U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Cambridge Open

Logo of cambridgeopen

On what motivates us: a detailed review of intrinsic v. extrinsic motivation

Laurel s. morris.

1 Department of Psychiatry, Depression and Anxiety Center for Discovery and Treatment, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY 10029 USA

Mora M. Grehl

2 Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122 USA

Sarah B. Rutter

Marishka mehta, margaret l. westwater.

3 Department of Radiology and Biomedical Imaging, Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06510 USA

Motivational processes underlie behaviors that enrich the human experience, and impairments in motivation are commonly observed in psychiatric illness. While motivated behavior is often examined with respect to extrinsic reinforcers, not all actions are driven by reactions to external stimuli; some are driven by ‘intrinsic’ motivation. Intrinsically motivated behaviors are computationally similar to extrinsically motivated behaviors, in that they strive to maximize reward value and minimize punishment. However, our understanding of the neurocognitive mechanisms that underlie intrinsically motivated behavior remains limited. Dysfunction in intrinsic motivation represents an important trans-diagnostic facet of psychiatric symptomology, but due to a lack of clear consensus, the contribution of intrinsic motivation to psychopathology remains poorly understood. This review aims to provide an overview of the conceptualization, measurement, and neurobiology of intrinsic motivation, providing a framework for understanding its potential contributions to psychopathology and its treatment. Distinctions between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are discussed, including divergence in the types of associated rewards or outcomes that drive behavioral action and choice. A useful framework for understanding intrinsic motivation, and thus separating it from extrinsic motivation, is developed and suggestions for optimization of paradigms to measure intrinsic motivation are proposed.

Introduction

Motivation is an integral component of human experience. Children spontaneously explore novel items, and adults autonomously engage in new hobbies, even in the absence of clear extrinsic reinforcers. Thus, not all actions are driven by tangible external stimuli or outcomes, known as ‘extrinsic’ motivation, but are driven by more internal drivers, known as ‘intrinsic’ motivation, where the activity is perceived as its own outcome.

Intrinsically motivated behaviors are computationally similar to extrinsically motivated behaviors, in that they strive to maximize goal attainment and minimize punishment, represented mathematically as value and effort cost functions, respectively (Gottlieb, Lopes, & Oudeyer, 2016 ). However, subjective internal value functions are difficult to characterize, and our understanding of how they are computed and integrated is limited (Gottlieb et al., 2016 ).

Dysfunction in intrinsic motivation represents an important transdiagnostic facet of psychiatric symptomology, which is often classified as distinct psychological constructs, such as apathy in neurological disorders, anhedonia in depression, and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Each of these symptom domains may be underpinned by a shared dysfunction of intrinsic motivation, and interventions targeting intrinsic motivation have the potential to improve treatment outcomes for affected individuals.

However, due to a lack of clear consensus, the contribution of intrinsic motivation to psychiatric disorders remains poorly understood. This review aims to provide an overview of the conceptualization, measurement, and neurobiology of intrinsic motivation, providing a framework for understanding the potential contributions to psychopathology and its treatment.

Historical conceptualizations of intrinsic motivation

During the early 20th century, prominent descriptions of motivation were at odds with each other. Woodworth ( 1918 ) suggested that intrinsic motivation governed activities perpetuated by their own ‘native drive’, whereas Thorndike ( 1911 ) and Watson ( 1913 ) argued that external stimuli governed behavior. Also centered on internal drives, Hull's ( 1943 ) ‘drive theory’ posited that all behaviors were performed to seek or avoid primary biological states, including hunger or pain. However, the drive theory could not explain many behavioral anomalies, such as hungry rats withstanding painful electric shocks to explore a novel environment (Nissen, 1930 ), or rhesus monkeys performing a puzzle task for no biological reason or external reinforcer (Harlow, 1950 ). By narrowly presuming that biological states drive all behavior, drive theory failed to account for instances in which an organism prioritizes higher-order cognitive drives over physiological ones.

The shortcomings of drive theory led to the emergence of alternate theories of intrinsic motivation. Some argued that homeostatic maintenance of optimal biological or cognitive states (Hebb, 1955 ; McClelland & Clark, 1953 ; McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1967 ), or mitigation of incongruency or uncertainty (Festinger, 1957 ; Kagan, 1972 ), drove behavior. However, these theories emphasized external stimuli or cognitive representations of external goal states as key drivers of behavior. In the mid-to-late 20th century, several models underscored the importance of novelty-seeking, interest, and autonomy in driving intrinsic motivation. Novelty-seeking was suggested to energize approach behavior via curiosity and exploration that leads to skill mastery, information attainment, or learning (Kaplan & Oudeyer, 2007 ). Interest and enjoyment in an activity might boost intrinsic motivation by engendering ‘flow’, a prolonged state of focus and enjoyment during task engagement that stretches one's skillset (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975 ; Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2009 ). Finally, self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1980 ) proposed that human needs for competence, achievement, and autonomy drive intrinsic motivation, aligning with observations that intrinsic motivation stems from an internal perceived autonomy during task engagement (DeCharms, 1968 ; Lamal, 2003 ). These models highlight the role of achievement and perceived autonomy (DeCharms, 1968 ) in driving intrinsic motivation, coinciding with current computational frameworks of intrinsic reward (Chew, Blain, Dolan, & Rutledge, 2021 ; Murayama, Matsumoto, Izuma, & Matsumoto, 2010 ).

The introduction of external goals: a shift to extrinsic motivation

While intrinsic motivation has been proposed to be divorced from external reinforcers, our understanding of motivation has been led largely by using external reinforcers as conceptual and experimental tools. Here, we briefly review historical perspectives on external drivers of motivated behavior, outlining prominent goal- and action-focused models of extrinsic motivation.

Early psychological models of extrinsic motivation suggested that ‘will’ and ‘intention’ fostered goal achievement, emphasizing the influence of goal expectation on action and control (Lewin, 1951 ; Tolman, 1932 ). Within this framework, environmental features, as well as an individual's internal state or memory, determine their actions when pursuing a goal, or, more specifically, the cognitive representation of a goal (Kagan, 1972 ). This requires multiple cognitive representations to be developed, maintained, and updated, with a particular reliance on external stimuli and learning (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999 ; Kagan, 1972 ; Kagan & Moss, 1983 ).

Alongside psychological model development, economic models of motivation emerged. These models propose that extrinsic goals, or incentives, elicit motivated behavior via a cost-benefit analysis, where motivated choice occurs when benefits outweigh costs. More recently, behavioral economics has considered how individual personality traits, biases, and irrationalities influence motivated behavior (Strombach, Strang, Park, & Kenning, 2016 ). A recent model (Strombach et al., 2016 ) incorporates various factors into the classical cost-benefit analysis, including traditional intrinsic (e.g. satisfaction) and extrinsic drivers (e.g. money), with negative influences from costs (e.g. effort, pain), which are merged into a single dynamic, subjective and state-dependent factor that drives motivated behavior. Though this approach is powerful, the explicit focus on incentives provides limited explanatory power for various paradoxical behaviors, including rodents overcoming the high cost to self-stimulate certain brain regions (e.g. nucleus accumbens; Nac) or extrinsic reinforcers' dampening effect on intrinsic motivation.

