology severity ( Eshkevari et al., 2014 ). Thus, there is a disconnection between cognitive and physiological information ( Nandrino et al., 2012 ). There may also be high levels of internal incongruence, with reported emotion mismatching physiological arousal and reactivity ( Oldershaw et al., 2011 ; Nandrino et al., 2012 ). Low weight further lessens physiological experience of emotion, despite increased self-reported arousal ( Miller et al., 2009 ). This results in interoceptive confusion and the experience of emotions as vague and overwhelming. Such poor interoceptive awareness is increasingly recognized as a core feature of AN, contributing to its emergence and maintenance ( Kaye et al., 2009 ; Nandrino et al., 2012 ; Strigo et al., 2013 ).

Interoceptive awareness does not simply inform us of our emotion experience, but of our sense of self; thus disrupted interoceptive processes and feedback, deepened by starvation, further damages emotional sense of self for people with AN. This sense of self is generated, not only by the bottom-up process of interoceptive emotion signals, but also a counter flow of top-down feedback on predictions of their outcomes (such as emotional validation or reflection by others); this establishes accuracy of bottom-up signals and highlights prediction errors ( Seth, 2013 ). Where there is a discrepancy, for example, where what we feel and observe (external feedback) are not highly correlated, we are driven to reduce the discrepancy and revise our predictive model accordingly ( Talsma, 2015 ). This is achieved by altering our sense of internal or external perception. For example, by updating our internal sense of emotional experience to match the one mirrored back to us or updating predictions of the environment in future, with greater reliance on seemingly more precise data (e.g., visual over proprioception/interoception; Seth et al., 2011 ). It is argued that interoceptive signals in people with EDs are insufficient to accurately predict consequences ( Riva and Dakanalis, 2018 ) or integrate emotional information with sensory experience ( Nunn et al., 2008 ). Even where one does act on one’s felt emotional sense, as described, a lack of attunement may result in non-corresponding feedback. For these reasons, people with AN may be subject to large prediction errors, furthering disruption of interoceptive emotion information and encouraging adjusting or quietening interoceptive feedback (even if felt) in favor of external feedback or heightening desire to manipulate external feedback (e.g., via submissiveness). Inability to integrate or reconcile predictions and confirmations causes persistent anxiety and sense of uncertainty. In this case, minimizing the discrepancy can lead to incorporation of the external signal as part of the self-representation, especially for people with low interoceptive sensitivity ( Seth, 2013 ). Self-objectification (experiencing one’s body via the perspective of an external observer) significantly predicts onset and maintenance of EDs over other more commonly proposed factors (e.g., dieting, body dissatisfaction; Dakanalis et al., 2017 ).

This hypothesis fits with observed patterns of anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty and over reliance on emotion regulation strategies (via continual checking, worry/rumination processes, submissiveness and social reassurance seeking). It further invalidates internal emotional experience and sense of self. Disrupted interoceptive awareness due to suppression and starvation effects once AN is established may result in an inability to update bodily memory or representation, in part explaining persistent belief in ‘fatness’ or lack of ‘insight’ into the physical severity of the disorder, even when the body becomes dangerously emaciated ( Riva and Dakanalis, 2018 ).

The Starved Brain

Abnormalities in brain functioning are reported in AN while ill, and following recovery ( Schmidt and Campbell, 2013 ). Findings of altered brain structure and function demonstrate that the brain is affected by prolonged malnutrition ( Bang et al., 2017 ), increasing damage and functional disability over time ( Treasure and Russell, 2011 ; Treasure et al., 2015a ). Indeed, differences in gray matter volume are not observed between newly diagnosed adolescents and healthy comparisons ( Olivo et al., 2018 ). Here, we briefly review key observations in brain functioning for people with AN and how these become exacerbated by the starved state, linking to emotional processing and sense of self.

The amygdala is considered the brain’s ‘threat detector,’ central to fear conditioning ( Fossati, 2012 ) and highly responsive to emotional stimuli, particularly faces ( Sergerie et al., 2008 ). Amygdala hyperactivation is observed in adolescents compared with younger children and adults, and does not habituate in anxious individuals due to poorer connectivity with areas providing top-down emotion inhibition (ventromedial prefrontal cortex) ( Hare et al., 2008 ). Adolescents have elevated amygdala-hippocampal complex responses when anticipating social feedback; activation which persists following feedback of rejection for anxious individuals ( Lau et al., 2012 ). Hyperactivity in the amygdala is argued to have a pivotal role in AN ( Joos et al., 2011 ). When exposed to disorder related (e.g., food) and ‘non-disorder related’ stimuli (including emotion stimuli), people with AN exhibit greater amygdala activation ( Seeger et al., 2002 ; Miyake et al., 2010 ; Joos et al., 2011 ; Seidel et al., 2018 ), and heightened fear response ( Friederich et al., 2006 ), indicating heightened emotional arousal ( Seidel et al., 2018 ). Hatch et al. (2010a) argue that for people who develop AN, there is early non-conscious amygdala hypersensitivity to emotion cues, irrespective of weight or nutritional status. It is suggested that bias toward emotional stimuli, poor recognition of facial emotion expression and lack of soothing may be linked to the hyperactive amygdala in AN, persistent following recovery ( Oldershaw et al., 2011 ). Greater ability to tolerate emotion sensations may occur following recovery, in spite of continued increased amygdala activation ( Merwin et al., 2013 ). This suggests a trait amygdala hypersensitivity for people who go on to develop AN contributing to the experience of emotion as overwhelming and aversive, and which may become further heightened during adolescence corresponding with disorder onset.

Prefrontal cortex

During displays of negative emotion stimuli, increased activity is also found in the right and left dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex ( Leppanen et al., 2017 ; Seidel et al., 2018 ). Inhibition of negative affect is associated with activation of dorsal anterior cingulate, dorsal medial prefrontal, and lateral prefrontal cortices, and attenuation of brain activity within limbic regions ( Phan et al., 2005 ). This pattern of activity is therefore argued to reflect active control mechanisms in response to emotion, considered particularly necessary in the context of high levels of amygdala activity ( Seidel et al., 2018 ). Studies using resting state and task-based fMRI also appear to support the use of prefrontal mediated cognitive control and self-control processes in AN ( Wierenga et al., 2014 ; Ehrlich et al., 2015 ; Boehm et al., 2018 ) with increased prefrontal activation also observed following AN recovery ( Wierenga et al., 2014 ; Ehrlich et al., 2015 ). Self-control is considered an important mechanism for emotion regulation ( Paschke et al., 2016 ); thus, fits with self-report evidence of emotion over-regulation by people with AN ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ).

During adolescence, neural adaptations are crucial to the experience of emotion and development of emotion regulation. Self-reported embarrassment and activation in the medial prefrontal cortex are elevated ( Somerville et al., 2013 ). Increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation in girls particularly, coupled with decreased activation in the amygdala, may underpin development of the ability during adolescence to contextualize and regulate emotional experience ( Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006 ). Thus, increased activation of both dorsolateral and amygdala regions for people susceptible to AN ( Seidel et al., 2018 ) may result in a vicious circle of high emotion, poor emotion regulation and need for over-regulation. It is argued that development of orbital and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regions during and after puberty and increased activity might contribute to excessive worry, perfectionism and strategizing in people with AN ( Kaye et al., 2009 ).

The insula is involved in affective processing and functionally connected with the anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala and ventral tegmental areas ( Ham et al., 2013 ). The insula receives input regarding internal body states ( Craig, 2002 ). It supports visceral, muscular and physiological bodily feedback ( Swick et al., 2008 ) and is involved in anxiety control, regulating disgust, hunger, taste, pain and experience of body image ( Suchan et al., 2013 ). It is implicated in interoception, integrating visual and body perception with emotion and emotional feeling states ( Nunn et al., 2008 ). Access to conscious awareness of interoceptive experience is supported by the posterior insula, while the anterior insula is involved in emotion processing, integration of interoceptive, emotional and cognitive input and contextual integration of interoceptive information ( Li et al., 2017 ). This highlights the role of the insula in emotional awareness and conscious self-representations ( Craig, 2002 ; Seth, 2013 ).

It is proposed that people with AN are unable to integrate emotional information with sensory experience due to disruptions within the insula ( Nunn et al., 2008 ). Indeed, impaired thalamo-insular circuits are thought to explain a failure of integration of visuospatial information (e.g., pertaining to body image) and homoeostatic (e.g., hunger) signals for people with AN ( Geisler et al., 2016 ). This lack in ability of the insula to integrate basic emotion detection for people with AN occurs in the backdrop of a hyperactive amygdala sending high levels of negative emotional threat information. It is argued that the anterior insula, alongside the amygdala, nucleus accumbens and orbitofrontal cortex are key to supporting our conscious awareness of emotion and integrating this with our sense of self ( Seth, 2013 ). Further, the posterior insula receives and encodes visceral interoceptive input, and increased right posterior insular volume correlates with disorder duration and severity in AN suggesting that difficulties in integrating bottom-up interoceptive information increase with length of disorder ( Zucker et al., 2017 ). Moreover, patterns of insula responding to unpleasant interoceptive states are significantly different for those in remission from AN versus healthy comparison participants ( Berner et al., 2018 ). Altered interoceptive processing within the insula is observed even following recovery from AN ( Strigo et al., 2013 ). It is argued that this may contribute to difficulties predicting and adapting to internal state fluctuation for people with AN, that relates to past AN severity ( Berner et al., 2018 ). These data highlight that disruptions within the insula might link to observations of poor interoceptive awareness for people with AN, and which are exacerbated by disorder duration or severity.

Starvation Effects: Summary

People with AN have poor interoceptive awareness exacerbated by low weight. This impedes development and awareness of a sense of self and may be exacerbated by a need to reduce discrepancy between internal and external perception via over-reliance and internalization of external feedback or concrete perceptive cues. People with AN appear to have hyperactive amygdala leading to overwhelming emotional experience and increased prefrontal control mechanisms reducing emotional awareness and clarity. Disruptions within the insula further impair the integration of internal experience with self-awareness and self-representation. This may become more entrenched as the disorder progresses.

AN as a Lost Sense of Emotional Self

In short, the argument provided outlines AN as a difficulty with attunement and differentiation, leading to a sense of overwhelming emotional experience, that cannot be fully integrated, and is unable to develop into a coherent sense of self during adolescence. This results in poor understanding or perceived value in the individual’s own needs and emotions, resulting in reliance upon external signaling and validation. AN emerges as a means of regulating and managing emotional experience while also providing a false sense of self, including a false sense of needs and a concrete means to meet those needs (e.g., weight and shape goals). Indeed, perhaps the most compelling indication that people with AN lack a known and integrated sense of self is the fact that disorder appears in part to be a quest for this; a “blind search for identity” ( Bruch, 1973 ). The disorder very quickly becomes conflated with actual self, yet at a simplistic and concrete level that can be measurably perfected (“self as weight”; Bates, 2015 ). The anorexic identity emerges within social interaction and, once established, becomes validated through others’ responses: it achieves basic needs by expressing, communicating and encouraging connectedness from others. In the context of the description of the disorder above, and the self-perpetuation of this cycle, the paradox of AN becomes clear: it is a search for a congruent self, yet one which only removes the sufferer further from an authentic sense of self.

Clinical Conceptualization and Practice

We have argued that current interventions may be improved if one core hypothesized maintenance factor is isolated and addressed in therapy, with robust assessment of whether the intervention successfully manipulates it. We outlined our argument that in fact several risk and maintenance factors for AN may exert their effects, at least in part, by a single underpinning difficulty with emotion leading to a poorly integrated sense of self. We propose focusing on this putative underpinning factor in a future intervention. This is in keeping with an ‘interventionist-causal model approach’ which allows for independent vetting of a proposed maintenance factor’s relevance, and of the ability of an intervention to manipulate it; achieved by directly assessing whether the intervention on it changes outcomes ( Kendler and Campbell, 2009 ). Using this approach, theoretical models can be successfully translated into practice building a treatment using one hypothesized factor at a time (cf. Freeman et al., 2015 ). This is consistent with the MRC framework for developing complex evaluations which argues better understanding of expected changes is necessary before embarking on evaluation studies of new complex interventions ( Craig et al., 2008 ).

Addressing Poor Integrated Sense of ‘Emotional Self’ in Psychological Therapy

In keeping with our hypothesis that a ‘lost sense of emotional self’ forms a core underpinning factor, we argue that working to change this will impact clinical outcomes. Emotion-focussed interventions are supported by empirical evidence and have recently been described as promising for people with AN ( Sala et al., 2016 ). Indeed, since their recognition as putative maintenance factors, interventions have sought to include addressing emotional difficulties (cf. MANTRA, CBT-ED). Yet despite this, as described, limited efficacy above control comparisons remains. In keeping with other authors ( Schmidt and Treasure, 2006 ; Williams et al., 2016 ), we do not emphasize addressing weight and shape difficulties directly as part of a therapeutic intervention. Indeed, our model is not inconsistent with goals of other treatments, such as developing a ‘non-AN’ identity outlined in MANTRA; yet we explicitly argue the meaning of establishing identity as establishing a core emotional sense of self, which can be flexibly and adaptively used to direct an individual’s needs and relationships. Further, we state that this should be the primary therapeutic goal.

Although emotion is itself inherently complex, maintaining this focus throughout therapy may afford greater opportunity for change. Moreover, we suggest that other interventions do not fully address dfficulties with emotion. It is notable that previous research and intervention development in this area tends to focus upon understanding how emotions arise (e.g., negative appraisals generating guilt or shame), increasing psychoeducation around emotion, and managing difficulties with consequences for emotional experience (i.e., improving regulation). Yet this does not sufficiently address the breadth of difficulties as we view it. Reflecting on her lifetime of work seeking to understand AN, Bruch (1988) describes the therapeutic task: “To help the anorexic patient in her search for autonomy and self-directed identity by evoking awareness of impulses, feelings and needs that originate within herself. The therapeutic focus needs to be on her failure in self-experience, on her defective tools and concepts for organizing and expressing needs, and on her bewilderment when dealing with others” ( Bruch, 1988 , p. 8). In the 30 years since, there has been considerable effort and attention paid to establishing what the “defective tools and concepts” are and how the “bewilderment when dealing with others” manifests. This has clarified the outward picture of somebody with AN and we have sought to describe herein how this becomes self-perpetuating. As discussed, tackling such factors (e.g., in cognitive-behavioral interventions) may have some benefits; yet we propose that this may become less effective once AN has become established and ‘self-perpetuating.’ We argue that it is the “failure in self experience,” in establishing a felt sense of self through awareness that would naturally lead to self-directed identity, which is missed.

Emotion-Focused Therapeutic Approaches

As with all therapeutic approaches, the goals and formulation surrounding the emotions at hand must be carefully considered before applying an intervention or technique. There is a distinction between “primary” emotion as an adaptive inherent “bottom-up” emotional response to a stimulus ( Damasio, 2003 ; Greenberg, 2004 ; Greenberg and Pascual-Leone, 2006 ; Barrett et al., 2007 ) and secondary “top-down” emotions in response to cognitive appraisal ( Greenberg and Pascual-Leone, 2006 ). It has been shown that cognitive reappraisal is effective when used to regulate emotions generated by a “top-down process” (those occurring in response to cognitive appraisal of a situation), but not when applied to those generated by a “bottom-up” process (inherent, often adaptive, emotional response to a stimulus; McRae et al., 2012 ). Indeed, applying cognitive reappraisal to emotions generated in a “bottom-up” process results in increased amygdala activity, suggesting increased arousal, not regulation ( McRae et al., 2012 ). Furthermore, in low-intensity negative situations, people demonstrate preference for reappraisal, while high-intensity negative emotions are preferably regulated by disengagement distraction, blocking emotional processing before it gathers force ( Sheppes et al., 2011 ). This highlights clear differences in how emotional experience can be most adaptively approached and understood across situations and contexts, dependent upon the emotional need. People with AN may have a persistent disruption in early automatic emotion cues – “primary emotion” ( Hatch et al., 2010a ). This seems supported by our review findings ( Oldershaw et al., 2015 ) that people with AN report clarity mostly in emotions of guilt and disgust. Such emotions are generally considered to be top-down emotional responses influenced using compassion-focused and cognitive behavioral techniques ( Goss and Allan, 2014 ). While this can improve ED outcomes ( Kelly et al., 2014 ), such treatment may also miss a core feature of AN as described in this article, namely the primary emotional experience.

By the account outlined here, models and interventions for AN should bring focus to core “primary,” “bottom-up” emotional experience. They should seek to embed and integrate this into a self, affording an internal information system of needs to motivate appropriate action, thereby increasing self-efficacy and autonomy. It cannot be that people with AN are not subject to events generating primary emotions. Indeed, as described, hyperactive amygdala suggests a large amount of unconscious early emotion not fully integrated into a felt experience of self; thus undifferentiated, loses clarity and experienced as overwhelming, vague, and negative. Yet, how can one work with something blocked so early that it is experienced only as a vague and confusing state and the continued denial and avoidance of which is highly valued by the experiencer?

Existing psychotherapeutic models may be of benefit when considering how to address this and promote experiential processing of emotion. In Emotion-Focused Therapy (EFT; Greenberg, 2011 ) the therapeutic process is viewed as co-constructive in which “therapist and client influence each other in non-imposing ways to deepen client experiencing and exploration and promote emotional processing.” (p. 65). The primary goal is to utilize this therapeutic relationship to apply key principles of emotion change (awareness, expression regulation, reflection, transformation and corrective emotional experience). Emotional processing is achieved by recognizing that “the only way out is through” ( Pascual-Leone and Greenberg, 2007 ). Similarly, in Young’s Schema Therapy (ST; Young et al., 2003 ) a key goal is to be with an individual’s primary emotions or ‘core pain’ sometimes described as being ‘housed’ within a ‘vulnerable child mode.’ Time is spent understanding how an individual learnt to cope with their ‘core pain’ through surrendering, avoiding or overcompensating in relation to core maladaptive schema and the costs of these coping styles to their developing sense of ‘self.’

In both EFT and ST, methods of working with emotion are relational and experiential and a core part of therapy relies on chair work ( Rice and Greenberg, 1984 ; Kellogg, 2014 ). In ST, chair work can map out ‘parts of the self’ and their relationship with core pain or vulnerable child self, with the goal to promote adaptive communication between ‘parts of the self.’ For example, to ‘by pass’ coping modes (or parts of the self) that have learnt to block or deflect primary emotion (such as by ‘cutting off’), such that emotion can be experienced and expressed safely with the therapist and responded to in new and attuned ways. In EFT, parts of the self are most commonly understood as having functions of coach, critic or guard and their relationship with the ‘experiencer’ (which can be adult or child parts of self) are explored to enable primary feelings to emerge and be expressed directly in dialog between the parts, with a process of resolution facilitated ( Elliott et al., 2015 ). One previous adaptation of identifying parts of self to AN is to consider the critical voice specifically as an ‘anorexic voice’ which criticizes using core anorexic cognitions around eating, weight and shape ( Dolhanty and Greenberg, 2009 ; Pugh and Waller, 2017 ), the power of which relates to disorder severity ( Pugh and Waller, 2016 ).

In both EFT and ST, chair work aims to put parts of the self into live contact, differentiating and intensifying emotion expression and facilitating the identification and expression of an unmet need ( Young et al., 2003 ; Arntz and Jacob, 2012 ; Elliott et al., 2015 ). In ST, accessing primary emotion enables the therapist to attune via a re-parenting stance (in a limited way) to some of the unmet needs underneath the core pain ( Arntz and Jacob, 2012 ). In EFT, a distinction is made between the emotional states accessed: those which are adaptive or maladaptive. Once accessed and expressed, primary maladaptive emotions (e.g., core shame; fear of abandonment) can be transformed by putting them in contact with (coactivating) a more adaptive emotional experience (e.g., empowering anger or compassion for the self) to forge a new emotion ( Greenberg, 2011 ) often via expression of needs.

In addition to chair work, further experiential therapeutic tasks and tools enable access to emotional processes and memories. EFT draws upon experiencing-based tasks to respond to in-session client markers, such as experiential focussing to clarify and facilitate feeling shifts (cf. Gendlin, 1981 ) or systematic evocative unfolding to draw links between stimuli and puzzling reactions to create meaning ( Rice and Saperia, 1984 ). Enactment and imagery can activate adaptive emotional experience for emotion transformation ( Greenberg, 2011 ). ST uses imagery to help access less verbal ‘emotional memories’ for expression and integration into the individuals developing sense of self ( Arntz and Jacob, 2012 ).

While it is beyond the scope of this review to outline these therapeutic models and adaptation to AN in full, we have sought to highlight the compatibility of the EFT and ST models and associated change techniques to working with those with AN, particularly within the context of the conceptualization of a ‘lost emotional self.’ The goal is that a healthy and known self emerges, with a capacity to become aware of, make sense of, regulate, accept, express, and transform emotional experience, thereby using this flexibly and adaptively to navigate themselves, relationships and the world (a reclaimed “conductor”).

Future Directions in Testing the ‘Lost Emotional Self’ Hypothesis

A clear direction in testing the hypothesis of a ‘lost emotional self’ is to develop a psychological intervention based on the theory, and assess whether: (i) these changes to emotion and self can be achieved, (ii) these changes occur via the proposed mechanisms of enhanced emotional processing, particularly in regard primary ‘bottom up’ emotion, along with less reliance on external cues (and therefore less prediction errors), and an enhanced experience of and belief in core needs and; (iii) this impacts on ED outcomes.

Individual aspects of the model could also be tested. For example, we assert that people with AN experience large perceived prediction errors ( Seth, 2013 ; see the section “Interoception”). This results in integration of external signals as part of self-representation, including ultimately the external perception of their own body as an object. We argue that large prediction errors would predict anxiety, intolerance of uncertainty and reinforce unhelpful emotion regulation strategies. Cross sectional designs could compare those currently ill and those with no illness history, to examine where interoceptive difficulties lie (i.e., objective accuracy, self-evaluated trait interoceptive sensibility or meta-cognitive awareness; Garfinkel et al., 2015 ). Reduced objective interoceptive sensitivity alongside elevated trait interoceptive sensibility indicates an ‘interoceptive trait prediction error’ ( Garfinkel et al., 2016 ; Seth and Friston, 2016 ). This could be furthered by comparing with those recovered from AN and examining relationships with anxiety processes and alexithymia.

We propose AN arises from and perpetuates a lost sense of emotional self; a person without the conductor of the orchestra (our emotional sense of self), persistently reliant upon and sensitive to audience feedback to ascertain if it is performing adequately. This model suggests working to improve awareness, acceptance and valuing of one’s own adaptive interoceptive emotional experience over exteroceptive feedback to achieve emotional validation, emotional self-efficacy and self-agency. Yet this is hampered by high valuing of AN by the sufferer and an increased lack of interoceptive and emotional experience as the disorder progresses, such that AN becomes ‘self-perpetuating,’ creating a ‘stuckness’ in therapy. We propose working in detail with this core putative underpinning maintenance factor and emphasize experiential therapies for psychological engagement with this presentation. This formulation is now to be directly tested, but provides a novel interpretation of existing data, grounded within previous empirical findings.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to conception of ideas within the manuscript. AO wrote the manuscript. HS and TL read and gave critical feedback on drafts. All authors approved the final version for submission.

This manuscript is independent research arising from an Integrated Clinical Academic Fellowship-Clinical Lectureship awarded to AO (ICA-CL-2015-01-005) supported by the National Institute for Health Research and Health Education England. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research, Health Education England or the Department of Health.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer DV and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation at the time of review.

Adambegan, M., Wagner, G., Nader, I. W., Fernandez-Aranda, F., Treasure, J., and Karwautz, A. (2012). Internalizing and externalizing behaviour problems in childhood contribute to the development of anorexia and bulimia nervosa-a study comparing sister pairs. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 20, 116–120. doi: 10.1002/erv.1152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 231–239. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

American Psychological Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: APA.

Google Scholar

Amianto, F., Northoff, G., Abbate Daga, G., Fassino, S., and Tasca, G. A. (2016). Is anorexia nervosa a disorder of the self? A psychological approach. Front. Psychol. 7:849. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00849

Arcelus, J., Haslam, M., Farrow, C., and Meyer, C. (2013). The role of interpersonal functioning in the maintenance of eating psychopathology: a systematic review and testable model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 156–167. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.10.009

Arntz, A., and Jacob, G. (2012). Schema Therapy in Practice: An Introductory Guide to the Schema Mode Approach. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

Bachner-Melman, R., Zohar, A. H., Kremer, I., Komer, M., Blank, S., Golan, M., et al. (2009). Self-monitoring in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 55, 170–179. doi: 10.1177/0020764008094647

Bang, L., Treasure, J., Ro, O., and Joos, A. (2017). Advancing our understanding of the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa: translation into treatment. J. Eat. Disord. 5:38. doi: 10.1186/s40337-017-0169-8

Barnea-Goraly, N., Menon, V., Eckert, M., Tamm, L., Bammer, R., Karchemskiy, A., et al. (2005). White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1848–1854. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi062

Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 373–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709

Bates, C. F. (2015). “I am a waste of breath, of space, of time”: metaphors of self in a pro-anorexia group. Qual. Health Res. 25, 189–204. doi: 10.1177/1049732314550004

Batsell, W. R. Jr., Brown, A. S., Ansfield, M. E., and Paschall, G. Y. (2002). “You will eat all of that!”: a retrospective analysis of forced consumption episodes. Appetite 38, 211–219. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0482

Beadle, J. N., Paradiso, S., Salerno, A., and McCormick, L. M. (2013). Alexithymia, emotional empathy, and self-regulation in anorexia nervosa. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 25, 107–120.

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev. 55, 83–96. doi: 10.2307/1129836

Berner, L. A., Simmons, A. N., Wierenga, C. E., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Paulus, M. P., Bailer, U. F., et al. (2018). Altered interoceptive activation before, during, and after aversive breathing load in women remitted from anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 48, 142–154. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001635

Beyers, W., and Goossens, L. (2008). Dynamics of perceived parenting and identity formation in late adolescence. J. Adolesc. 31, 165–184. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.04.003

Bigman, Y. E., Mauss, I. B., Gross, J. J., and Tamir, M. (2016). Yes I can: expected success promotes actual success in emotion regulation. Cogn. Emot. 30, 1380–1387. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1067188

Bion, W. R. (1962). Learning from Experience. London: Heinemann.

Blakemore, S. J., and Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x

Boehm, I., King, J. A., Bernardoni, F., Geisler, D., Seidel, M., Ritschel, F., et al. (2018). Subliminal and supraliminal processing of reward-related stimuli in anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 48, 790–800. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002161

Bora, E., and Kose, S. (2016). Meta-analysis of theory of mind in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a specific Impairment of cognitive perspective taking in anorexia nervosa? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 739–740. doi: 10.1002/eat.22572

Bosma, H. A., and Kunnen, E. S. (2001). Identity & Emotion: Development through Self-Organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511598425

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss: Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Brand, A. E., and Klimes-Dougan, B. (2010). Emotion socialization in adolescence: the roles of mothers and fathers. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2010, 85–100. doi: 10.1002/cd.270

Brockmeyer, T., Grosse Holtforth, M., Bents, H., Herzog, W., and Friederich, H. C. (2013). Lower body weight is associated with less negative emotions in sad autobiographical memories of patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 210, 548–552. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.024

Brockmeyer, T., Holtforth, M. G., Bents, H., Kammerer, A., Herzog, W., and Friederich, H. C. (2012). Starvation and emotion regulation in anorexia nervosa. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.003

Brody, L. R., and Hall, J. A. (2008). “Gender and emotion in context,” in Handbook of Emotions , eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: The Guildford press), 395–408.

Brown, A., McClelland, J., Boysen, E., Mountford, V., Glennon, D., and Schmidt, U. (2018). The FREED Project (first episode and rapid early intervention in eating disorders): service model, feasibility and acceptability. Early Interv. Psychiatry 12, 250–257. doi: 10.1111/eip.12382

Bruch, H. (1962). Perceptual and conceptual disturbances in anorexia nervosa. Psychosom. Med. 24, 187–194. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196203000-00009

Bruch, H. (1973). Eating Disorders. Obesity, Anorexia Nervosa, and the Person within. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Bruch, H. (1988). Conversations with Anorexics. London: Jason Aronson, Inc.

Buckholdt, K. E., Parra, G. R., and Jobe-Shields, L. (2014). Intergenerational transmission of emotion dysregulation through parental invalidation of emotions: implications for adolescent internalizing and externalizing behaviors. J. Child Fam. Stud. 23, 324–332. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9768-4

Buhl, H. M. (2008). Development of a model describing individuated adult child–parent relationships. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 32, 381–389. doi: 10.1177/0165025408093656

Bulik, C. M. (2014). The challenges of treating anorexia nervosa. Lancet 383, 105–106. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61940-6

Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., and Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003

Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., and Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion regulation and culture: are the social consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion 7, 30–48. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30

Caglar-Nazali, H. P., Corfield, F., Cardi, V., Ambwani, S., Leppanen, J., Olabintan, O., et al. (2014). A systematic review and meta-analysis of ’Systems for Social Processes’ in eating disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 55–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.002

Carper, J. L., Orlet Fisher, J., and Birch, L. L. (2000). Young girls’ emerging dietary restraint and disinhibition are related to parental control in child feeding. Appetite 35, 121–129. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0343

Carter, F. A., Jordan, J., McIntosh, V. V., Luty, S. E., McKenzie, J. M., Frampton, C. M., et al. (2011). The long-term efficacy of three psychotherapies for anorexia nervosa: a randomized, controlled trial. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 44, 647–654. doi: 10.1002/eat.20879

Chaplin, T. M. (2015). Gender and emotion expression: a developmental contextual perspective. Emot. Rev. 7, 14–21. doi: 10.1177/1754073914544408

Chaplin, T. M., and Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 735–765. doi: 10.1037/a0030737

Christov-Moore, L., Simpson, E. A., Coude, G., Grigaityte, K., Iacoboni, M., and Ferrari, P. F. (2014). Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 46(Pt 4), 604–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001

Claesson, K., Birgegard, A., and Sohlberg, S. (2007). Shame: mechanisms of activation and consequences for social perception, self-image, and general negative emotion. J. Pers. 75, 595–627. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00450.x

Cooke, L., Wardle, J., and Gibson, E. L. (2003). Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite 41, 205–206. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00048-5

Corstorphine, E., Mountford, V., Tomlinson, S., Waller, G., and Meyer, C. (2007). Distress tolerance in the eating disorders. Eat. Behav. 8, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.02.003

Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2000). “Evolutionary psychology and the emotions,” in Handbook of Emotions , 2nd Edn, eds M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones (New York, NY: Guildford Press), 91–115.