In reinforcement learning models of decision-making, an organism, or agent, learns which actions maximize total reward. This process has been formalized within computational sciences and modern artificial intelligence systems (Sutton & Barto, 1981 ; Witten, 1977 ), where learning and decision-making depend on an extrinsic outcome. One theory suggests that motivated action is driven solely by a need to reduce reward prediction errors (RPEs; Kaplan and Oudeyer, 2007 ), or the mismatch between expectation and outcome (Montague, Dayan, & Sejnowski, 1996 ; Schultz et al., 1997 ). RPEs can also be conceptualized as valuation signals for novel outcomes or unexpected stimuli. RPE-based learning then drives motivated behavior, or action choice, but even if the agent displays intact encoding of action or outcome value, motivated behavior can be dampened by reduced novelty. This highlights the role of novelty, expectation and prediction in learning per se , rather than choice valuation.

In action-focused models of motivation, incentives can trigger approach or avoidance behavior by signaling a potential goal state (Berridge, Robinson, & Aldridge, 2009 ). Incentive motivation thus relies on expectancy, probability, and value of outcomes, which are thought to dictate behavioral choice and decision-making. While greater reliance on stimulus-outcome rather than stimulus-response contingencies has led some to describe incentive motivation as proactive (Beckmann & Heckhausen, 2018 ), others have characterized it as reactive due to the central role of learning from past experience (Bolles, 1972 ). Reliance on an expected outcome was central to behaviorism (Watson, 1913 , 1930 ) and operant conditioning (Skinner, 1938 ), which assume that actions are driven by a reinforcer, and instrumental value is assigned to the behavior itself. Stimulus-response pairs dominate behaviorism and modern theories of habitual behavior (Gläscher, Daw, Dayan, & O'Doherty, 2010 ), where the dependency on previously reinforced actions ultimately governs motivated choice (de Wit et al., 2011 ; Gillan, Robbins, Sahakian, van den Heuvel, & van Wingen, 2016 ; Voon et al., 2014 ). However, this renders behaviors as repetitive, insensitive to punishment and divorced from goals (Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit, & Ersche, 2012 ). Therefore, these action-focused models of motivated behavior almost entirely discount intrinsic motivation since extrinsic motivators usurp control of behavior.

Several limitations of extrinsic motivation models must be considered when attempting to characterize intrinsic motivation. First, for cost-benefit analysis and reinforcement learning, an internal representation of the outcome must first be learned, which requires previous experience of the goal. However, intrinsic motivation can occur for novel outcomes, or behaviors that are uncertain or ambiguous. Second, motivation can occur for activities that may already be fully predictable, marking a significant limitation for reinforcement-learning models of motivation, which assume that reward prediction errors drive learning for motivated action. Third, these frameworks cannot fully explain spontaneous novelty seeking or exploratory behavior, in which no external reward is expected and no cost is overcome (Deci et al., 1999 ; Marsden, Ma, Deci, Ryan, & Chiu, 2014 ).

Separating and integrating intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

A key question is whether intrinsic and extrinsic motivation can, or should, be experimentally or theoretically separated. There is some evidence that they are dissociable constructs at the neural level. The most compelling support comes from case reports of patients with basal ganglia lesions who developed ‘psychic akinesia’, a syndrome characterized by difficulty with self-generated action initiation but no difficulty in performing complex cognitive or motor tasks when prompted (Laplane, Baulac, Widlocher, & Dubois, 1984 ; Lugaresi, Montagna, Morreale, & Gallassi, 1990 ). In patients with alien hand syndrome, medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) lesions lead to a loss of intentional motor control, whereas (pre)-supplementary motor area lesions lead to impairments in implementing motor intentions (Brugger, Galovic, Weder, & Kägi, 2015 ; Nachev, Kennard, & Husain, 2008 ). Preclinical findings further show that photostimulation of GABAergic amygdala projections modulates extrinsic motivation without affecting intrinsically motivated behavior (Seo et al., 2016 ). Together, these findings suggest that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation reflect different cortico-striatal-limbic circuits.

Behavioral research primarily supports the view that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are partially distinct, interacting processes. For example, if the motivation for intrinsic and extrinsic goals were independent constructs, they might demonstrate an additive or subtractive effect on each other (Woodworth, 1921 ). Indeed, the expectation (Liu & Hou, 2017 ) and experience (Badami, VaezMousavi, Wulf, & Namazizadeh, 2011 ) of an extrinsic reinforcer can increase intrinsic motivation. However, reports of the ‘undermining effect’, in which an external reinforcer reduces intrinsic motivation (Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford, 2014 ; Deci, 1971 ; Deci, Benware, & Landy, 1974 ; Lepper & Greene, 1978 ; Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973 ) have sparked debate over how extrinsic reinforcers affect internally-motived behaviors (Cameron & Pierce, 2002 ; Lamal, 2003 ; Lepper, Keavney, & Drake, 1996 ). One explanation for the undermining effect suggests that the presence of an external reinforcer shifts one's perception of the locus of control over the behavior from internal to external (Deci & Ryan, 1980 ). This implicates a key role of agency, or the belief of action ownership, in intrinsic motivation. While controversial, mounting evidence supports this account of the undermining effect, where various extrinsic motivators (e.g. food, social observation; Ryan, 1982 ) decrease intrinsic motivation when their delivery is contingent on task-performance.

A useful framework for parsing motivated action into intrinsic and extrinsic is the Rubicon model of action phases (Heckhausen & Heckhausen, 2018 ; Heckhausen, 1989 ). Within this framework, pre-decisional option deliberation occurs, which is followed by choice intention formation and planning, volitional action, outcome achievement, and evaluation ( Fig. 1 ). Husain and Roiser ( 2018 ) recently proposed a complementary model to deconstruct apathy and anhedonia into underlying cognitive processes, including option generation, anticipation, action initiation, prediction, consumption and learning. This parcellation broadly reflects the five main stages of the Rubicon model: (1) pre-decisional deliberation ( option generation ); (2) intention formation, planning, initiation ( anticipation ); (3) volitional action ( action initiation, prediction ), (4) outcome achievement ( consumption ); and (5) evaluation ( learning ; Figure 1 ). Within these overlapping frameworks, the initial pre-decisional deliberation/option generation phase represents the point at which intrinsic and extrinsic facets of motivation diverge, as early drivers of behavior can be intrinsic (e.g. enjoyment, interest, exploration) or extrinsic (e.g. social reward). The differences between these early drivers highlight a key distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, in which the former is a fundamentally proactive process and the latter reactive.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is S0033291722001611_fig1.jpg

Schematic framework for parsing motivated action. Motivated decision-making and action is parsed into separate phases of choice, action and outcome valuation, combining and building upon separate frameworks including the Rubicon model of action phases, well-established computational mechanisms and a recent cognitive framework describing anhedonia and apathy. During choice valuation, pre-decisional deliberation includes option generation, a cost-benefit analysis and option selection. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation diverges during this early choice valuation phase. Once choice valuation has been computed and an option selected, planning and anticipation occurs. During action valuation, volitional action is initiated and action sustainment or acceleration is maintained. During outcome valuation, outcome achievement and consumption ensue, followed by evaluation based on learning via prediction error (PE) updating. Created with BioRender.com .