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655–666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

Craig, P., Dieppe, P., Macintyre, S., Michie, S., Nazareth, I., Petticrew, M., et al. (2008). Developing and evaluating complex interventions: the new Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ 337:a1655. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a1655

Crone, E. A. (2013). Considerations of fairness in the adolescent brain. Child Dev. Perspectives 7, 97–103. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12022

Crucianelli, L., Cardi, V., Treasure, J., Jenkinson, P. M., and Fotopoulou, A. (2016). The perception of affective touch in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 239, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.078

Dakanalis, A., Clerici, M., Bartoli, F., Caslini, M., Crocamo, C., Riva, G., et al. (2017). Risk and maintenance factors for young women’s DSM-5 eating disorders. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 20, 721–731. doi: 10.1007/s00737-017-0761-6

Damasio, A. (2003). Feelings of emotion and the self. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1001, 253–261. doi: 10.1196/annals.1279.014

Dan-Glauser, E. S., and Gross, J. J. (2011). The temporal dynamics of two response-focused forms of emotion regulation: experiential, expressive, and autonomic consequences. Psychophysiology 48, 1309–1322. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01191.x

Dapelo, M. M., Surguladze, S., Morris, R., and Tchanturia, K. (2016). Emotion recognition in blended facial expressions in women with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 24, 34–42. doi: 10.1002/erv.2403

Davies, H., Schmidt, U., Stahl, D., and Tchanturia, K. (2011). Evoked facial emotional expression and emotional experience in people with anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 44, 531–539. doi: 10.1002/eat.20852

Davies, H., Schmidt, U., and Tchanturia, K. (2013). Emotional facial expression in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. BMC Psychiatry 13:291. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-291

Davies, H., Swan, N., Schmidt, U., and Tchanturia, K. (2012). An experimental investigation of verbal expression of emotion in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 20, 476–483. doi: 10.1002/erv.1157

de Groot, J. M., and Rodin, G. (1994). Eating disorders, female psychology, and the self. J. Am. Acad. Psychoanal. 22, 299–317. doi: 10.1521/jaap.1.1994.22.2.299

den Uijl, L. C., Jager, G., de Graaf, C., Waddell, J., and Kremer, S. (2014). It is not just a meal, it is an emotional experience - a segmentation of older persons based on the emotions that they associate with mealtimes. Appetite 83, 287–296. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.002

Dias, P., Soares, I., Klein, J., Cunha, J. P., and Roisman, G. I. (2011). Autonomic correlates of attachment insecurity in a sample of women with eating disorders. Attach. Hum. Dev. 13, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2011.554005

Doba, K., Berna, G., Constant, E., and Nandrino, J. L. (2018). Self-differentiation and eating disorders in early and middle adolescence: a cross-sectional path analysis. Eat. Behav. 29, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.03.003

Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science 298, 1191–1194. doi: 10.1126/science.1076358

Dolhanty, J., and Greenberg, L. S. (2009). Emotion-focused therapy in a case of anorexia nervosa. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 366–382. doi: 10.1002/cpp.624

Duclos, J., Dorard, G., Berthoz, S., Curt, F., Faucher, S., Falissard, B., et al. (2014). Expressed emotion in anorexia nervosa: what is inside the “black box”? Compr. Psychiatry 55, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.10.002

Ehrlich, S., Geisler, D., Ritschel, F., King, J. A., Seidel, M., Boehm, I., et al. (2015). Elevated cognitive control over reward processing in recovered female patients with anorexia nervosa. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 40, 307–315. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140249

Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., and Greenberg, L. S. (2015). Learning Emotion-Focused Therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M. R., Haggard, P., and Treasure, J. (2014). Persistent body image disturbance following recovery from eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 400–409. doi: 10.1002/eat.22219

Fairburn, C. G., Bailey-Straebler, S., Basden, S., Doll, H. A., Jones, R., Murphy, R., et al. (2015). A transdiagnostic comparison of enhanced cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT-E) and interpersonal psychotherapy in the treatment of eating disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 70, 64–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.010

Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., O’Connor, M. E., Bohn, K., Hawker, D. M., et al. (2009). Transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioral therapy for patients with eating disorders: a two-site trial with 60-week follow-up. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 311–319. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040608

Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., and Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 509–528. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8

Fairburn, C. G., Shafran, R., and Cooper, Z. (1999). A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behav. Res. Ther. 37, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00102-8

Fivush, R., Brotman, M. A., Buckner, J. P., and Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent–child emotion narratives. Sex Roles 42, 233–253. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068

Fivush, R., McDermott Sales, J., and Bohanek, J. G. (2008). Meaning making in mothers’ and children’s narratives of emotional events. Memory 16, 579–594. doi: 10.1080/09658210802150681

Fivush, R., and Zaman, W. (2015). “Gendered narrative voices: sociocultural and feminist approaches to emerging identity in childhood and adolescence,” in The Oxford Handbook of Identity Development , eds K. C. McLean and M. Syed (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 33–52.

Fossati, P. (2012). Neural correlates of emotion processing: from emotional to social brain. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 22(Suppl. 3), S487–S491. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.07.008

Fox, J. R. (2009). A qualitative exploration of the perception of emotions in anorexia nervosa: a basic emotion and developmental perspective. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 276–302. doi: 10.1002/cpp.631

Freeman, D., Dunn, G., Startup, H., Pugh, K., Cordwell, J., Mander, H., et al. (2015). Effects of cognitive behaviour therapy for worry on persecutory delusions in patients with psychosis (WIT): a parallel, single-blind, randomised controlled trial with a mediation analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 2, 305–313. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00039-5

Friederich, H. C., Kumari, V., Uher, R., Riga, M., Schmidt, U., Campbell, I. C., et al. (2006). Differential motivational responses to food and pleasurable cues in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: a startle reflex paradigm. Psychol. Med. 36, 1327–1335. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008129

Fuhrmann, D., Knoll, L. J., and Blakemore, S. J. (2015). Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 558–566. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008

Gander, M., Sevecke, K., and Buchheim, A. (2015). Eating disorders in adolescence: attachment issues from a developmental perspective. Front. Psychol. 6:1136. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01136

Garcia, N. V., and Scherf, K. S. (2015). Emerging sensitivity to socially complex expressions: a unique role for adolescence? Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 84–90. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12114

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

Garfinkel, S. N., Tiley, C., O’Keeffe, S., Harrison, N. A., Seth, A. K., and Critchley, H. D. (2016). Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: implications for emotion and anxiety. Biol. Psychol. 114, 117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.12.003

Gaudio, S., Brooks, S. J., and Riva, G. (2014). Nonvisual multisensory impairment of body perception in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review of neuropsychological studies. PLoS One 9:e110087. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110087

Geisler, D., Borchardt, V., Lord, A. R., Boehm, I., Ritschel, F., Zwipp, J., et al. (2016). Abnormal functional global and local brain connectivity in female patients with anorexia nervosa. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 41, 6–15. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140310

Gendlin, E. T. (1981). Focusing. New York, NY: Bantam.

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a Different Voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UniversityPress.

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101,8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

Goss, K., and Allan, S. (2014). The development and application of compassion-focused therapy for eating disorders (CFT-E). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 53, 62–77. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12039

Grabhorn, R., Stenner, H., Stanguer, U., and Kaufhold, J. (2006). Social anxiety in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: the mediating role of shame. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 13, 12–19. doi: 10.1002/cpp.463

Gramaglia, C., Ressico, F., Gambaro, E., Palazzolo, A., Mazzarino, M., Bert, F., et al. (2016). Alexithymia, empathy, emotion identification and social inference in anorexia nervosa: a case-control study. Eat. Behav. 22, 46–50. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.028

Greenberg, L. S. (2004). Emotion-focused therapy. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 11, 3–16. doi: 10.1002/cpp.388

Greenberg, L. S. (2011). Emotion-Focused Therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Greenberg, L. S., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emotion in psychotherapy: a practice-friendly research review. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 611–630. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20252

Gross, J. J. (2013). Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward. Emotion 13, 359–365. doi: 10.1037/a0032135

Gross, J. J., and Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 106, 95–103. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95

Gunnard, K., Krug, I., Jimenez-Murcia, S., Penelo, E., Granero, R., Treasure, J., et al. (2012). Relevance of social and self-standards in eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 20, 271–278. doi: 10.1002/erv.1148

Habermas, T., and Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: the emergence of the life story in adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 126, 748–769. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.5.748

Ham, T., Leff, A., de Boissezon, X., Joffe, A., and Sharp, D. J. (2013). Cognitive control and the salience network: an investigation of error processing and effective connectivity. J. Neurosci. 33, 7091–7098. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4692-12.2013

Hambrook, D., Oldershaw, A., Rimes, K., Schmidt, U., Tchanturia, K., Treasure, J., et al. (2011). Emotional expression, self-silencing, and distress tolerance in anorexia nervosa and chronic fatigue syndrome. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 50,310–325. doi: 10.1348/014466510X519215

Hambrook, D., Tchanturia, K., Schmidt, U., Russell, T., and Treasure, J. (2008). Empathy, systemizing, and autistic traits in anorexia nervosa: a pilot study. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 47(Pt 3), 335–339. doi: 10.1348/014466507X272475

Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Voss, H. U., Glover, G. H., and Casey, B. J. (2008). Biological substrates of emotional reactivity and regulation in adolescence during an emotional go-nogo task. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 927–934. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.015

Harrison, A., O’Brien, N., Lopez, C., and Treasure, J. (2010). Sensitivity to reward and punishment in eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 177, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.010

Harrison, A., Sullivan, S., Tchanturia, K., and Treasure, J. (2009). Emotion recognition and regulation in anorexia nervosa. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 348–356. doi: 10.1002/cpp.628

Hatch, A., Madden, S., Kohn, M. R., Clarke, S., Touyz, S., Gordon, E., et al. (2010a). Emotion brain alterations in anorexia nervosa: a candidate biological marker and implications for treatment. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 35, 267–274.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hatch, A., Madden, S., Kohn, M., Clarke, S., Touyz, S., and Williams, L. M. (2010b). Anorexia nervosa: towards an integrative neuroscience model. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 18, 165–179. doi: 10.1002/erv.974

Haworth-Hoeppner, S., and Manies, D. (2005). A sociological account of the persistence of invalidated anorexic identities. Symb. Interact. 28, 1–23. doi: 10.1525/si.2005.28.1.1

Haynos, A. F., and Fruzzetti, A. E. (2011). Anorexia nervosa as a disorder of emotion dysregulation: evidence and treatment implications. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 18, 183–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2011.01250.x

Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2009). Adolescent eating disorders: definitions, symptomatology, epidemiology and comorbidity. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 18, 31–47. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.07.005

Hill, J. P., and Lynch, M. E. (1983). “The intensification of gender-related role expectations during early adolescence,” in Girls at Puberty: Biological and Psychosocial Perspectives , eds J. Brooks-Gunn and A. Petersen (Boston, MA: Springer), 201–228.

Illing, V., Tasca, G. A., Balfour, L., and Bissada, H. (2010). Attachment insecurity predicts eating disorder symptoms and treatment outcomes in a clinical sample of women. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 198, 653–659. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181ef34b2

Jacobi, C., Fittig, E., Bryson, S. W., Wilfley, D., Kraemer, H. C., and Taylor, C. B. (2011). Who is really at risk? Identifying risk factors for subthreshold and full syndrome eating disorders in a high-risk sample. Psychol. Med. 41, 1939–1949. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002631

Jacobi, C., Hayward, C., de Zwaan, M., Kraemer, H. C., and Agras, W. S. (2004). Coming to terms with risk factors for eating disorders: application of risk terminology and suggestions for a general taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 130, 19–65. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.19

Jappe, L. M., Frank, G. K., Shott, M. E., Rollin, M. D., Pryor, T., Hagman, J. O., et al. (2011). Heightened sensitivity to reward and punishment in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 44, 317–324. doi: 10.1002/eat.20815

Jewell, T., Collyer, H., Gardner, T., Tchanturia, K., Simic, M., Fonagy, P., et al. (2016). Attachment and mentalization and their association with child and adolescent eating pathology: a systematic review. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 354–373. doi: 10.1002/eat.22473

Jobe-Shields, L., Parra, G. R., Buckholdt, K. E., and Tillery, R. N. (2014). Adolescent reactions to maternal responsiveness and internalizing symptomatology: a daily diary investigation. Pers. Relatsh. 21, 335–348. doi: 10.1111/pere.12034

Joos, A. A., Saum, B., van Elst, L. T., Perlov, E., Glauche, V., Hartmann, A., et al. (2011). Amygdala hyperreactivity in restrictive anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 191, 189–195. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.008

Karwautz, A., Nobis, G., Haidvogl, M., Wagner, G., Hafferl-Gattermayer, A., Wober-Bingol, C., et al. (2003). Perceptions of family relationships in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their unaffected sisters. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 12, 128–135. doi: 10.1007/s00787-003-0319-1

Karwautz, A., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Hu, X., Zhao, J., Sham, P., Collier, D. A., et al. (2001a). Individual-specific risk factors for anorexia nervosa: a pilot study using a discordant sister-pair design. Psychol. Med. 31, 317–329.

Karwautz, A., Volkl-Kernstock, S., Nobis, G., Kalchmayr, G., Hafferl-Gattermayer, A., Wober-Bingol, C., et al. (2001b). Characteristics of self-regulation in adolescent patients with anorexia nervosa. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 74(Pt 1), 101–114. doi: 10.1348/000711201160830

Kaye, W. H., Fudge, J. L., and Paulus, M. (2009). New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 573–584. doi: 10.1038/nrn2682

Kellogg, S. (2014). Transformational Chairwork: Using Psychotherapeutic Dialogues in Clinical Practice. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Kelly, A. C., Carter, J. C., and Borairi, S. (2014). Are improvements in shame and self-compassion early in eating disorders treatment associated with better patient outcomes? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 54–64. doi: 10.1002/eat.22196

Kendler, K. S., and Campbell, J. (2009). Interventionist causal models in psychiatry: repositioning the mind-body problem. Psychol. Med. 39, 881–887. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004467

Klimes-Dougan, B., Pearson, T. E., Jappe, L., Mathieson, L., Simard, M. R., Hastings, P., et al. (2014). Adolescent emotion socialization: a longitudinal study of friends’ responses to negative emotions. Soc. Dev. 23, 395–412. doi: 10.1111/jora.12434

Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J. L., and Rhee, S. H. (2008). The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion 8, 737–752. doi: 10.1037/a0014179

Koepke, S., and Denissen, J. J. (2012). Dynamics of identity development and separation-individuation in parente-child relationships during adolescence and emerging adulthood–A conceptual integration. Dev. Rev. 32, 67–88. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2012.01.001

Kucharska-Pietura, K., Nikolaou, V., Masiak, M., and Treasure, J. (2004). The recognition of emotion in the faces and voice of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 42–47. doi: 10.1002/eat.10219

Kyriacou, O., Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2008). Expressed emotion in eating disorders assessed via self-report: an examination of factors associated with expressed emotion in carers of people with anorexia nervosa in comparison to control families. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 37–46. doi: 10.1002/eat.20469

Laghi, F., Pompili, S., Zanna, V., Castiglioni, M. C., Criscuolo, M., Chianello, I., et al. (2017). How adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their parents perceive family functioning? J. Health Psychol. 22, 197–207. doi: 10.1177/1359105315597055

Lang, K., Dapelo, M. M., Khondoker, M., Morris, R., Surguladze, S., Treasure, J., et al. (2015). Exploring emotion recognition in adults and adolescents with anorexia nervosa using a body motion paradigm. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 23, 262–268. doi: 10.1002/erv.2358

Lang, K., Larsson, E. E., Mavromara, L., Simic, M., Treasure, J., and Tchanturia, K. (2016). Diminished facial emotion expression and associated clinical characteristics in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 236, 165–172. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.004

Lau, J. Y., Guyer, A. E., Tone, E. B., Jenness, J., Parrish, J. M., Pine, D. S., et al. (2012). Neural responses to peer rejection in anxious adolescents: contributions from the amygdala-hippocampal complex. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 36, 36–44. doi: 10.1177/0165025411406854

Lavender, J. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Engel, S. G., Gordon, K. H., Kaye, W. H., and Mitchell, J. E. (2015). Dimensions of emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a conceptual review of the empirical literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 40, 111–122. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.05.010

Leppanen, J., Cardi, V., Paloyelis, Y., Simmons, A., Tchanturia, K., and Treasure, J. (2017). FMRI study of neural responses to implicit infant emotion in anorexia nervosa. Front. Psychol. 8:780. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00780

Li, D., Zucker, N. L., Kragel, P. A., Covington, V. E., and LaBar, K. S. (2017). Adolescent development of insula-dependent interoceptive regulation. Dev. Sci. 20:e12438. doi: 10.1111/desc.12438

Lieberman, M., Doyle, A. B., and Markiewicz, D. (1999). Developmental patterns in security of attachment to mother and father in late childhood and early adolescence: associations with peer relations. Child Dev. 70, 202–213. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00015

Lieberman, M., Gauvin, L., Bukowski, W. M., and White, D. R. (2001). Interpersonal influence and disordered eating behaviors in adolescent girls: the role of peer modeling, social reinforcement, and body-related teasing. Eat. Behav. 2, 215–236. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(01)00030-7

Luebbe, A. M., and Bell, D. J. (2014). Positive and negative family emotional climate differentially predict youth anxiety and depression via distinct affective pathways. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 897–911. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9838-5

Luyckx, K., Goossens, L., Soenens, B., and Beyers, W. (2006). Unpacking commitment and exploration: preliminary validation of an integrative model of late adolescent identity formation. J. Adolesc. 29, 361–378. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.03.008

Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., and Berzonsky, M. D. (2007). Parental psychological control and dimensions of identity formation in emerging adulthood. J. Fam. Psychol. 21, 546–550. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.546

Mahler, M. S. (1963). Thoughts about development and individuation. Psychoanal. Study Child 18, 307–324. doi: 10.1080/00797308.1963.11822933

Mallorqui-Bague, N., Vintro-Alcaraz, C., Sanchez, I., Riesco, N., Aguera, Z., Granero, R., et al. (2018). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature among eating disorders: cross-sectional and longitudinal approach. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 26, 53–61. doi: 10.1002/erv.2570

Marchi, M., and Cohen, P. (1990). Early childhood eating behaviors and adolescent eating disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 29, 112–117. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199001000-00017

Mauss, I. B., Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A. S., John, O. P., Ferrer, E., Wilhelm, F. H., et al. (2011). Don’t hide your happiness! Positive emotion dissociation, social connectedness, and psychological functioning. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 738–748. doi: 10.1037/a0022410

Mazzeo, S. E., and Bulik, C. M. (2009). Environmental and genetic risk factors for eating disorders: what the clinician needs to know. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 18, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.07.003

McAdams, C. J., Harper, J. A., and Van Enkevort, E. (2018). Mentalization and the left inferior frontal gyrus and insula. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 26, 265–271. doi: 10.1002/erv.2580

McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychol. Bull. 126, 424–453. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.424

McIntosh, V. V., Jordan, J., Luty, S. E., Carter, F. A., McKenzie, J. M., Bulik, C. M., et al. (2006). Specialist supportive clinical management for anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 39, 625–632. doi: 10.1002/eat.20297

McLean, K. C., and Jennings, L. E. (2012). Teens telling tales: how maternal and peer audiences support narrative identity development. J. Adolesc. 35, 1455–1469. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.12.005

McLean, K. C., and Mansfield, C. D. (2012). The co-construction of adolescent narrative identity: narrative processing as a function of adolescent age, gender, and maternal scaffolding. Dev. Psychol. 48, 436–447. doi: 10.1037/a0025563

McRae, K., Misra, S., Prasad, A. K., Pereira, S. C., and Gross, J. J. (2012). Bottom-up and top-down emotion generation: implications for emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 253–262. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq103

Meeus, W., Iedema, J., Maassen, G., and Engels, R. (2005). Separation-individuation revisited: on the interplay of parent-adolescent relations, identity and emotional adjustment in adolescence. J. Adolesc. 28, 89–106. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.07.003

Merwin, R. M., Moskovich, A. A., Wagner, H. R., Ritschel, L. A., Craighead, L. W., and Zucker, N. L. (2013). Emotion regulation difficulties in anorexia nervosa: relationship to self-perceived sensory sensitivity. Cogn. Emot. 27, 441–452. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.719003

Micali, N., Hagberg, K. W., Petersen, I., and Treasure, J. L. (2013). The incidence of eating disorders in the UK in 2000-2009: findings from the General Practice Research Database. BMJ Open 3:e002646. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002646

Miller, S. P., Erickson, S. J., Branom, C., and Steiner, H. (2009). Habitual response to stress in recovering adolescent anorexic patients. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 40, 43–54. doi: 10.1007/s10578-008-0112-y

Miller, S. P., Redlich, A. D., and Steiner, H. (2003). The stress response in anorexia nervosa. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 33, 295–306. doi: 10.1023/A:1023036329399

Miller-Day, M. A. (2012). “Two of me: mothers and daughters in connection,” in Mothers and Daughters: Complicated Connections Across Cultures , eds A. H. Deakins, R. B. Lockridge, and H. M. Sterk (Lanham, MD: University Press of America), 89–104.

Miller-Slough, R. L., and Dunsmore, J. C. (2016). Parent and friend emotion socialization in adolescence: associations with psychological adjustment. Adolesc. Res. Rev. 1, 287–305. doi: 10.1007/s40894-016-0026-z

Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., and Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic Families: Anorexia Nervosa in Context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. doi: 10.4159/harvard.9780674418233

Miyake, Y., Okamoto, Y., Onoda, K., Kurosaki, M., Shirao, N., Okamoto, Y., et al. (2010). Brain activation during the perception of distorted body images in eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 181, 183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.09.001

Moed, A., Gershoff, E. T., Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Losoya, S., Spinrad, T. L., et al. (2015). Parent-adolescent conflict as sequences of reciprocal negative emotion: links with conflict resolution and adolescents’ behavior problems. J. Youth Adolesc. 44, 1607–1622. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0209-5

Monteleone, A. M., Monteleone, P., Esposito, F., Prinster, A., Volpe, U., Cantone, E., et al. (2017). Altered processing of rewarding and aversive basic taste stimuli in symptomatic women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: an fMRI study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 90, 94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.013

Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., and Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 16, 361–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x

Nandrino, J. L., Berna, G., Hot, P., Dodin, V., Latree, J., Decharles, S., et al. (2012). Cognitive and physiological dissociations in response to emotional pictures in patients with anorexia. J. Psychosom. Res. 72, 58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.11.003

Nandrino, J. L., Doba, K., Lesne, A., Christophe, V., and Pezard, L. (2006). Autobiographical memory deficit in anorexia nervosa: emotion regulation and effect of duration of illness. J. Psychosom. Res. 61, 537–543. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.02.008

National Institute for Health, and Care Excellence [NICE] (2004). Eating Disorders in Over 8s: Management. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence.

National Institute for Health, and Care Excellence [NICE] (2017). Eating Disorders: Recognition and Treatment: NICE Guideline [NG69]. London: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence.

Nelson, E. E., Jarcho, J. M., and Guyer, A. E. (2016). Social re-orientation and brain development: an expanded and updated view. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.008

Nesse, R. M. (2017). Anorexia: a perverse effect of attempting to control the starvation response. Behav. Brain Sci. 40:e125. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16001503

Nordbo, R. H., Espeset, E. M., Gulliksen, K. S., Skarderud, F., and Holte, A. (2006). The meaning of self-starvation: qualitative study of patients’ perception of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 39, 556–564. doi: 10.1002/eat.20276

Nunn, K., Frampton, I., Gordon, I., and Lask, B. (2008). The fault is not in her parents but in her insula–a neurobiological hypothesis of anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 16, 355–360. doi: 10.1002/erv.890

Oldershaw, A., Hambrook, D., Stahl, D., Tchanturia, K., Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2011). The socio-emotional processing stream in Anorexia Nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 970–988. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.001

Oldershaw, A., Hambrook, D., Tchanturia, K., Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2010). Emotional theory of mind and emotional awareness in recovered anorexia nervosa patients. Psychosom. Med. 72, 73–79. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181c6c7ca

Oldershaw, A., Lavender, T., Sallis, H., Stahl, D., and Schmidt, U. (2015). Emotion generation and regulation in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis of self-report data. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 39, 83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.04.005

Oldershaw, A., Lavender, T., and Schmidt, U. (2018). Are socio-emotional and neurocognitive functioning predictors of therapeutic outcomes for adults with anorexia nervosa? Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 26, 346–359. doi: 10.1002/erv.2602

Olivo, G., Solstrand Dahlberg, L., Wiemerslage, L., Swenne, I., Zhukovsky, C., Salonen-Ros, H., et al. (2018). Atypical anorexia nervosa is not related to brain structural changes in newly diagnosed adolescent patients. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 39–45. doi: 10.1002/eat.22805

Orbach, S. (1993). Hunger Strike. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Pace, C. S., Cavanna, D., Guiducci, V., and Bizzi, F. (2015). When parenting fails: alexithymia and attachment states of mind in mothers of female patients with eating disorders. Front. Psychol. 6:1145. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01145

Pace, C. S., Guiducci, V., and Cavanna, D. (2016). A controlled study of attachment representations and emotion regulation in female adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 4. doi: 10.6092/2282-1619/2016.4.1187

Park, A., Bryson, C., Clery, E., Curtice, J., and Phillips, M. (2013). British Social Attitudes: the 30th Report. London: NatCen.

Paschke, L. M., Dorfel, D., Steimke, R., Trempler, I., Magrabi, A., Ludwig, V. U., et al. (2016). Individual differences in self-reported self-control predict successful emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1193–1204. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw036

Pascual-Leone, A., and Greenberg, L. S. (2007). Emotional processing in experiential therapy: why “the only way out is through”. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 75, 875–887. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.875

Pennesi, J. L., and Wade, T. D. (2016). A systematic review of the existing models of disordered eating: do they inform the development of effective interventions? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 43, 175–192. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.12.004

Pfeifer, J. H., and Berkman, E. T. (2018). The development of self and identity in adolescence: neural evidence and implications for a value-based choice perspective on motivated behavior. Child Dev. Perspect. 12, 158–164. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12279

Pfeifer, J. H., Kahn, L. E., Merchant, J. S., Peake, S. J., Veroude, K., Masten, C. L., et al. (2013). Longitudinal change in the neural bases of adolescent social self-evaluations: effects of age and pubertal development. J. Neurosci. 33, 7415–7419. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4074-12.2013

Phan, K. L., Fitzgerald, D. A., Nathan, P. J., Moore, G. J., Uhde, T. W., and Tancer, M. E. (2005). Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 210–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.030

Phillipou, A., Abel, L. A., Castle, D. J., Hughes, M. E., Gurvich, C., Nibbs, R. G., et al. (2015). Self perception and facial emotion perception of others in anorexia nervosa. Front. Psychol. 6:1181. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01181

PricewaterhouseCoopers (2015). The Costs of Eating Disorders: Social, Health and Economic Impacts. London: PricewaterhouseCoopers LLP.

Pugh, M., and Waller, G. (2016). The anorexic voice and severity of eating pathology in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 622–625. doi: 10.1002/eat.22499

Pugh, M., and Waller, G. (2017). Understanding the ’Anorexic Voice’ in Anorexia Nervosa. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 24, 670–676. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2034

Racine, S. E., and Wildes, J. E. (2015). Dynamic longitudinal relations between emotion regulation difficulties and anorexia nervosa symptoms over the year following intensive treatment. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 83, 785–795. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000011

Rajhans, P., Jessen, S., Missana, M., and Grossmann, T. (2016). Putting the face in context: body expressions impact facial emotion processing in human infants. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 115–121. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.01.004

Reau, N. R., Senturia, Y. D., Lebailly, S. A., and Christoffel, K. K. (1996). Infant and toddler feeding patterns and problems: normative data and a new direction. Pediatric Practice Research Group. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 17, 149–153. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199606000-00002

Reese, E., and Newcombe, R. (2007). Training mothers in elaborative reminiscing enhances children’s autobiographical memory and narrative. Child Dev. 78, 1153–1170. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01058.x

Reese, E., Yan, C., Jack, F., and Hayne, H. (2010). “Emerging identities: narrative and self from early childhood to early adolescence,” in Narrative Development in Adolescence , eds K. C. McLean and M. Pasupathi (Boston, MA: Springer), 23–43.

Rice, L. N., and Greenberg, L. S. (1984). Patterns of Change: Intensive Analysis of Psychotherapy Process. New York, NY: Guilford.

Rice, L. N., and Saperia, E. P. (1984). “Task analysis of the resolution of problematic reactions,” in Patterns of Change , ed. L. N. R. L. S. Greenberg (New York, NY: Guilford), 29–66.

Richmond, S. (2002). Psychoanalysis and Feminism: Anorexia, the Social World, and the Internal World , Vol. 8. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins UniversityPress, 1–12.

Riva, G., and Dakanalis, A. (2018). Altered processing and integration of multisensory bodily representations and signals in eating disorders: a possible path toward the understanding of their underlying causes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:49. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00049

Rowa, K., Kerig, P. K., and Geller, J. (2001). The family and anorexia nervosa: examining parent–child boundary problems. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 9, 97–114. doi: 10.1002/erv.383

Saarni, H. U. (1984). An observational study of children’s attempts to monitor their expressive behavior. Child Dev. 55, 1504–1513. doi: 10.2307/1130020

Sala, M., Heard, A., and Black, E. A. (2016). Emotion-focused treatments for anorexia nervosa: a systematic review of the literature. Eat. Weight Disord. 21, 147–164. doi: 10.1007/s40519-016-0257-9

Sanders, W., Zeman, J., Poon, J., and Miller, R. (2015). Child regulation of negative emotions and depressive symptoms: the moderating role of parental emotion socialization. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 402–415. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9850-y

Schmidt, U., and Campbell, I. C. (2013). Treatment of eating disorders can not remain ’brainless’: the case for brain-directed treatments. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 21, 425–427. doi: 10.1002/erv.2257

Schmidt, U., Magill, N., Renwick, B., Keyes, A., Kenyon, M., Dejong, H., et al. (2015). The Maudsley Outpatient Study of Treatments for Anorexia Nervosa and Related Conditions (MOSAIC): comparison of the Maudsley Model of Anorexia Nervosa Treatment for Adults (MANTRA) with specialist supportive clinical management (SSCM) in outpatients with broadly defined anorexia nervosa: a randomized controlled trial. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 83, 796–807. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000019

Schmidt, U., Oldershaw, A., Jichi, F., Sternheim, L., Startup, H., McIntosh, V., et al. (2012). Out-patient psychological therapies for adults with anorexia nervosa: randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 201, 392–399. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.112078

Schmidt, U., and Treasure, J. (2006). Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 45(Pt 3), 343–366.