If a behavior were intrinsically motivated, the pre-decisional deliberation phase might be determined by biological drives, the need to restore homeostasis (Hebb, 1949 ; Hull, 1943 ), or a state of incongruency resolution (Festinger, 1957 ; Kagan, 1972 ) as described by early theories of intrinsic motivation. In contemporary frameworks, novelty-seeking, exploration, or interest in learning or achievement would render subsequent actions as intrinsically motivated. If a behavior were extrinsically motivated, this pre-decisional deliberation phase would represent the cost-benefit analysis in economic models, prediction-error minimization in reinforcement learning, or effort-reward trade-off computation. Under incentive motivation and behaviorist theories, the pre-decisional deliberation phase would be triggered by conditioned stimuli making conscious deliberation unnecessary and inefficient.

A combination of intrinsic and extrinsic factors likely enters into the pre-decisional deliberation phase to guide motivated behavior ( Fig. 1 ). Although intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are conceptually distinct processes, attempts to formally define them have identified several mechanisms by which they interact, leading to questions about their dissociability. Since they can interact in an additive or subtractive fashion, they may indeed be separate, independent drivers of behavior that are amalgamated during a pre-decisional deliberation phase of behavioral choice.

Measuring intrinsic motivation

Human behavior.

Early attempts to quantify intrinsic motivation were largely based on behavioral observation, wherein intrinsic motivation was measured as free choice of an activity in the absence of an external stimulus or performance rating (Butler & Nisan, 1986 ; Daniel & Esser, 1980 ; Liu & Hou, 2017 ). These studies also implemented self-report measures of participants' interest or enjoyment in an activity. While such measures do capture intrinsic motivation as inherent task enjoyment, they are limited by their qualitative and indirect nature, as well as by variability in participant insight. However, more objective measures are difficult to develop due to the inherently unobservable nature of intrinsic motivation.

Since spontaneous novelty-seeking and exploratory behavior reflect intrinsic motivation, one candidate objective measure may be the explore-exploit paradigm (Gittins & Jones, 1979 ; Robbins, 1952 ). In explore-exploit foraging tasks, participants must choose among various options and either exploit a previously reinforced choice or explore a novel alternative option. An individual's tendency to either explore an environment or exploit their pre-existing knowledge is influenced by perseverance (Von Culin et al., 2014 ), which acts as an indicator of confidence in the absence of immediate reward. Healthy adults flexibly employ a mix of exploitative and exploratory choices, where striatal and prefrontal dopamine signaling is proposed to drive exploration and exploitation, respectively (Badre, Doll, Long,, & Frank,, 2012 ; Daw, O'Doherty, Dayan, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006 ; Mansouri, Koechlin, Rosa, & Buckley, 2017 ). While these tasks capture one's willingness to trade-off exploratory v. exploitative behaviors, they do not measure free-choice exploratory behavior in the absence of explicit reinforcers, which would be most consistent with intrinsic motivation.

Paradigms that allow an individual to choose to explore an environment without extrinsic reinforcers, or to engage in a previously enjoyable or interesting activity, would more closely index intrinsic motivation. Additionally, outcomes that relate to achievement or autonomy, without socially rewarding feedback or monetary outcomes, would also putatively engage intrinsic motivation. Task parameters related to exploration, enjoyment, achievement, and autonomy can each be modulated and computationally modeled to determine their effects on free choice or behavioral activation vigor.

Current computational approaches depend on modeling decision-making, outcome learning, or action-outcome associations to drive our understanding of motivation. Traditional decision-making models often rely on softmax functions to compute values of available actions (Wilson & Collins, 2019 ), where action selection is based on the ‘policy’ of the best outcome. Computationally, an action selection process computes the probability of an action occurring in any state and the expected reward. A policy is developed based on the assumption that motivated actions are performed to increase the probability of rewards and decrease the probability of punishment. Yet, in everyday life, our actions can be motivated by an arbitrary cue that may signal an internal rewarding state. For example, a standard algorithm solving for motivated action assumes that all actions have equal probability, yet this discounts the unknown drivers and evaluators of internal rewards. Hence, they act as limiting factors to the applicability of decision-making models in studies of intrinsic motivation.

Neuroimaging

Functional neuroimaging [e.g., functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG)] offers a measurement modality that may be particularly apt for the study of internally driven processes like intrinsic motivation. Research using fMRI has characterized the neural correlates of various internal processes that lack clear behavioral indicators (e.g. rumination, emotion regulation, pain perception; Zhou et al., 2020 ; Wagner, N'Diaye, Ethofer, and Vuilleumier, 2011 ), yet few studies have assessed the neural correlates of intrinsic motivation in humans, which likely reflects the limitations in its behavioral measurement. Studies have largely assessed intrinsic motivation via comparisons with neural responses to extrinsic reinforcers during fMRI, which can be correlated with self-reported intrinsic motivation (Bengtsson, Lau, & Passingham, 2009 ; Chew et al., 2021 ; Linke et al., 2010 ). Despite the relative paucity of neuroimaging studies that clearly separate intrinsic v. extrinsic motivation, existing work provides preliminary insight into the neural circuitry of intrinsic motivation.

First, extrinsic reinforcers have elicited amygdala, ACC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and ventral striatal (VS) or Nac activity in healthy subjects that was associated with higher self-reported extrinsic motivation but lower self-reported intrinsic motivation (Linke et al., 2010 ). This could suggest that intrinsic motivation relates to a lower sensitivity of these regions to extrinsic reinforcers, general deactivation of these regions, or that the dampening impact of extrinsic reinforcers on intrinsic motivation is subserved by these regions. Others report that intrinsic motivation (operationalized as the amount of free-time spent on a puzzle-task, which did not relate to task enjoyment, interest, or accuracy), was associated with deactivation in the amygdala, dorsal ACC, dorsomedial striatum, and insula during puzzle-task onset (Marsden et al., 2014 ). This is another piece of evidence linking neural deactivation to intrinsic motivation; however, since these tasks were not related to traditional ‘intrinsic motivators’ like task enjoyment, findings may relate to boredom-reduction behavior that might be more related to punishment avoidance rather than intrinsic motivation per se .

Bengtsson et al. ( 2009 ) operationalized intrinsic motivation as task-performance with and without explicit experimental observation during fMRI scanning, which boosted self-reported intrinsic motivation. The authors found greater neural activation of ACC, OFC, and lateral prefrontal cortex during task-performance errors when participants were observed (Bengtsson et al., 2009 ). While implicating a similar network of brain regions as prior studies, these findings cannot be divorced from error-related neural activation modulated by task salience (e.g. observed v. not).

In contrast, Murayama et al . ( 2010 ) provide a more optimal operationalization of intrinsic motivation, in which participants performed a task that was previously rated as inherently interesting, and successful task performance served as the intrinsic reward. During fMRI scanning, feedback for both extrinsic (monetary feedback) and intrinsic (accuracy feedback) rewards elicited VS activation. Participants then had the option to perform the same task without feedback, and intrinsic motivation was operationalized as time spent on the second version of the task. During the second session, VS activation was only diminished for extrinsic rewards, which could reflect reduced VS habituation to intrinsic rewards (Murayama et al., 2010 , 2015 ). Additionally, greater reductions in neural responses to extrinsic reinforcers were related to lower intrinsic motivation (i.e. task engagement time outside of the scanner), suggesting that neural habituation to extrinsic reinforcers may relate to lower intrinsic motivation. A recent computational neuroimaging study modeled intrinsic rewards as successful spatial-motor task performance without experienced errors, which was divorced from learning (Chew et al., 2021 ). This modeling of intrinsic rewards was akin to the accuracy feedback operationalization of Murayama et al . ( 2010 ). Both extrinsic (monetary) reward and intrinsic performance-based rewards (successful task completion) recruited vmPFC activation, which related to subjective happiness (Chew et al., 2021 ). Although limited in their ability to dissociate activation from task performance per se and explicit feedback related to achievement, these studies are the closest examples of objective measures of intrinsic motivation, and they suggest that putative reward-processing regions (VS, vmPFC) encode intrinsic rewards.

Complementary studies have examined how curiosity, or the intrinsic motivation to learn, modulates neural responses and influences memory recall (Gruber, Gelman, & Ranganath, 2014 ; Kang et al., 2009 ). High-curiosity states augment midbrain and v. activity (Gruber et al., 2014 ), as well as bilateral caudate (Kang et al., 2009 ) and anterior insula (Lee & Reeve, 2017 ) responses, which may improve learning and memory. As these paradigms index intrinsic motivation independently from a rewarding outcome, they perhaps provide the strongest support for partially overlapping circuits of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation.

The brain's dopamine system supports a range of appetitive and aversive motivational processes, including behavioral activation, exertion of effort, and sustained task engagement (Diederen & Fletcher, 2020 ; Salamone, Yohn, López-Cruz, San Miguel, & Correa, 2016 ). The mesolimbic pathway, projecting from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to limbic regions, including the Nac, amygdala, and hippocampus, facilitates reinforcement and associative learning by acting as a ‘Go’ signal for foraging or exploration (Huang, Lv, & Wu, 2016 ). Although it has long been known that dopamine transmission subserves motivational processes, some evidence suggests that it is particularly important for intrinsic motivation. For example, mesolimbic dopamine contributes to exploration for the sake of interest (DeYoung, 2013 ; Panksepp & Moskal, 2008 ), and novel and unexpected stimuli elicit phasic dopamine spikes in rodents (Fiorillo, 2003 ; Hooks & Kalivas, 1994 ; Schultz, 1998 ). In patients with depression, deep-brain stimulation of dopaminergic brain regions including the Nac (Schlaepfer et al., 2007 ) and the mesolimbic dopamine projections from the VTA (Fenoy et al., 2018 ) increased subjective interest in, and motivational energy for, previously enjoyable activities (Schlaepfer et al., 2007 ). Dopamine has also been associated with intrinsically motivated flow states (de Manzano et al., 2013 ; Gyurkovics et al., 2016 ).

However, since VTA dopamine spiking is reduced for expected events (Schultz, 1998 ), it may not be a strong candidate neural mechanism for intrinsic motivation, which can occur for predictable activities. Efforts to reconcile the role of dopamine in learning and motivation suggest that while phasic cell firing signals RPEs (Kim et al., 2020 ), phasic dopamine release and local modulation in key regions, such as the VS/NAc, relates to approach motivation (Berke, 2018 ; Mohebi et al., 2019 ). Indeed, while VTA dopamine cell firing occurs during reward prediction, only NAc dopamine release covaries with reward availability and ramps up during approach and consumption of reward (Mohebi et al., 2019 ). Moreover, increasing dopamine in rodents increases their willingness to exert effort, and this has since been replicated across species, including via pharmacological manipulation in humans (Salamone, Correa, Farrar, & Mingote, 2007 ; Treadway & Zald, 2011 ). This suggests that, while VTA dopamine spiking underpins reward prediction and learning, it is local NAc dopamine release that encodes motivational drive.

Opioids, norepinephrine, and related neurotransmitter systems

Though a comprehensive account of the neurotransmitter systems subserving motivated behavior is beyond the scope of this review, we note that endogenous opioid and cannabinoid systems may uniquely modulate intrinsically motivated behavior. For example, mu- and delta-opioid receptor activation underlies the pleasurable effects of opioid and non-opioid drugs of abuse (Berrendero, Robledo, Trigo, Martín-García, & Maldonado, 2010 ; Trigo, Martin-García, Berrendero, Robledo, & Maldonado, 2010 ), as well as primary reinforcers (Hsu et al., 2013 ; Kelley & Berridge, 2002 ). Activation of mu-opioid receptors has also been shown to mediate motivational states following delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) administration in rodents (Ghozland et al., 2002 ), likely via interactions with the mesolimbic dopamine system. Further evidence implicates antidepressant effects of endogenous opioids in both animals and humans (Peciña et al., 2018 ), which many partly reflect improved intrinsic motivation (e.g. time mice spent swimming during the forced swim test; Kastin, Scollan, Ehrensing, Schally, and Coy, 1978 ). Additionally, the endocannabinoid system interacts with both endogenous opioid and dopaminergic systems to influence intrinsic motivation, such as social play (Trezza et al., 2012 ; Trezza & Vanderschuren, 2008 ), and voluntary exercise, in rodents (Dubreucq, Koehl, Abrous, Marsicano, & Chaouloff, 2010 ). Since these systems have been primarily examined in animal models, pharmacological manipulation in humans would be an important next step in delineating the contribution of opioid and endocannabinoid systems to intrinsic v. extrinsic motivation.

Intrinsic motivation and psychiatry: focus on anhedonia

Problems with motivation are observed across many neuropsychiatric disorders, and these often correspond to distinct symptoms ( Table 1 ). This section focuses on anhedonia, a reduced ability to experience pleasure (Ribot, 1986 ), as a prevalent clinical manifestation of deficient intrinsic and extrinsic motivation.