Sebastian, C., Viding, E., Williams, K. D., and Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cogn. 72, 134–145. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008

Seeger, G., Braus, D. F., Ruf, M., Goldberger, U., and Schmidt, M. H. (2002). Body image distortion reveals amygdala activation in patients with anorexia nervosa – a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 326, 25–28. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00312-9

Seidel, M., King, J. A., Ritschel, F., Boehm, I., Geisler, D., Bernardoni, F., et al. (2018). Processing and regulation of negative emotions in anorexia nervosa: an fMRI study. Neuroimage Clin. 18, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.035

Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., and Armony, J. L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 811–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002

Serpell, L., Teasdale, J. D., Troop, N. A., and Treasure, J. (2004). The development of the P-CAN, a measure to operationalize the pros and cons of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 36, 416–433. doi: 10.1002/eat.20040

Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007

Seth, A. K., and Friston, K. J. (2016). Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 271:20160007. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0007

Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2011). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Front. Psychol. 2:395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395

Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008

Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., and Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1391–1396. doi: 10.1177/0956797611418350

Skǎrderud, F. (2007). Eating one’s words, part I: ’Concretised metaphors’ and reflective function in anorexia nervosa–an interview study. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 15, 163–174. doi: 10.1002/erv.777

Smink, F. R., van Hoeken, D., and Hoek, H. W. (2013). Epidemiology, course, and outcome of eating disorders. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 26, 543–548. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a24f

Somerville, L. H. (2013). Special issue on the teenage brain: sensitivity to social evaluation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22, 121–127. doi: 10.1177/0963721413476512

Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., Ruberry, E. J., Dyke, J. P., Glover, G., and Casey, B. J. (2013). The medial prefrontal cortex and the emergence of self-conscious emotion in adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1554–1562. doi: 10.1177/0956797613475633

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., and Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004

Stanghellini, G., Castellini, G., Brogna, P., Faravelli, C., and Ricca, V. (2012). Identity and eating disorders (IDEA): a questionnaire evaluating identity and embodiment in eating disorder patients. Psychopathology 45, 147–158. doi: 10.1159/000330258

Stanghellini, G., Trisolini, F., Castellini, G., Ambrosini, A., Faravelli, C., and Ricca, V. (2015). Is feeling extraneous from one’s own body a core vulnerability feature in eating disorders? Psychopathology 48, 18–24. doi: 10.1159/000364882

Startup, H., Lavender, A., Oldershaw, A., Stott, R., Tchanturia, K., Treasure, J., et al. (2013). Worry and rumination in anorexia nervosa. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 41, 301–316. doi: 10.1017/S1352465812000847

Startup, H., Mountford, V., Lavender, A., and Schmidt, U. (2015). “Cognitive behavioural case formulation in complex eating disorders,” in Case Formulation in Cognitive Behaviour Therapy: The Treatment of Challenging and Complex Cases , ed. N. J. Tarrier (Hove: Routledge), 239–264.

Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: what changes, and why? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1021, 51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005

Steiner-Adair, C. (1990). “The body politic: normal female adolescent development and the development of eating disorders,” in Making Connections , eds C. Gilligan, N. Lyons, and T. Hanmer (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press), 162–182.

Steinhausen, H. C., and Jensen, C. M. (2015). Time trends in lifetime incidence rates of first-time diagnosed anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa across 16 years in a Danish nationwide psychiatric registry study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 845–850. doi: 10.1002/eat.22402

Sternheim, L., Konstantellou, A., Startup, H., and Schmidt, U. (2011). What does uncertainty mean to women with anorexia nervosa? An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 19, 12–24. doi: 10.1002/erv.1029

Sternheim, L., Startup, H., Saeidi, S., Morgan, J., Hugo, P., Russell, A., et al. (2012). Understanding catastrophic worry in eating disorders: process and content characteristics. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 43, 1095–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.05.006

Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 128, 825–848. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.825

Striegel-Moore, R. H., and Bulik, C. M. (2007). Risk factors for eating disorders. Am. Psychol. 62, 181–198. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.181

Strigo, I. A., Matthews, S. C., Simmons, A. N., Oberndorfer, T., Klabunde, M., Reinhardt, L. E., et al. (2013). Altered insula activation during pain anticipation in individuals recovered from anorexia nervosa: evidence of interoceptive dysregulation. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 23–33. doi: 10.1002/eat.22045

Suchan, B., Bauser, D. S., Busch, M., Schulte, D., Gronemeyer, D., Herpertz, S., et al. (2013). Reduced connectivity between the left fusiform body area and the extrastriate body area in anorexia nervosa is associated with body image distortion. Behav. Brain Res. 241, 80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.12.002

Suveg, C., Sood, E., Barmish, A., Tiwari, S., Hudson, J. L., and Kendall, P. C. (2008). “I’d rather not talk about it”: emotion parenting in families of children with an anxiety disorder. J. Fam. Psychol. 22, 875–884. doi: 10.1037/a0012861

Swick, D., Ashley, V., and Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci. 9:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102

Talsma, D. (2015). Predictive coding and multisensory integration: an attentional account of the multisensory mind. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9:19. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00019

Tasca, G. A., and Balfour, L. (2014). Attachment and eating disorders: a review of current research. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 710–717. doi: 10.1002/eat.22302

Tchanturia, K., Davies, H., Harrison, A., Fox, J. R., Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2012). Altered social hedonic processing in eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 962–969. doi: 10.1002/eat.22032

Treasure, J., and Cardi, V. (2017). Anorexia nervosa, theory and treatment: where are we 35 years on from Hilde Bruch’s foundation lecture? Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 25, 139–147. doi: 10.1002/erv.2511

Treasure, J., and Russell, G. (2011). The case for early intervention in anorexia nervosa: theoretical exploration of maintaining factors. Br. J. Psychiatry 199, 5–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.087585

Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2013). The cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: a summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. J. Eat. Disord. 1:13. doi: 10.1186/2050-2974-1-13

Treasure, J., Smith, G., and Crane, A. (2016). Skills-Based Caring for a Loved One with an Eating Disorder: The New Maudsley Method. Hove: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315735610

Treasure, J., Stein, D., and Maguire, S. (2015a). Has the time come for a staging model to map the course of eating disorders from high risk to severe enduring illness? An examination of the evidence. Early Interv. Psychiatry 9, 173–184. doi: 10.1111/eip.12170

Treasure, J., Zipfel, S., Micali, N., Wade, T., Stice, E., Claudino, A., et al. (2015b). Anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 1:15074. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.74

Tronick, E. Z., and Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Dev. 60, 85–92. doi: 10.2307/1131074

Ty, M., and Francis, A. J. (2013). Insecure attachment and disordered eating in women: the mediating processes of social comparison and emotion dysregulation. Eat. Disord. 21, 154–174. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2013.761089

Vandereycken, W., and Van Deth, R. (1990). A tribute to Lasègue’s description of anorexia nervosa (1873), with completion of its English translation. Br. J. Psychiatry 157, 902–908. doi: 10.1192/bjp.157.6.902

CrossRef Full Text

Viana, M. C., Gruber, M. J., Shahly, V., Alhamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Andrade, L. H., et al. (2013). Family burden related to mental and physical disorders in the world: results from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Braz. J. Psychiatry 35, 115–125. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2012-0919

Wade, T. D., Treasure, J., and Schmidt, U. (2011). A case series evaluation of the Maudsley Model for treatment of adults with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 19, 382–389. doi: 10.1002/erv.1078

Wagner, A., Aizenstein, H., Venkatraman, V. K., Fudge, J., May, J. C., Mazurkewicz, L., et al. (2007). Altered reward processing in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 1842–1849. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040575

Waller, G., Kennerley, H., and Ohanian, V. (2010). “Schema-focused cognitive behaviour therapy with the eating disorders,” in Cognitive Schemas and Core Beliefs in Psychological Problems: A Scientist-Practitioner Guide , eds L. P. Riso, P. L. du Tiot, D. J. Stein, and J. E. Young (Washington DC: American Psychological Association), 139–175.

Walsh, B. T. (2013). The enigmatic persistence of anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 477–484. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12081074

Ward, A., Ramsay, R., Turnbull, S., Steele, M., Steele, H., and Treasure, J. (2001). Attachment in anorexia nervosa: a transgenerational perspective. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 74(Pt 4), 497–505. doi: 10.1348/000711201161145

Wardle, J., Carnell, S., and Cooke, L. (2005). Parental control over feeding and children’s fruit and vegetable intake: how are they related? J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 105, 227–232. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2004.11.006

Watson, H. J., and Bulik, C. M. (2013). Update on the treatment of anorexia nervosa: review of clinical trials, practice guidelines and emerging interventions. Psychol. Med. 43, 2477–2500. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002620

Webber, L., Cooke, L., Hill, C., and Wardle, J. (2010). Associations between children’s appetitive traits and maternal feeding practices. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 110, 1718–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.08.007

Weil, L. G., Fleming, S. M., Dumontheil, I., Kilford, E. J., Weil, R. S., Rees, G., et al. (2013). The development of metacognitive ability in adolescence. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.004

Westenberg, P. M., Drewes, M. J., Goedhart, A. W., Siebelink, B. M., and Treffers, P. D. (2004). A developmental analysis of self-reported fears in late childhood through mid-adolescence: social-evaluative fears on the rise? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 481–495. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00239.x

Wierenga, C., Bischoff-Grethe, A., Melrose, A. J., Grenesko-Stevens, E., Irvine, Z., Wagner, A., et al. (2014). Altered BOLD response during inhibitory and error processing in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. PLoS One 9:e92017. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092017

Wildes, J. E., Marcus, M. D., Cheng, Y., McCabe, E. B., and Gaskill, J. A. (2014). Emotion acceptance behavior therapy for anorexia nervosa: a pilot study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 870–873. doi: 10.1002/eat.22241

Wildes, J. E., Ringham, R. M., and Marcus, M. D. (2010). Emotion avoidance in patients with anorexia nervosa: initial test of a functional model. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 43, 398–404. doi: 10.1002/eat.20730

Williams, K., King, J., and Fox, J. R. (2016). Sense of self and anorexia nervosa: a grounded theory. Psychol. Psychother. Theory Res. Pract. 89, 211–228. doi: 10.1111/papt.12068

Wolf, L. K., Bazargani, N., Kilford, E. J., Dumontheil, I., and Blakemore, S. J. (2015). The audience effect in adolescence depends on who’s looking over your shoulder. J. Adolesc. 43, 5–14. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.05.003

Wonderlich, S. A., Lilenfeld, L. R., Riso, L. P., Engel, S., and Mitchell, J. E. (2005). Personality and anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 37(Suppl.), S68–S71. doi: 10.1002/eat.20120

Woodside, D. B., Bulik, C. M., Halmi, K. A., Fichter, M. M., Kaplan, A., Berrettini, W. H., et al. (2002). Personality, perfectionism, and attitudes toward eating in parents of individuals with eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 31, 290–299. doi: 10.1002/eat.10032

Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., and Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema Therapy: A Practitioner’s Guide. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Zachrisson, H. D., and Skarderud, F. (2010). Feelings of insecurity: review of attachment and eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 18, 97–106. doi: 10.1002/erv.999

Zeman, J., Cassano, M., and Adrian, M. C. (2012). “Socialization influences on children’s and adolescents’ emotional self-regulation processes,” in Handbook of Self-Regulatory Processes in Development: New Directions and International Perspectives , eds K. C. Barrett, N. A. Fox, G. A. Morgan, D. J. Fidler, and L. A. Daunhauer (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 79–101.

Zipfel, S., Wild, B., Gross, G., Friederich, H. C., Teufel, M., Schellberg, D., et al. (2014). Focal psychodynamic therapy, cognitive behaviour therapy, and optimised treatment as usual in outpatients with anorexia nervosa (ANTOP study): randomised controlled trial. Lancet 383, 127–137. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61746-8

Zucker, N., Wagner, H. R., Merwin, R., Bulik, C. M., Moskovich, A., Keeling, L., et al. (2015). Self-focused attention in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 9–14. doi: 10.1002/eat.22307

Zucker, N. L., Kragel, P. A., Wagner, H. R., Keeling, L., Mayer, E., Wang, J., et al. (2017). The clinical significance of posterior insular volume in adolescent anorexia nervosa. Psychosom. Med. 79, 1025–1035. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000510

Keywords: Anorexia Nervosa, emotion regulation, emotion, eating disorders, psychological therapy

Citation: Oldershaw A, Startup H and Lavender T (2019) Anorexia Nervosa and a Lost Emotional Self: A Psychological Formulation of the Development, Maintenance, and Treatment of Anorexia Nervosa. Front. Psychol. 10:219. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00219

Received: 25 April 2018; Accepted: 22 January 2019; Published: 04 March 2019.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2019 Oldershaw, Startup and Lavender. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anna Oldershaw, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2024 Jan-.

Cover of StatPearls

StatPearls [Internet].

Anorexia nervosa.

Christine A. Moore ; Brooke R. Bokor .

Affiliations

Last Update: August 28, 2023 .

  • Continuing Education Activity

Anorexia nervosa is defined by the restriction of nutrient intake relative to requirements, which leads to significantly low body weight. Patients with this eating disorder will have a fear of gaining weight along and a distorted body image with the inability to comprehend the seriousness of their condition. This activity reviews the evaluation and management of anorexia nervosa and highlights the role of the interprofessional team in improving care for patients with this condition.

  • Outline the epidemiology of anorexia nervosa.
  • Explain the pathophysiology of anorexia nervosa.
  • Describe the management of anorexia nervosa.
  • Summarize the importance of improving care coordination among the interprofessional team members to enhance the delivery of care for those with anorexia nervosa.
  • Introduction

Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder defined by restriction of energy intake relative to requirements, leading to a significantly low body weight. Patients will have an intense fear of gaining weight and distorted body image with the inability to recognize the seriousness of their significantly low body weight. [1] [2] [3]

The success of many professions depends on a person's weight. Models and actors portray a level of thinness that is difficult to attain, and it is enhanced by make-up and photographic alterations. Athletes in sports such as ballet, long-distance running, and martial arts are pressured to maintain lean body weights to outperform the competition. Media outlets promote diet secrets and weight loss tips in excess. Populations such as maturing females identify thin body types with increased self-esteem and link weight loss with self-control. [4] [5]

  • Epidemiology

Anorexia nervosa is more common in females than males. Onset is late adolescence and early adulthood. Lifetime prevalence is 0.3% to 1% (European studies have demonstrated a prevalence of 2% to 4%), irrespective of culture, ethnicity, and race. Risk factors for eating disorders include childhood obesity, female sex, mood disorders, personality traits (impulsivity and perfectionism), sexual abuse, or weight-related concerns from family or peer environments. [6] [7] [8]

  • Pathophysiology

Studies demonstrate biological factors play a role in the development of anorexia nervosa in addition to environmental factors. Genetic correlations exist between educational attainment, neuroticism, and schizophrenia. Patients with anorexia nervosa have altered brain function and structure there are deficits in neurotransmitters dopamine (eating behavior and reward) and serotonin (impulse control and neuroticism), differential activation of the corticolimbic system (appetite and fear), and diminished activity among the frontostriatal circuits (habitual behaviors). Patients have co-morbid psychiatric disorders such as major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder.