Explicit studies of ‘intrinsic motivation’ in neuropsychiatric disorders

DisorderRelated symptomCohortMeasureEvidenceReference
Depressive disordersAnhedonia  = 537 undergraduate studentsMotivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire, 9-item intrinsic value subscale, Pintrich and De Groot ( ).Academic IM was negatively associated with depression and stress.Huang et al. ( )
 = 95 MDDAutonomous and Controlled Motivations for Treatment Questionnaire.Autonomous motivation predicted a higher probability of remission and lower post-treatment depression severity among patients across three outpatient treatments: 16 sessions of manualized interpersonal therapy, cognitive–behavior therapy, or pharmacotherapy with clinical management.Zuroff et al. ( )
 = 59 subthreshold MDDPerformance of a stopwatch task based on intrinsic motivation during fMRI scanningBehavioral activation therapy (identify and complete enjoyable activities that provide a sense of achievement) increased activation and connectivity in frontostriatal regions, associated with improved sensitivity to rewards.Mori et al. ( )
 = 106 healthy volunteersIntrinsic Motivation Inventory: two items from the interest/enjoyment subscale.Participants who were unable to differentiate between positive emotions had stronger links between positive emotions and intrinsic motivation, whereas subjects that were able to differentiate between negative emotions showed a weaker link between negative emotions and intrinsic motivation.Vandercammen, Hofmans, and Theuns ( )
 = 33 treatment resistant MDDIntrinsic Motivation Inventory.Examined the effectiveness of cognitive remediation with supplemental Internet-based homework, Treatment consisted of 10 weeks of weekly group sessions and daily online cognitive exercises completed at home. Homework completion was associated with worse depressive symptoms and not intrinsic motivation.Bowie et al. ( )
 = 300 working adultsRated 10 job aspects on 6-point scales related in intrinsic (e.g. self growth) and extrinsic (e.g. pay, social status) job features.Intrinsic work motivation was associated with higher job satisfaction. Higher extrinsic motivation was associated with higher depression scores.Lu ( )
 = 215 elite team-sport athletesSport Motivation Scale II, Perceived Motivational Climate in Sport Questionnaire II, Basic Need Satisfaction in Sport Scale.Intrinsic regulation of sport motivation was related to higher depressive symptoms.Sheehan, Herring, and Campbell ( )
 = 236 healthy adolescentsPerceived Teacher Autonomy Support Questionnaire, General Basic Needs Satisfaction Scale.Teacher autonomy support increased psychological needs satisfaction and intrinsic motivation for school engagement, which, in turn, was associated with decreased anxiety and depression scores.Yu, Li, Wang, and Zhang ( )
 = 115 healthy childrenPerception of Success, Enjoyment of the Practice of Sports, Achievement Motivation in Physical Education.In 11-12-year-old children, skill mastery ‘intrinsic’ motivation training increased task enjoyment, perceived ability and effort, as well as baseline anxiety.Cecchini et al. ( )
Schizophrenia spectrum disorders‘Negative symptoms' in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and other psychotic illnesses span a range of behaviors again underscored by a lack of self-generated initiation, not limited to alogia, avolition, social withdrawal and affective blunting.  = 66 SCZ or SZA;  = 44 controlsMotivational Trait Questionnaire: 3 components of intrinsic motivation (personal mastery, competitive excellence, motivation related to anxiety).In control subjects only, IM was related to cognitive performance. Both groups showed positive relationships between intrinsic motivation and approach and avoidance behaviors.Barch, Yodkovik, Sypher-Locke, and Hanewinkel ( )
 = 120 SCZQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.In patients who were at the start of outpatient psychosocial rehabilitation programs, IM mediated the relationship between neurocognition and psychosocial functioning.Nakagami, Xie, Hoe, and Brekke ( )
 = 57 SCZ or SZAIntrinsic Motivation Inventory.Intrinsically motivating instructional techniques during difficult task learning increased intrinsic motivation for the task, self-efficacy and achievement.Choi and Medalia ( )
 = 130 SCZ or SZAQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.In patients from 4 community-based, psychosocial rehabilitation programs in Los Angeles, USA, IM was dynamic over time. Baseline IM predicted improvements in neurocognition, and change in IM was associated with change in psychosocial functioning.Nakagami, Hoe, and Brekke ( )
 = 18 SCZ;  = 17 healthy controlsEnjoyable stop watch timing task where subjects stop a watch at an exact time. In this task, the watch starts automatically and must be stopped with a single button press within 50 ms of the 5s time point. The total number of successful trials is continuously displayed. A control task is passive watch viewing with a single button press when the watch stops.Participants with SCZ showed lower IM for the task. Lateral prefrontal cortex activity during the cue period was associated with higher IM.Takeda et al. ( )
 = 75 SCZQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.High IM related to greater metacognitive mastery in a sample of patients with chronic illness.Vohs and Lysaker ( )
 = 32 SCZ in functional remissionIntrinsic Motivation Inventory for Schizophrenia Research.IM was associated with metacognition and subjects with greater intrinsic motivation and metacognition improved.Tas, Brown, Esen-Danaci, Lysaker, and Brüne ( )
 = 58 SCZ spectrum disordersQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.IM was linked to extraversion, neuroticism and negative symptoms in this all-male cohort.Vohs, Lysaker, and Nabors ( )
 = 12 SCZIntrinsic Motivation Inventory.Among patients in outpatient treatment, IM for a cognitive task was associated with performance.Fervaha, Agid, Foussias, and Remington ( )
 = 166 SCZ spectrum disordersQuality of Life Scale.All participants attended psychosocial rehabilitation programs in a diverse urban community. IM fully mediated the relationship between functioning and negative, disorganized, and global symptoms, and partially mediated the relationship between positive symptoms and functioning.Yamada, Lee, Dinh, Barrio, and Brekke ( )
 = 49 SCZ or SZAIntrinsic Motivation Inventory for Schizophrenia Research.Perceived program value was the only predictor of attendance and cognitive improvement increased with improvements in program interest. Motivational changes over time were variable between subjects.Bryce et al. ( )
 = 125 psychotic disorderQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.IM mediated the relationship between poor metacognition and impaired functioning.Luther et al. ( )
 = 40 FEP;  = 66 prolonged psychosisQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity; PANSS.FEP patients had higher IM and lower amotivation levels than the prolonged psychosis group. IM was associated with lower amotivation in both groups.Luther, Lysaker, Firmin, Breier, and Vohs ( )
 = 535 SCZ with comorbid SUDsQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity.The IM measure was reliable for this cohort. IM was negatively associated with alcohol and drug use severity, and changes in IM over time predicted alcohol/drug use severity.Bahorik, Eack, Cochran, Greeno, and Cornelius ( )
 = 858 SCZ;  = 576 SCZ with comorbid SUDsHeinrichs-Carpenter Quality of Life ScaleIM was negatively related to the likelihood of any alcohol or substance use at baseline. Reduced IM was associated with greater likelihood of alcohol or substance use at 6-month follow-up, whereas greater IM was protective against drug use.Bahorik, Greeno, Cochran, Cornelius, and Eack ( )
 = 71 SCZ spectrum disordersQuality of Life Scale: Sum of 3 items, purpose, motivation, and curiosity; Intrinsic Motivation Inventory.The two IM measures were not significantly correlated among patients in an outpatient rehabilitation program. Only the QLS IM score was associated with rehabilitation outcomes.Choi, Choi, Felice Reddy, and Fiszdon ( )
Parkinson's diseaseApathy- In Parkinson's disease (PD), apathy describes reduced interest and execution of goal-directed activities, unrelated to depressive emotional states or cognitive impairment. There is an absence of spontaneous auto-activation, or self-generated behavior. three subtypes of disrupted processing: ‘cognitive’, ‘emotional-affective’, and ‘auto-activation’.  = 27 PD;  = 27 healthy controlsCuriosity for resolving uncertainty, despite negative outcomes, via choice to view or skip negative images.The PD group viewed the images less frequently under the certain and uncertain conditions. The amount of pictures viewed was positively associated with the distribution of dopamine transporters in the striatum.Shigemune et al. ( )
 = 28 PDParticipants stood on a stabilometer and aimed to maintain a horizontal platform position during each 30s trial, with the self-control group having autonomy to choose to use a balance pole while the yoked group used the balance pole on a set schedule.The self-control group were more accurate and more motivated to learn the task compared to the yoked group.Chiviacowsky, Wulf, Lewthwaite, and Campos ( )
 = 28 PDIntrinsic Motivation Inventory.In PD patients at general psychiatric outpatient clinics in Nanjing, those assigned to core stability training showed (1) higher IM compared to the home exercise group, and (2) increased interest and pleasure, perceived merit, effort and general motivation at the 8-week follow-up.Sun and Chen ( )
 = 57 PD Regulatory Mode Questionnaire.Patients showed reduced assessment motivation only.Foerde, Braun, Higgins, and Shohamy ( )
SUD, AUD, and gambling disorderOne symptom of SUDs and AUD relates to individuals forgoing important work-related, social or recreational activities due to their substance use. Among others, this symptom relates to reduced goal-directed behaviors, which may indicate impaired IM.  = 454 SUDCircumstances, Motivation, Readiness, and Suitability instrument, Norwegian version.In patients from 5 inpatient SUD centers in Norway, higher IM for changing substance use was associated with lower dropout risk.Andersson, Steinsbekk, Walderhaug, Otterholt, and Nordfjærn ( )
 = 15 SUD adolescents;  = 15 caretakersInterview about treatment experience coded for dyadic categories: ; ; both or / ; and disagreement/conflicting.Adolescent patients with higher IM were more engaged in treatment.Cornelius, Earnshaw, Menino, Bogart, and Levy ( )
 = 611 SUDReasons for Quitting Questionnaire adapted for use with substance users other than tobacco smokers.Intrinsic self-concept issues were related to abstinence. IM was higher than IM in this sample of treatment-seeking individuals with poly-substance use disordersDowney, Rosengren, and Donovan ( )
 = 252 undergraduate studentsGambling Motives Scale & General Causality Orientation ScaleIn an at-risk sample, greater autonomy was associated with lower problematic gambling, in part, due to a lower tendency of chasing losses.Rodriguez, Neighbors, Rinker, and Tackett ( )
 = 887 regular gamblersGlobal Motivation Scale & Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration ScaleGreater IM was weakly associated with increased problematic gambling.Mills, Li Anthony, and Nower ( )
 = 94 undergraduate studentsIntrinsic–Extrinsic Aspirations Scale.IM and sense of control were positively associated with adaptive motivation and negatively with alcohol intake.Shamloo and Cox ( )
 = 1137 smokersReasons for Quitting scale.In this population-based sample, higher IM relative to EM was associated with greater readiness to quit and successful smoking cessation at 1-year follow-up.Curry, Grothaus, and McBride ( )
 = 1961 adolescentsRatings of emotional engagement.In a diverse adolescent sample, positive time attitudes were indirectly associated with less marijuana use via IM, engagement, and less alcohol use. The indirect effect of positive time attitudes on engagement via IM was significant and substantial. Negative time attitudes and IM were indirectly associated with less marijuana use via behavioral engagement.Froiland, Worrell, Olenchak, and Kowalski ( )

Note: Cohort abbreviations: AUD, alcohol use disorder; FEP, first-episode psychosis; MDD, major depressive disorder; PD, Parkinson's disease; SCZ, schizophrenia; SUDs, substance use disorders; SZA, schizoaffective disorder. Evidence abbreviations: EM, extrinsic motivation; IM, intrinsic motivation.

In the Diagnostic and Statistical Model of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM- 5 ), anhedonia serves as one of two cardinal symptoms of depressive disorders, where it is defined as the ‘loss of interest or pleasure in all, or almost all, activities’, (American Psychiatric Association, 2013 ). The second cardinal symptom relates to persistent depressed mood. Approximately one-third of individuals with depression report clinically significant anhedonia (Pelizza & Ferrari, 2009 ), and these individuals are at-risk for poorer treatment outcomes, including nonresponse, relapse, and increased suicidality, relative to their non-anhedonic peers (Morris, Bylsma, & Rottenberg, 2009 ; Nierenberg et al., 1999 ).

Anhedonia remains an important clinical target that, by definition, implicates perturbations in intrinsically-motivated behavior, yet most empirical studies of anhedonia and motivation have investigated their relationship using extrinsic reinforcers. Findings broadly support theories of reward dysfunction in depression (reviewed by Sescousse, Caldú, Segura, and Dreher, 2013 ; Roiser & Husain, 2018; Borsini, Wallis, Zunszain, Pariante, and Kempton, 2020 ), where anhedonia has been associated with a reduced bias toward a monetary reward in individuals with depression (Liu et al., 2011 ) and their first-degree relatives (Liu et al., 2016 ). Children who are at-risk for depression show reduced VS and anterior insula responses to monetary gains, implicating blunted reward sensitivity as an antecedent to anhedonia (Luking, Pagliaccio, Luby, & Barch, 2016 ). Moreover, vmPFC responses during unexpected reward receipt may indirectly relate to anhedonia in depressed patients by modulating task motivation (Segarra et al., 2016 ). Interestingly, reward sensitivity disturbances in depression might not extend to aberrant reward learning (Huys, Pizzagalli, Bogdan, & Dayan, 2013 ) where adults with moderate depression show intact VS RPE-signaling during probabilistic learning (Rutledge et al., 2017 ). Nevertheless, there have been suggestions that perturbations in domains more related to intrinsic motivation, such as model-based future planning or effort initiation and invigoration, may be key in underlying anhedonia (Berwian et al., 2020 ; Cooper, Arulpragasam, & Treadway, 2018 ; Rutledge et al., 2017 ). Finally, affect can also alter both the valence and evaluation of an activity, which can, in turn, modulate the likelihood of selecting a more inherently interesting task (Isen & Reeve, 2006 ). Anhedonic individuals have more pessimistic likelihood estimates and reduced positive affective forecasts relative to controls while also demonstrating greater reliance on negative emotion during future-oriented cognition (Marroquín & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2015 ).