  • History and Physical

Patients will report symptoms such as amenorrhea, cold intolerance, constipation, extremity edema, fatigue, and irritability. They may describe restrictive behaviors related to food like calorie counting or portion control, and purging methods, for example, self-induced vomiting or use of diuretics or laxatives. Many exercise compulsively for extended periods of time. Patients with anorexia nervosa develop multiple complications related to prolonged starvation and purging behaviors.

Workup includes a thorough medical history (comprehensive review of systems, family and social history, medications including nonprescribed, past medical and psychiatric history, prior abuse) and physical exam (looking for complications above). Basic labs include coagulation panel, complete blood count, complete metabolic profile, 25-hydroxyvitamin D, testosterone (males), thyroid-stimulating hormone, and urine testing (beta-hCG [females] and drugs, either illicit or prescription). An electrocardiogram is recommended to assess for life-threatening arrhythmias. Additional studies may be necessary if BMI is less than 14 kg/m, for example, echocardiogram in patients with hemodynamic compromise (dyspnea, murmurs, syncope) or computed tomography of the abdomen to rule out superior mesenteric artery syndrome or amenorrhea more than 9 months (dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry). [9] [8]

Complications of anorexia nervosa are listed:

  • Cardiovascular: bradycardia, dilated cardiomyopathy, electrolyte-induced arrhythmias, hypotension, mitral valve prolapse, pericardial effusion
  • Constitutional: arrested growth, hypothermia, low body mass index (BMI), muscle wasting
  • Dermatologic: carotenoderma, lanugo, xerosis
  • Endocrine: hypothalamic hypogonadism, osteoporosis
  • Gastrointestinal: constipation (laxative abuse), gastroparesis
  • Hematologic: cytopenias (inc. normocytic anemia), bone marrow hypoplasia/aplasia
  • Neurologic: brain atrophy, peripheral neuropathy (mineral and vitamin deficiencies)
  • Obstetric: antenatal and postnatal complications
  • Psychiatric: depression, impaired concentration, insomnia, irritability
  • Renal and electrolytes: hypokalemic metabolic acidosis or alkalosis (laxative or diuretic abuse, resp.), prerenal renal failure, refeeding syndrome.

The American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) provides the diagnostic criteria for anorexia nervosa (A-C). It classifies the disease by type, status, and severity.

Of note, amenorrhea has been removed from the DSM-5 criteria. Patients who meet the new criteria and continue to menstruate have similar outcomes as those who do not.

Other eating disorders have similar features to anorexia nervosa. Avoidant or restrictive food intake disorder involves food restriction with failure to meet the nutritional need. While patients are often underweight, this disturbance does not meet diagnostic criteria for anorexia nervosa. Individuals with binge eating disorder eat excessive amounts of calories in a short period with a lack of self-control but do not display compensatory behaviors such as purging or restriction. Patients with bulimia nervosa will binge and purge without a corresponding low BMI. Pica refers to chronic ingestion of nonfood substances and may be a manifestation of underlying medical or psychiatric condition. For example, patients with anorexia nervosa may eat toilet paper when they are hungry. Rumination disorder occurs when patients repeatedly regurgitate food for one month when no other medical condition can be identified and does not occur solely during the course of another eating disorder. Other specified feeding or eating disorder refers to conditions with symptoms that impair functioning but do not meet criteria for a specific eating disorder, for example, patients who meet criteria for anorexia nervosa but have BMI more than 18.5 kg/m are classified as “atypical anorexia nervosa.”

Major depressive disorder can cause anorexia and weight loss. However, patients are not obsessed with body habitus. Patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder may have food rituals but maintain a normal weight. Patients who abuse stimulants such as cocaine and methamphetamine experience weight loss through increased metabolism and concentrated efforts to obtain illicit substances rather than consume calories.

Medical conditions can cause weight loss. Examples are celiac disease, hyperthyroidism, inflammatory bowel disease, malignancy, poorly controlled diabetes mellitus, primary adrenal insufficiency, and tuberculosis. The diagnosis will come from the history and physical examination. Order labs as dictated by the clinical picture.

  • Treatment / Management

Treatment for anorexia nervosa is centered on nutrition rehabilitation and psychotherapy. Patients who need inpatient treatment have the following characteristics:

  • Existing psychiatric disorders requiring hospitalization
  • High risk for suicide (intent with highly lethal plan or failed attempt)
  • Lack of support system (severe family conflict or homelessness)
  • Limited access (lives too far away to participate in a daily treatment program)
  • Medically unstable (bradycardia, dehydration, hypoglycemia or poorly controlled diabetes, hypokalemia or other electrolyte imbalances indicative of refeeding syndrome, hypothermia, hypotension, organ compromise requiring acute treatment)
  • Poorly motivated to recover (uncooperative, preoccupied with intrusive thoughts)
  • Purging behaviors that are persistent, severe, and occur multiple times a day
  • Severe anorexia nervosa (less than 70% of ideal body weight or acute weight loss with food refusal)
  • Supervised feeding and/or specialized feeding (nasogastric tube) required
  • Unable to stop compulsively exercising (not a sole indication for hospitalization).

Outpatient treatment includes intensive therapy (2 to 3 hours per weekday) and partial hospitalization (6 hours per day). Pediatric patients benefit from family-based psychotherapy to explore underlying dynamics and restructure the home environment.

Refeeding syndrome can occur following prolonged starvation. As the body utilizes glucose to produce molecules of adenosine triphosphate (ATP), it depletes the remaining stores of phosphorus. Also, glucose entry into cells is mediated by insulin and occurs rapidly following long periods without food. Both cause electrolyte abnormalities such as hypophosphatemia and hypokalemia, triggering cardiac and respiratory compromise. Patients should be followed carefully for signs of refeeding syndrome and electrolytes closely monitored.

Pharmacotherapy is not used initially. For acutely ill patients who do not respond to initial treatment, olanzapine is a first-line medication. Other antipsychotics have not demonstrated similar effects on weight gain. For patients who are not acutely ill but have co-morbid psychiatric conditions such as generalized anxiety disorder or major depressive disorder, combination therapy with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and therapy is best. Patients who do not respond to SSRIs may need a second-generation antipsychotic. Tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) are less preferred due to concerns about cardiotoxicity, especially in malnourished patients. Bupropion is contraindicated in patients with eating disorders due to the increased risk of seizures. [10] [11] [12]

  • Differential Diagnosis
  • Chronic mesenteric ischemia
  • Malabsorption
  • Hyperthyroidism
  • Irritable bowel syndrome
  • Celiac disease

Remission in AN varies. Three-fourths of patients treated in out-patient settings remit within 5 years and the same percentage experience intermediate-good outcomes (including weight gain). Relapse is more common in patients who are older with a longer duration of disease or lower body fat/weight at the end of treatment, have co-morbid psychiatric disorders, or receive therapy outside of a specialized clinic. Patients who achieve partial remission often develop another form of eating disorder (ex. bulimia nervosa or unspecified eating disorder).

All-cause mortality is greater in AN compared to the rest of the population. It has one of the highest mortality rates of all eating disorders due to medical complications, substance abuse, and suicide. Patients with AN have increased rates of suicide and this accounts for 25% of deaths associated.

  • Complications
  • Delayed puberty
  • Hypercarotenemia
  • Hypothermia
  • Hypoglycemia
  • Osteoporosis
  • Failure to thrive 
  • Cardiomyopathy
  • Bradycardia
  • Arrhythmias
  • Renal failure
  • Constipation
  • Peripheral neuropathy
  • Pancytopenia
  • Infertility
  • Deterrence and Patient Education

Anorexia nervosa is a psychiatric disease in which patients restrict their food intake relative to their energy requirements through eating less, exercising more, and/or purging food through laxatives and vomiting. Despite being severely underweight, they do not recognize it and have distorted body images. They can develop complications from being underweight and purging food. Diagnose by history, physical, and lab work that rules out other conditions that can make people lose weight. Treatment includes gain weight (sometimes in a hospital if severe), therapy to address body image, and management of complications from malnourishment.

  • Enhancing Healthcare Team Outcomes

Anorexia nervosa is a serious eating disorder that has very high morbidity. The disorder is usually managed with an interprofessional team that consists of a psychiatrist, dietitian, social worker, internist, endocrinologist, gastroenterologist, and nurses.  The disorder cannot be prevented and there is no cure. Hence patient and family education is key to preventing high morbidity. The dietitian should educate the family on the importance of nutrition and limiting exercise. The mental health nurse should educate the patient on changes in behavior, easing stress, and overcoming any emotional issues. The pharmacist should educate the patient and family on the use of drugs like laxatives and weight loss pills. Only through close follow-up and monitoring can patient outcomes be improved. [13] [14]  [Level 5]

Evidence-based Outcomes

Remission in anorexia nervosa varies. Three-fourths of patients treated in out-patient settings remit within five years and the same percentage experience intermediate-good outcomes, including weight gain. Relapse is more common in patients who are older with a longer duration of disease or lower body fat/weight at the end of treatment, have co-morbid psychiatric disorders, or receive therapy outside of a specialized clinic. Often, patients who achieve partial remission develop another form of eating disorders like bulimia nervosa or unspecified eating disorder.

All-cause mortality is greater in anorexia nervosa compared to the rest of the population. It has one of the highest mortality rates of all eating disorders due to medical complications, substance abuse, and suicide. Patients with anorexia nervosa have increased rates of suicide, and this accounts for 25% of deaths associated with the disorder. [15] [16] [9]  [Level 5]

  • Review Questions
  • Access free multiple choice questions on this topic.
  • Click here for a simplified version.
  • Comment on this article.

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) Criteria for Anorexia Nervosa Contributed by Christine Moore, D.O.

Disclosure: Christine Moore declares no relevant financial relationships with ineligible companies.

Disclosure: Brooke Bokor declares no relevant financial relationships with ineligible companies.

This book is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ ), which permits others to distribute the work, provided that the article is not altered or used commercially. You are not required to obtain permission to distribute this article, provided that you credit the author and journal.

  • Cite this Page Moore CA, Bokor BR. Anorexia Nervosa. [Updated 2023 Aug 28]. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2024 Jan-.

In this Page

Bulk download.

  • Bulk download StatPearls data from FTP

Related information

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

  • Anorexia Nervosa - StatPearls Anorexia Nervosa - StatPearls

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Cookies on this website

We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you click 'Accept all cookies' we'll assume that you are happy to receive all cookies and you won't see this message again. If you click 'Reject all non-essential cookies' only necessary cookies providing core functionality such as security, network management, and accessibility will be enabled. Click 'Find out more' for information on how to change your cookie settings.

Department of Psychiatry

  • Accessibility
  • Knowledge Base
  • Our research

Oxford Brain-Body Research into Eating Disorders

Logo

Quick Facts

Founded in 2010

Novel integrative approach of clinical- neuroscience collaborations

Funding from HEFCE, MRC, Sir Jules Thorne Charitable Trust, Charles Wolfson Charitable Trust, Swiss Anorexia Foundation, Placito Bequest

We work to understand the cognitive, biological, emotional and somatic processes underpinning the severe eating disorder Anorexia Nervosa in particular, and Eating Disorders in general. Our trans-disciplinary research, involving clinicians and neuroscientists, aims to translate research findings into novel treatment strategies.

Girl holding a dandelion flower with the sun setting in the distance.

Our work focuses on Anorexia Nervosa , a severe eating disorder, which has the highest mortality rate of any psychiatric disorder. It  remains one of the most challenging to treat and recover from, with a lack of evidence-based treatments. We aim to develop more effective treatments we need a better understanding of processes underpinning the illness.

We are proud of the innovative way our research brings together cognitive science, neuroscience and experimental psychology. This integration helps generate a deeper understanding of how cognitive, emotional and bodily processes interact to maintain the illness. With an international reputation in the field of Eating Disorders, we  have made important advances in understanding the neurobiology of Anorexia Nervosa using functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI and MEG. These findings not only help us to better understand the disorder but enable us to push forward with research that is directly relevant to the development of new treatments.

Collaboration

In collaboration with world-class neuroscientists our work investigates brain processes underpinning thinking, feeling and experiencing reward, and how these differ for people with Anorexia Nervosa. Recent research has focused on the role of ruminative thought processes,  abnormal reward processing and compulsivity. We hope to translate research findings into developing new forms of treatment and relapse prevention.

Multimodal Imaging

In  collaboration with Professor Tipu Aziz, the Nuffield Department of Surgery, the Wellcome Centre for Ethics and Humanities and  the Oxford Centre for Human Brain Activity allowed us to initiate complimentary multimodal imaging studies of neural processing and reward in individuals with current and past Anorexia Nervosa, now published.

Deep Brain Stimulation

We also developed the first registered UK study of Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) targeted at neural reward centres, for individuals with severe enduring Anorexia Nervosa, with full HRA approval. These studies explored the neural processing and behavioural correlates of aberrant reward and habit formation in Anorexia Nervosa, and importantly to set the worlds first  ethical  gold standard to guide experimental brain research in  Anorexia nervosa. The findings will contribute to  an understanding of the neural processes underpinning Anorexia Nervosa and in tandem develop novel treatment strategies. With the aid of additional grants and generous charitable donations we continue with DBS study, which is now in the follow-up phase. We have now published the protocol and an important ethics gold standard to guide such studies worldwide.

Affiliated groups:

OxBREaD benefits from affiliations with HBA  well established groups within the Department of Psychiatry:

Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, OHBA

Wellcome Centre for ethics and humanities :  Professor Ilina Singh

CREDO1 : Professor Chris Fairburn’s research group is world leading in the development of treatments for Eating Disorders.

PERL : Professors Catherine Harmer's group has an international reputation for excellence in the field of neuroscience and neuroimaging.

Rebecca Park

Rebecca Park

Associate Professor and Honorary Consultant Psychiatrist

Selected publications

Journal article

Tsompanaki E. et al, (2024), Contemp Clin Trials

Pike AC. et al, (2023), Transl Psychiatry, 13

Martens MA. et al, (2022), J Psychopharmacol

SCAIFE J. et al, (2022), Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience

Braeutigam S. et al, (2022), Front Behav Neurosci, 16

Interested in helping eating disorder research?

Do join our research interested list if you have any personal experience of an eating disorder, currently or in the past; or if you would be keen to participate in future research studies on eating disorders . Please contact us if you want to know more about our work, and we will then send you information. 

Email: [email protected]  

Do check out our new papers in the 'selected publications' section of this page!

In memoriam

We are deeply grateful for the recent donations from the family and friends of the late Emma Bruce. We never met Emma but she had suffered from Anorexia for many years, and not long before she died, she was given hope after hearing about the work of OxBREaD.