While few studies have implemented objective measures of intrinsic motivation in studying anhedonia, recent work links this symptom with difficulties with representations of future states during early stages of motivated behavior (Moutoussis et al., 2018 ). Since intrinsic motivation is driven more by proactive factors as opposed to the more reactive domain of extrinsic motivation, parsing future-oriented decision-making might provide novel insights not only into mechanisms of intrinsic motivation but also anhedonia. When considering the pre-decisional deliberation phase of motivated action ( Fig. 1 ), the representation of a future state may be critical for distinguishing intrinsic v. extrinsic motivation. For example, disrupted representations of intrinsic reinforcers (e.g. autonomy, achievement, task enjoyment, novelty seeking), energy expenditure (Treadway, Cooper, & Miller, 2019 ; Winch, Moberly, & Dickson, 2014 ), or fatigue (Müller, Klein-Flügge, Manohar, Husain, & Apps, 2021 ) might disrupt choice deliberation and interrupt ensuing stages of motivation. This could critically determine the capacity for self-generated, intrinsically-motivated actions (Husain & Roiser, 2018 ). However, relatively few studies have examined this distinction. One study developed a cognitive task that aimed to capture separate measures of self-generated ( intrinsic ) v. externally generated ( extrinsic ) motivation during the option-generation phase (Morris et al., 2020 ). This distinction linked self-generated option generation (intrinsic motivation) to anhedonia symptoms in healthy adults (Morris et al., 2020 ). However, this task still relies on extrinsic rewards, and there is a need for improved tasks that index both behavioral and neural correlates of intrinsic drivers of motivated behavior.

Summary and future directions

In this review, we summarize how intrinsic motivation has been conceptualized, measured, and related to neural function to elucidate its role in psychopathology. In contrast to extrinsic motivation, which has been rapidly incorporated into prominent cognitive, computational, and neurobiological models of human behavior, knowledge of intrinsic motivation remains limited due to evolving conceptualizations, imprecise measurement, and incomplete characterization of its biological correlates. We identify three potential areas of interest for future research.

First, additional objective measures of intrinsically motivation should be developed. This remains challenging experimentally since even the closest approximations of intrinsic motivation (Murayama et al., 2010 ; Rutledge et al., 2017 ) define the construct relative to extrinsic motivation, and other paradigms (e.g. exploration/exploitation tasks) rely on the presence of extrinsic reinforcers. Rather than defining motivated behavior as intrinsic or extrinsic, a more tractable approach might be to consider separate drivers of behavior that can be intrinsic or extrinsic. Future paradigms could index intrinsic motivation by characterizing the effects of intrinsic v. extrinsic reinforcers on motivation for an activity that is enjoyable. Such a design would enable more complex modeling of the effects of distinct reinforcers, and interactions between them, on motivated behavior, which would resolve inconsistencies surrounding the impact of extrinsic reinforcers on intrinsic motivation. For example, monetary incentives might reduce motivation only when a perceived agency is low, or when task enjoyment is high. These interactions might explain paradoxical observations like the undermining effect.

Second, computational models are needed to characterize intrinsic motivation. Computational models of motivation have been successfully implemented in studies of extrinsic motivation, yet few are appropriate for intrinsic motivation due to a focus on action-outcome associations. However, if the intrinsic reward were operationalized as a measurable outcome (e.g. completion of an enjoyable task), reinforcement-learning models could estimate how intrinsic reward value is represented. Advancements in the computational area could significantly improve understanding of the latent processes underlying (ab)normal decision-making, thereby identifying novel therapeutic targets.

Third, although evidence supports the bifurcation of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation at the psychological level, findings at the neural level are more equivocal. Given the overarching role of the mesolimbic dopamine system in learning, reward value estimation, and exploratory behavior, it is perhaps unsurprising that current evidence supports largely overlapping neural circuits for intrinsically and extrinsically motivated behavior. One potential avenue involves targeted pharmacological manipulations or neuromodulation of cortico-limbic circuits to determine if intrinsically and extrinsically motivated behaviors can be systematically modulated in humans. By elucidating the neural circuits of distinct motivational processes and their associations with specific symptom profiles, this approach would improve targeted interventions for highly heterogenous and debilitating disorders like depression.

Financial support

All authors report no financial disclosures. This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (LSM, grant number K01MH120433) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (MLW, T32DA022975).

  • DOI: 10.36713/EPRA6848
  • Corpus ID: 236343668

LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES MOTIVATION

  • Alex Acquah , Takyi Kwabena Nsiah , +1 author B. Otoo
  • Published 21 May 2021
  • Business, Psychology

38 Citations

Critically analyzing the role of total rewards and compensa-tion in increasing employee motivation, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in distance education: a self-determination perspective, what is entrepreneurial fear of failure, digitalization of human resource management and the significance of motivation in acquiring digital skills for employee retention, intrinsic satisfaction and turnover intentions: the moderating roles of collegial and managerial values congruence, the effect of work motivation, employee engagement and work facilities on employee satisfaction at pt marinal indo prima, the influence of compensation, person job fit, non-physical work environment on job satisfaction through work motivation, motivation in relation to teachers’ performance, the influence of existence–relatedness–growth need satisfaction and job burnout of young university teachers: the mediating role of job satisfaction, achievement motivation among students in indonesia: what is the role of teacher-student relations, peer relations and moderation of collectivist culture, related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

behavsci-logo

Article Menu

literature review motivation theory

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Effective teacher professional development for school-based mental health promotion: a review of the literature.

literature review motivation theory

1. Introduction

1.1. teachers’ role in smhp, 1.2. teacher preparation for smhp, 1.3. capacity building is critical for implementation, 1.4. effective teacher pd for smhp, 1.5. purpose of current review.

  • What are the components of high-quality teacher PD for SMHP that lead to effective implementation of innovations?
  • What is the evidence base for these components?

2. Materials and Methods

3.1. training, 3.1.1. interactive, 3.1.2. staff input, 3.2. technical assistance, 3.2.1. goals, 3.2.2. approach, 3.2.3. activities, 3.3. pd delivery considerations, 3.3.1. online vs. in-person format, 3.3.2. dosage, 3.3.3. expertise of the provider.