Current collaborations

  • Professor Phil Cowen (Neurosciences, Oxford University)
  • Professor Catherine Harmer (Neurosciences, Oxford University)
  • Professor Kia Nobre (OHBA, Oxford University)
  • Professor Tipu Aziz  (Department of Neurosurgery, Oxford University),
  • Dr Jacinta Tan, Psychiatrist and ethicist, (University of Swansea)
  • Dr Sanne de Witt (University of Amsterdam),
  • Dr Claire Gillan (Trinity College Dublin) 

We are not currently recruiting. 

Related research themes

Applied Clinical Research – developing, delivering, and evaluating evidence-based treatments for mental illness

Advertisement

Advertisement

Anorexia nervosa and familial risk factors: a systematic review of the literature

  • Open access
  • Published: 25 August 2022
  • Volume 42 , pages 25476–25484, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

anorexia and psychology research

  • Antonio Del Casale   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2427-6944 1 ,
  • Barbara Adriani 2 ,
  • Martina Nicole Modesti 2 ,
  • Serena Virzì 2 ,
  • Giovanna Parmigiani 1 ,
  • Alessandro Emiliano Vento 1 &
  • Anna Maria Speranza 1  

8524 Accesses

3 Citations

1 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is a psychological disorder involving body manipulation, self-inflicted hunger, and fear of gaining weight.We performed an overview of the existing literature in the field of AN, highlighting the main intrafamilial risk factors for anorexia. We searched the PubMed database by using keywords such as “anorexia” and “risk factors” and “family”. After appropriate selection, 16 scientific articles were identified. The main intrafamilial risk factors for AN identified include: increased family food intake, higher parental demands, emotional reactivity, sexual family taboos, low familial involvement, family discord, negative family history for Eating Disorders (ED), family history of psychiatric disorders, alcohol and drug abuse, having a sibling with AN, relational trauma. Some other risk factors identified relate to the mother: lack of maternal caresses, dysfunctional interaction during feeding (for IA), attachment insecurity, dependence. Further studies are needed, to identify better personalized intervention strategies for patients suffering from AN.

Highlights:

This systematic review aims at identifying the main intrafamilial risk factors for anorexia nervosa, including maternal ones.

Intrafamilial risk factors identified mostly regard family environment and relational issues, as well as family history of psychiatric diseases.

Family risk factors identified may interact with genetic, environmental, and personal risk factors.

These findings may help develop tailored diagnostic procedures and therapeutic interventions.

Similar content being viewed by others

anorexia and psychology research

Anorexia nervosa in children and adolescents: an early detection of risk factors

anorexia and psychology research

Anorexia Nervosa and Impact After Three Decades

anorexia and psychology research

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

Introduction

Eating behavior encompasses all responses associated with the act of eating and is influenced by social conditions, individual perception, previous experiences, and nutritional status. Additional influencing factors include mass media and idealization of thinness. Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a psychological disorder concerning body manipulation, including fear of becoming fat and self-inflicted hunger. This disorder is interpreted as a response to the social context and a woman’s rejection of fat to deny mature sexuality (Gonçalves et al., 2013 ; Korb, 1994 ) and it was once supposed to have “hysterical” causes (Valente, 2016 ). The current definition of AN provided by the DSM-5 describes it as “a restriction of energy intake relative to requirements such as to lead to a significantly low body weight […]; intense fear of gaining weight or becoming fat, or persistence in behaviors that interfere with weight gain […]; alteration in the way weight or body shape are experienced […]” (Cuzzolaro, 2014 ). The lifetime prevalence of AN is estimated being of 1.4% (0.1–3.6%) in women and 0.2% (0-0.3%) in men (Galmiche et al., 2019 ). The lifetime prevalence rates of anorexia nervosa might be up to 4% among females and 0.3% among males (Van Eeden et al., 2021 ). AN finds its roots in biological, psychological, social, and familial risk factors.

More precisely, heritable risk factors for AN can be found in 48–74% of cases (Baker et al., 2017 ): for example, it has a higher prevalence in female relatives of individuals with AN (Bulik et al., 2019 ). The presence of genetic correlations between AN and metabolic and anthropometric traits may explain why people with AN achieve very low BMIs and may even maintain and relapse to low body weight despite clinical improvement (Bulik et al., 2019 ). On the other hand, psychological risk factors include excessive concerns about weight and figure, low self-esteem, and depression; while social risk factors are related to peer diet, peer criticism, and poor social support (Haynos et al., 2016 ). As far as family is concerned, it has been observed that anorexic girls’ families are often characterized by poor communication with one another, overprotection, conflicts, and hostility (Emanuelli et al., 2003 ; Horesh et al., 2015 ; Sim et al., 2009 ).

Overall, the puzzle of AN risk factors is still obscure and needs deeper investigations as far as some predisposing aspects are concerned, such as intrafamilial risk factors, which have been extensively analyzed but not properly clarified for clinical applications. Because of the multifactorial etiology of AN, intrafamilial risk factors identification can help to establish preventive interventions in at-risk individuals, and to provide tailored treatments from the earliest stages of the disorder. Our main hypothesis is that intrafamilial as well as maternal risk factors play an essential role in the development of the disease.

Therefore, the main objective of this work is to provide a scientific review of the existing literature about familial relational risk factors involved in the development of AN, with the aim of improving: prevention, establishment of an early diagnosis, and development of a tailored treatment.

Methodology

On February the 16th, 2022, a first research was conducted on PubMed with the title/abstract filter, using the terms “anorexia AND risk factors AND family” in the search bar. For eligibility, we included only randomized controlled studies and case-control studies focused on the issue, as well as case-control studies with at least 50 participants. We excluded reviews, single case studies, case reports, other types of articles and other studies that did not focus on the main topic. The system provided 76 articles, of which 24 were ignored for low relevance. Hence, 52 were assessed for eligibility, from which 26 articles were excluded for not respecting the inclusion criteria, and 12 were excluded for not analyzing the research subject specifically. To the remaining 14 articles, 2 were added from citation search.

In the PRISMA diagram below (Fig.  1 ), the articles identified for the review (76) are reported schematically: screened (76), assessed for eligibility (52) and included (16).

figure 1

PRISMA diagram of the study

The main results of the studies analyzed are summarized in Table  1 .

Despite anorexia having been usually considered an expression of age-specific conflicts intensified by constrictive cultural ideas and certain kinds of familial constellations (Bemporad et al., 1988 ), having our review included studies from 1990 to 2021 and conducted across many countries (i.e. US, Japan, Poland, UK, etc.) we can hypothesize that such a condition just evolves with culture and time, still maintaining certain background issues that we are aiming to emphasize in order to recognize certain red flags.

Eating disorders mark deficits in the ability to be nourished and to symbolize embodied experience. Psychoanalytic theories suggest that mothers who are insufficiently developed leave the child either austerely avoiding intrusion or struggling to digest maternal provisions without becoming lost in them. (Charles, 2021 ). Infantile Anorexia (IA) has been defined as a child’s refusal of food for more than 1 month, between 6 months and 3 years of age; acute and/or chronic malnutrition; parental concern about the child’s eating; mother-child conflict, talk, and distraction during mealtime (Chatoor et al., 1998 ). Maternal risk factors for (IA) we have identified across the review can confirm this widely accepted theory, specifically lack of maternal caresses (Mangweth et al., 2005 ), dysfunctional interaction during feeding in IA (Ammaniti et al., 2010 ), and attachment insecurity (Chatoor et al., 2000 ). Regarding maternal history of psychiatric diseases, it has been noted that maternal depression has an influence on the development of conflicts during mother-child interaction in younger children, while maternal psychoticism predicts mother-child conflict during feeding in older children (Ammaniti et al., 2010 ). This means that depressed mothers engage in less positive interactions with their infants while breastfeeding, with difficulties in empathically recognizing their infant’s affective states at mealtimes (Ammaniti, Ambruzzi et al., 2004 ; Feldman et al., 2004 ).

In addition to the relational risk factors, maternal diet seems to play a role in the development of AN (Haynos et al., 2016 ). This mechanism seems to find its roots early during childhood, since the infant’s weight appears to be inversely related to the mother’s degree of concern about her body shape (Ammaniti, Lucarelli et al., 2004 ). The “modelling theory of AN” (Pike & Rodin, 1991 ) argues that adolescent girls begin the diet by mimicking their dieting mothers. It seems that family concerns about weight and appearance are directly linked to the development of low satisfaction with one’s body, and therefore directly or indirectly related to eating problems (Leung et al., 1996 ).

Maternal risk factors are synthesized in Table  2 .

Enlarging our highlight from the mother to the whole family nucleus, the onset and maintenance of AN seems to be closely related to familial risk factors, and knowing them is crucial to identify the best therapeutic approach in order to target the unhealthy family environment as well as the needs of the patient. In addition, being aware of the familiar background may help in strengthening the hypothesis of genetic correlates within Eating Disorders (ED). Intrafamilial risk factors for the development of ED seem to have a greater impact when they occur early in adolescence (Field et al., 2008 ), but most of them are chronic in time and one can suppose they can be found in a family at any time during the life of the patient.

The major intrafamilial risk factors identified in this review are summarized in the following Table  3 .

Increased food intake in the family (Hilbert et al., 2014 ) seems to play a role in the development of ED. This seems counterintuitive, but the discrepancy between one’s family food intake and peer and media influences on body ideals may contribute to triggering a subtle mechanism by which diet represents a way to affirm oneself in front of the family and reestablish social acceptance.

Perfectionism (Hilbert et al., 2014 ; Pike et al., 2008 , 2021 ) is widely recognized as a familiar risk factor across many studies, and it can be assimilated to higher parental demands (Pike et al., 2008 ). It surely contributes to creating a tense family environment in which the development of oneself is more difficult, therefore inhibiting progressive differentiation of self from other (Charles, 2021 ). Perfectionism itself will become a personal risk factor for the outcome and severity of disease (Longo, Aloi et al., 2021 ) in a way that could be mimicking the family environment.

In general, unhealthy family functioning is predictive of adolescence problems (Lyke & Matsen, 2013 ). General family malfunction is predictive for AD onset during adolescence, and the level of affective expression of the family seems to be relate to ED risk during adolescence (Felker & Stivers, 1994 ), but our review has highlighted that all those features of what could be described as a “toxic” family environment in the common sense play a role in the development of AN. Emotional reactivity (Lyke & Matsen, 2013 ), as well as family taboos regarding nudity and sexuality (Mangweth et al., 2005 ), low familial involvement (Haynos et al., 2016 ), negative affectivity (Pike et al., 2008 , 2021 ), and family discord (Pike et al., 2008 ) may lie in the background in the lives of a future AN patient, and should be recognized as environmental risk factors in order to develop a tailored psychotherapeutic intervention that may involve the family as well as the patient, since it seems clear that the quality of family functioning influences the development (McGrane & Carr, 2002 ) and maintenance of EDs (North et al., 1997 ; Strober et al., 1997 ; Wewetzer et al., 1996 ).

As far as the presence of other disorders in family members is concerned, our review established that a familiar history of almost any psychiatric disorder (Longo, Marzola et al., 2021 ; Pike et al., 2021 ), including depression (Lyon et al., 1997 ), affective disorders (Steinhausen et al., 2015 ), alcohol and drug abuse (Lyon et al., 1997 ) plays a role in the development of AN. Nevertheless, having a sibling with AN increases the risk of developing AN (Machado et al., 2014 ; Steinhausen et al., 2015 ). We can hypothesize that the role of genetics in this mechanism is crucial yet still obscure, and nevertheless, talking about the presence of these diseases in members of the family nucleus, having to cope and live with the difficulties of others’ conditions is what can predispose to AN. In addition, the opposite may happen as well: there is an increased risk for relatives of patients with AN and BN to develop subclinical forms of ED, major depressive disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and anxiety disorders (Lilenfeld et al., 1998 ). What is curious to note is that, on the one hand, having a sibling with AN predisposes to the development of AN (Felker & Stivers, 1994 ; Machado et al., 2014 ; Steinhausen et al., 2015 ), probably because of shared intrafamilial risk factors, therefore underlining the importance of the aim of this review; but, on the other hand, negative family history for ED predicts poor outcome (Ackard et al., 2014 ), probably because of the familiar unpreparedness to cope with such a difficult condition and the discrepancy created between the healthy members and the patient, which remains alone and uncapable of sharing certain issues with the others, so close yet so far from them.

Another risk factor identified is having suffered a relational trauma (Longo, Marzola et al., 2021 ). In general, individuals who have suffered from traumatic events (physical violence, being threatened with a weapon, sexual violence, being a victim of robbery) more frequently develop maladaptive eating behaviors (Field et al., 2008 ). Some evidence also suggests an increase of severe life events in the year preceding the onset of AN (Råstam & Gillberg, 1991 ). Children of mothers who have experienced the loss of a vital member of their family (i.e. older child or partner) in the six months prior to pregnancy have a higher risk of ED than children and infants who have not been exposed to this risk factor (Su et al., 2015 ). Further confirming the possible role of relational trauma as a red flag not only in the development of AN, but also in determining the severity of the disease, patients with AN and comorbid Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) show more severe concerns about body shape and weight (Field et al., 2008 ). Having suffered physical and sexual abuse during childhood appears to be related to the onset of psychiatric pathologies in general, and not specifically to the onset of EDs in the young adult (Bruch, 1977 ; McGrane & Carr, 2002 ; Smith et al., 1995 ): therefore, this risk factor needs further investigation to confirm its specific role in the development of AN.

Strength and limits

The strength of this work lies in the comparison between different studies regarding AN showing high level of evidence and providing a complete picture of the constellation of intrafamilial risk factors of anorexia nervosa. There main limit of this study is that few articles from those included are from the very last years, while many other studies were conducted and published earlier (1990–2014), underlining the need of further investigations.

Conclusions

The main intrafamilial risk factors for AN identified from this study are: increased food intake in the family, perfectionism, higher parental demands, emotional reactivity, family taboos regarding nudity and sexuality, low familial involvement, negative affectivity, family discord, dependence, negative family history for ED (as a predictor of poor outcome), family history of depression, positive family history for psychiatric disorders, affective disorders in family members, alcohol and drug abuse, having a sibling with AN, relational trauma. Some other risk factors identified may relate to the role of the mother during childhood especially, and are as follows: lack of maternal caresses, dysfunctional interaction during feeding (for IA), attachment insecurity, dependence, maternal diet.

Complex interactions occur between intrafamilial risk factors and other personal aspects and symptoms, including perfectionism, individual body image issues, social concerns, excessive preoccupation with weight control, stress and adjustment problems, lack of close friends, social prejudice.

In conclusion, further studies are needed to understand more clearly how intrafamilial risk factors for AN interact with other environmental, personal and genetic ones, in order to connect the dots that can lead to an improvement of diagnostic and therapeutic procedures, and to the development of tailored intervention strategies that may target multiple issues in the life of the patient, including intrafamilial mechanisms that may be identified precociously and addressed through familial therapy, for the sake of the whole family nucleus.

Data availability

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Ackard, D. M., Richter, S., Egan, A., & Cronemeyer, C. (2014). Poor outcome and death among youth, young adults, and midlife adults with eating disorders: An investigation of risk factors by age at assessment. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 47 (7), 825–835. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22346

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Ammaniti, M., Ambruzzi, A. M., Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., & D’Olimpio, F. (2004). Malnutrition and dysfunctional mother-child feeding interactions: clinical assessment and research implications. Journal of the American College of Nutrition , 23 (3), 259–271. https://doi.org/10.1080/07315724.2004.10719369

Ammaniti, M., Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., & D’Olimpio, F. (2004). Transmission intergénérationnelle: troubles alimentaires de l’enfance et psychopathologie maternelle. Devenir , 16 (3), 173. https://doi.org/10.3917/dev.043.0173

Article   Google Scholar  

Ammaniti, M., Lucarelli, L., Cimino, S., D’Olimpio, F., & Chatoor, I. (2010). Maternal psychopathology and child risk factors in infantile anorexia. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 43 (3), 233–240. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20688

Baker, J. H., Schaumberg, K., & Munn-Chernoff, M. A. (2017). Genetics of Anorexia Nervosa. Current Psychiatry Reports , 19 (11), 84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0842-2

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Bemporad, J. R., Ratey, J. J., O’Driscoll, G., & Daehler, M. L. (1988). Hysteria, Anorexia and the Culture of Self-Denial. Psychiatry (New York) , 51 (1), 96–103. https://doi.org/10.1521/00332747.1988.11024383

Bruch, H. (1977). Patologia del comportamento alimentare . Italian Tr). Feltrinelli.

Bulik, C. M., Flatt, R., Abbaspour, A., & Carroll, I. (2019). Reconceptualizing anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 73 (9), 518–525. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12857

Charles, M. (2021). Meaning, metaphor, and metabolization: the case of eating disorders. American Journal of Psychoanalysis , 81 (4), 444–466. https://doi.org/10.1057/s11231-021-09324-9

Chatoor, I., Hirsch, R., Ganiban, J., Persinger, M., & Hamburger, E. (1998). Diagnosing infantile anorexia: the observation of mother-infant interactions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 37 (9), 959–967. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199809000-00016

Chatoor, I., Ganiban, J., Hirsch, R., Borman-Spurrell, E., & Mrazek, D. A. (2000). Maternal characteristics and toddler temperament in infantile anorexia. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 39 (6), 743–751. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200006000-00013

Cuzzolaro, M. (2014). Anoressie e bulimie . Il Mulino.

Emanuelli, F., Ostuzzi, R., Cuzzolaro, M., Watkins, B., Lask, B., & Waller, G. (2003). Family functioning in anorexia nervosa: British and Italian mothers’ perceptions. Eating Behaviors , 4 (1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1471-0153(02)00097-1

Feldman, R., Keren, M., Gross-Rozval, O., & Tyano, S. (2004). Mother-Child touch patterns in infant feeding disorders: relation to maternal, child, and environmental factors. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 43 (9), 1089–1097. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000132810.98922.83

Felker, K. R., & Stivers, C. (1994). The relationship of gender and family environment to eating disorder risk in adolescents. Adolescence , 29 (116), 821–834. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7892793

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Field, A. E., Javaras, K. M., Aneja, P., Kitos, N., Camargo, C. A., Taylor, C. B., & Laird, N. M. (2008). Family, peer, and media predictors of becoming eating disordered. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine , 162 (6), 574–579. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.162.6.574

Galmiche, M., Déchelotte, P., Lambert, G., & Tavolacci, M. P. (2019). Prevalence of eating disorders over the 2000–2018 period: A systematic literature review. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition , 109 (5), 1402–1413. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqy342

Gonçalves, J. A., Moreira, E. A. M., de Trindade, E. B. S. M., & Fiates, G. M. R. (2013). Eating disorders in childhood and adolescence.Revista Paulista de Pediatria: Orgao Oficial Da Sociedade de Pediatria de Sao Paulo, 31 (1),96–103. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0103-05822013000100016

Haynos, A. F., Watts, A. W., Loth, K. A., Pearson, C. M., & Neumark-Stzainer, D. (2016). Factors Predicting an Escalation of Restrictive Eating During Adolescence. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine , 59 (4), 391–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.03.011

Hilbert, A., Pike, K. M., Goldschmidt, A. B., Wilfley, D. E., Fairburn, C. G., Dohm, F. A., Walsh, B. T., & Weissman, S., R (2014). Risk factors across the eating disorders. Psychiatry Research , 220 (1–2), 500–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.054

Horesh, N., Sommerfeld, E., Wolf, M., Zubery, E., & Zalsman, G. (2015). Father-daughter relationship and the severity of eating disorders. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists , 30 (1), 114–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.04.004

Korb, M. (1994). Anorexia as Symbolic Expression of a Woman’s Rejection of Her Mother’s Life. Western Journal of Nursing Research , 16 (1), 69–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/019394599401600106

Leung, F., Schwartzman, A., & Steiger, H. (1996). Testing a dual-process family model in understanding the development of eating pathology: a structural equation modeling analysis. The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 20 (4), 367–375. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199612)20:4%3C367::AID-EAT4%3E3.0.CO;2-L

Lilenfeld, L. R., Kaye, W. H., Greeno, C. G., Merikangas, K. R., Plotnicov, K., Pollice, C., Rao, R., Strober, M., Bulik, C. M., & Nagy, L. (1998). A controlled family study of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: Psychiatric disorders in first-degree relatives and effects of proband comorbidity. Archives of General Psychiatry , 55 (7), 603–610. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.603

Longo, P., Aloi, M., Delsedime, N., Rania, M., Garcia, S., Abbate-Daga, C., G., & Marzola, E. (2021). Different clusters of perfectionism in inpatients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls. Eating Disorders . https://doi.org/10.1080/10640266.2021.1938937

Longo, P., Marzola, E., De Bacco, C., Demarchi, M., & Abbate-Daga, G. (2021). Young patients with anorexia nervosa: The contribution of post-traumatic stress disorder and traumatic events. Medicina (Lithuania) , 57 (1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina57010002

Lyke, J., & Matsen, J. (2013). Family functioning and risk factors for disordered eating. Eating Behaviors , 14 (4), 497–499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.08.009

Lyon, M., Chatoor, I., Atkins, D., Silber, T., Mosimann, J., & Gray, J. (1997). Testing the hypothesis of the multidimensional model of anorexia nervosa in adolescents. Adolescence , 32 (125), 101–111.

Machado, B. C., Gonçalves, S. F., Martins, C., Hoek, H. W., & Machado, P. P. (2014). Risk factors and antecedent life events in the development of anorexia nervosa: A Portuguese case-control study. European Eating Disorders Review , 22 (4), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2286

Mangweth, B., Hausmann, A., Danzl, C., Walch, T., Rupp, C. I., Biebl, W., Hudson, J. I., & Pope, H. G. (2005). Childhood body-focused behaviors and social behaviors as risk factors of eating disorders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics , 74 (4), 247–253. https://doi.org/10.1159/000085149

McGrane, D., & Carr, A. (2002). Young Women at Risk for Eating Disorders: Perceived Family Dysfunction and Parental Psychological Problems. Contemporary Family Therapy , 24 , 385–395.

North, C., Gowers, S., & Byram, V. (1997). Family functioning and life events in the outcome of adolescent anorexia nervosa. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science , 171 , 545–549. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.171.6.545

Pike, K. M., Hilbert, A., Wilfley, D. E., Fairburn, C. G., Dohm, F. A., Walsh, B. T., & Striegel-Moore, R. (2008). Toward an understanding of risk factors for anorexia nervosa: A case-control study. Psychological Medicine , 38 (10), 1443–1453. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707002310

Pike, K. M., & Rodin, J. (1991). Mothers, daughters, and disordered eating. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 100 (2), 198–204. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.100.2.198

Pike, K. M., So, M., Hilbert, A., Maekawa, H., Shimanouchi, T., Wilfley, D., Dohm, F. A., Fairburn, C. G., & Weissman, R. S. (2021). Risk factors for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa in Japan and compared to a U.S. sample. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 54 (2), 155–167. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23442

Råstam, M., & Gillberg, C. (1991). The family background in anorexia nervosa: a population-based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry , 30 (2), 283–289. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199103000-00018

Sim, L. A., Homme, J. H., Lteif, A. N., Voort, V., Schak, J. L., K. M., & Ellingson, J. (2009). Family functioning and maternal distress in adolescent girls with anorexia nervosa. The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 42 (6), 531–539. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20654

Smith, L. M., Mullis, R. L., & Hill, E. W. (1995). Identity Strivings within the Mother-Daughter Relationship. Psychological Reports , 76 (2), 495–503. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.76.2.495

Steinhausen, H. C., Jakobsen, H., Helenius, D., Munk-Jørgensen, P., & Strober, M. (2015). A nation-wide study of the family aggregation and risk factors in anorexia nervosa over three generations. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 48 (1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22293

Strober, M., Freeman, R., & Morrell, W. (1997). The long-term course of severe anorexia nervosa in adolescents: survival analysis of recovery, relapse, and outcome predictors over 10–15 years in a prospective study. The International Journal of Eating Disorders , 22 (4), 339–360. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199712)22:4%339::aid-eat1%3.0.co;2-n

Su, X., Xu, B., Liang, H., Olsen, J., Yuan, W., Cnattingius, S., László, K. D., & Li, J. (2015). Prenatal maternal bereavement and risk of eating disorders in infants and toddlers: a population-based cohort study. Bmc Psychiatry , 15 , 229. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0612-9

Valente, S. (2016). The hysterical anorexia epidemic in the French nineteenth century. Dialogues in Philosophy Mental and Neuro Sciences , 9 (1), 22–23.

Google Scholar  

Van Eeden, A. E., Van Hoeken, D., & Hoek, H. W. (2021). Incidence, prevalence and mortality of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Current Opinion in Psychiatry , 34 (6), 515–524. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000739

Wewetzer, C., Deimel, W., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Mattejat, F., & Remschmidt, H. (1996). Follow-up investigation of family relations in patients with anorexia nervosa. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry , 5 (1), 18–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00708210

Download references

Acknowledgement

Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza within the CRUI-CARE Agreement.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, and Health Studies, Unit of Psychiatry ‘Sant’Andrea’ University Hospital, Sapienza University of Rome, Via di Grottarossa, 1035-1039, 00189, Rome, Italy

Antonio Del Casale, Giovanna Parmigiani, Alessandro Emiliano Vento & Anna Maria Speranza

Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Barbara Adriani, Martina Nicole Modesti & Serena Virzì

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

Conceptualization: ADC, SV; Data curation ADC, GP; Investigation: SV, BA, MNM, AV, GP; Methodology: ADC; MNM; Supervision: ADC, AMS; Roles/Writing - original draft: ADC, SV, BA, MNM; Writing - review & editing: ADC, MNM.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antonio Del Casale .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest statement.

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Informed consent

Not applicable.

Conflict of interest

Additional information, publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Del Casale, A., Adriani, B., Modesti, M.N. et al. Anorexia nervosa and familial risk factors: a systematic review of the literature. Curr Psychol 42 , 25476–25484 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03563-4

Download citation

Received : 09 May 2022

Revised : 09 May 2022

Accepted : 24 July 2022

Published : 25 August 2022

Issue Date : October 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03563-4

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Anorexia nervosa
  • Risk factors
  • Eating disorders
  • Environment

Level of evidence:

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Developments in the psychological treatment of anorexia nervosa

    anorexia and psychology research

  2. Anorexia Nervosa in Psychological Point of View

    anorexia and psychology research

  3. Anorexia Nervosa

    anorexia and psychology research

  4. (PDF) What does anorexia nervosa mean? Qualitative study of the

    anorexia and psychology research

  5. (PDF) Psychological Impact of Pro-Anorexia and Pro-Eating Disorder

    anorexia and psychology research

  6. Activity Anorexia: Theory, Research, and Treatment

    anorexia and psychology research

VIDEO

  1. A cruel disease

  2. Psychology says anorexia is.. #psychology #shorts #fyp #fypシ゚viral #fyp #trend #subscribe #health

  3. My Anorexia Story: Childhood

  4. Anorexia study

  5. Anorexia recovery update

  6. PSYCHOLOGY

COMMENTS

  1. Psychological Treatments for Eating Disorders

    Keywords: eating disorders, psychological treatments, evidence-based treatment. INTRODUCTION. This review discusses evidence-based psychological treatments for anorexia nervosa (AN), bulimia nervosa (BN), binge eating disorder (BED), and subclinical diagnoses, with a focus on clinical research updates from the past 18 months.

  2. New insights on eating disorders

    Still other eating disorders fall into a catchall category known as "eating disorders not otherwise specified." What all of these disorders share is a dangerously maladaptive approach to food. Unsurprisingly, binge-eating disorder is often associated with obesity. People with anorexia and bulimia, on the other hand, fear gaining weight.

  3. Developments in the psychological treatment of anorexia nervosa and

    no caption available. FINDINGS ON ANOREXIA NERVOSA TREATMENTS. In recent years, there have been several publications on randomized control trials (RCTs), systematic reviews and meta-analyses on the efficacy of psychological treatments for anorexia nervosa [17 ,18 ,19 ].These studies focused on treatment interventions, patient characteristics and other factors that might affect outcome.

  4. Psychological Meanings of Eating Disorders and Their Association With

    Her research interests include the etiology and treatment of eating disorders. Catherine Bégin is a professor of psychology at the School of psychology of Laval University. She is the director of a multidisciplinary clinic specialized in the treatment of eating and weight disorders.

  5. Frontiers

    Introduction. Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is an eating disorder (ED) characterized by self-starvation driven by weight, shape and eating concerns and extreme dread of food, eating and normal body weight (American Psychological Association [APA], 2013; Walsh, 2013; Treasure et al., 2015b).The annual United Kingdom female incidence of AN is approximately 14 cases per 100,000 (Micali et al., 2013 ...

  6. Eating disorders

    Eating disorders are abnormal eating habits that can threaten your health or even your life, including anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and binge eating. ... Topics in Psychology. Explore how scientific research by psychologists can inform our professional lives, family and community relationships, emotional wellness, and more. Popular Topics ...

  7. Anorexia Nervosa

    Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder defined by restriction of energy intake relative to requirements, leading to a significantly low body weight. Patients will have an intense fear of gaining weight and distorted body image with the inability to recognize the seriousness of their significantly low body weight.[1][2][3]

  8. Oxford Brain-Body Research into Eating Disorders

    We work to understand the cognitive, biological, emotional and somatic processes underpinning the severe eating disorder Anorexia Nervosa in particular, and Eating Disorders in general. Our trans-disciplinary research, involving clinicians and neuroscientists, aims to translate research findings into novel treatment strategies.

  9. The evolving profile of eating disorders and their treatment in a

    It has been more than 70 years since the first eating disorder, anorexia nervosa, was included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM). Since then, recognition of a spectrum of disorders with eating difficulties has grown.1 We draw on academic, clinical, and lived experience expertise to identify two inter-related challenges in the understanding and treatment of ...

  10. Anorexia nervosa and familial risk factors: a systematic review of the

    Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is a psychological disorder involving body manipulation, self-inflicted hunger, and fear of gaining weight.We performed an overview of the existing literature in the field of AN, highlighting the main intrafamilial risk factors for anorexia. We searched the PubMed database by using keywords such as "anorexia" and "risk factors" and "family". After appropriate ...