GoalsApproachActivities
StudyTrainingStaff InputInteractiveTechnical AssistanceDevelop SkillsMotivationGeneralization/AdaptationCollaborativeIndividualizedData-drivenStrengths-BasedModelingObservation/FeedbackReflectionProblem Solving
Becker et al., 2013 [ ]X XXX XXX XXXX
Berkowitz, 2011 [ ]XXXX
Chafouleas et al., 2016 [ ]XX XX
Domitrovich et al., 2012 [ ] X XX
Edgar, 2013 [ ] XXX XX X
Elias, 2008 [ ]XX X X XX
Eppler-Wolff et al., 2019 [ ]XXXX XXX
Erchul, 2015 [ ] XXXXXXX XX X
Flaspohler et al., 2006 [ ]X X
Gibson et al., 2014 [ ]X XXX XX X
Hamre et al., 2012 [ ]X XX X X
Hemmeter et al., 2018 [ ]X X XXX XX
Lee et al., 2014 [ ] X X XXX XXX
Owens et al., 2014 [ ]XXXXXX X X X
Pas et al., 2015 [ ] X XX XX X XXXX
Schultz et al., 2015 [ ] XXX X X X
Silva & Gimbert, 2001 [ ] X XX
Swain-Bradway et al., 2015 [ ]X XX X X X
Vetter, 2008 [ ] X X
GoalsApproachActivitiesOther Considerations
StudyFoundational TrainingStaff InputInteractiveTechnical AssistanceDevelop SkillsMotivationGeneralization/AdaptationCollaborativeIndividualizedData-drivenStrengths-BasedModelingObservation/FeedbackReflectionProblem SolvingOnlineDosageExpertise of Provider
Ashworth et al., 2018 [ ] Q QQQ QQQ XQ
Becker et al., 2014 [ ]T TXX X XX XTX
Bradshaw et al., 2012 [ ]T TX X X X
Cappella et al., 2011 [ ]QQQX QXQQQQX X
Cunningham et al., 2013 [ ]P PPP P
Hamre et al., 2012 [ ]X T X TX XPT
Hough, 2011 [ ]T PX P TP
Kutcher et al., 2013 [ ]T X X
Livet et al., 2018 [ ]X X X XX QT
Owens et al., 2017 [ ]X PTT XTXXTXTX X
Pas et al., 2015 [ ]X T TX XX T
Reinke et al., 2012 [ ]X XXXT TTXXXX X X
Steed & Durand, 2013 [ ] XXT X XXX X
Sutherland et al., 2015 [ ]X TX XXX XXXX X
Wanless et al., 2013 [ ]X Q Q QX XQ

4. Discussion

4.1. future directions: research to practice, 4.2. future directions: practice to research, 5. conclusions, author contributions, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Youth Risk Behavior Survey: Data Summary and Trends Report 2011–2021. 2023. Available online: www.cdc.gov/yrbs (accessed on 20 October 2023).
  • Office of the Surgeon General (OSG). Protecting Youth Mental Health: The U.S. Surgeon General’s Advisory ; US Department of Health and Human Services: Washington, DC, USA, 2021. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenberg, M.T.; Domitrovich, C.; Bumbarger, B. The Prevention of Mental Disorders in School-Aged Children: Current State of the Field. Prev. Treat. 2001 , 4 , 1–62. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Swain-Bradway, J.; Johnson, J.; Eber, L.; Barret, S.; Weist, M.D. Interconnecting School Mental Health and School-Wide Positive Behavior Support. In School Mental Health: Global Challenges and Opportunities ; Kutcher, S., Wei, Y., Weist, M.D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 282–298. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durlak, J.A.; Weissberg, R.P.; Dymnicki, A.B.; Taylor, R.D.; Schellinger, K.B. The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Dev. 2011 , 82 , 405–432. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hoagwood, K.E.; Olin, S.S.; Kerker, B.D.; Kratochwill, T.R.; Crowe, M.; Saka, N. Empirically based school interventions targeted at academic and mental health functioning. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 2007 , 15 , 65–92. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Rones, M.; Hoagwood, K. School-based mental health services: A research review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2000 , 3 , 223–241. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Weare, K.; Nind, M. Mental health promotion and problem prevention in schools: What does the evidence say? Health Promot. Int. 2011 , 26 , i29–i69. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kaufman, S.E.R.; Hulleman, C.S. SEL in Elementary School Settings: Identifying Mechanisms that Matter. In The Handbook of Social and Emotional Learning ; Durlak, J.A., Domitrovich, C.E., Weissberg, R.P., Gullotta, T.P., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 151–166. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fixsen, D.L.; Blase, K.A.; Naoom, S.F.; Wallace, F. Core implementation components. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 2009 , 19 , 531–540. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Flaspohler, P.D.; Duffy, J.; Wandersman, A.; Stillman, L.; Maras, M.A. Unpacking prevention capacity: An intersection of research-to-practice models and community-centered models. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2008 , 41 , 182–196. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lyon, A.R.; Bruns, E.J. From evidence to impact: Joining our best school mental health practices with our best implementation strategies. Sch. Ment. Health 2019 , 11 , 106–114. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Flaspohler, P.D.; Anderson-Butcher, D.; Paternite, C.E.; Weist, M.; Wandersman, A. Community science and expanded school mental health: Bridging the research-to-practice gap to promote child well-being and academic success. Educ. Child Psychol. 2006 , 23 , 27–41. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lee, J.; Frey, A.J.; Herman, K.; Reinke, W. Motivational interviewing as a framework to guide school-based coaching. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2014 , 7 , 225–239. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Owens, J.S.; Lyon, A.R.; Brandt, N.E.; Warner, C.M.; Nadeem, E.; Spiel, C.; Wagner, M. Implementation science in school mental health: Key constructs in a developing research agenda. Sch. Ment. Health 2014 , 6 , 99–111. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Weist, M.D. Fulfilling the promise of school-based mental health: Moving toward a Public Mental Health Promotion approach. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005 , 33 , 735–741. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • ScHARR Public Health Collaborating Centre. Systematic Review of the Effectiveness of Universal Interventions Which Aim to Promote Emotional and Social Wellbeing in Secondary Schools ; The University of Sheffield: Sheffield, UK, 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Franklin, C.G.S.; Kim, J.S.; Ryan, T.N.; Kelly, M.S.; Montgomery, K.L. Teacher involvement in school mental health interventions: A systematic review. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2012 , 34 , 973–982. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Han, S.S.; Weiss, B. Sustainability of teacher implementation of school-based mental health programs. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005 , 33 , 665–679. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Baskin, T.W.; Slaten, C.D.; Sorenson, C.; Glover-Russell, J.; Merson, D.N. Does youth psychotherapy improve academically related outcomes? A meta-analysis. J. Couns. Psychol. 2010 , 57 , 290–296. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Domitrovich, C.E.; Bradshaw, C.P.; Poduska, J.M.; Hoagwood, K.; Buckley, J.A.; Olin, S.; Romanelli, L.H.; Leaf, P.J.; Greenberg, M.T.; Ialongo, N.S. Maximizing the implementation quality of evidence-based preventive interventions in schools: A conceptual framework. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2008 , 1 , 6–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gottfredson, D.C.; Gottfredson, G.D. Quality of school-based prevention programs: Results from a national survey. J. Res. Crime Delinq. 2002 , 39 , 3–35. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ball, A.; Anderson-Butcher, D. Understanding Teachers’ Perceptions of Student Support Systems in Relation to Teachers’ Stress. Child. Sch. 2014 , 36 , 221–229. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ransford, C.R.; Greenberg, M.T.; Domitrovich, C.E.; Small, M.; Jacobson, L. The role of teachers’ psychological experiences and perceptions of curriculum supports on the implementation of a social and emotional learning curriculum. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2009 , 38 , 510–532. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koller, J.R.; Bertel, J.M. Responding to Today’s Mental Health Needs of Children, Families and Schools: Revisiting the Preservice Training and Preparation of School-Based Personnel. Educ. Treat. Child. 2006 , 29 , 197–217. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/42899882 (accessed on 2 August 2019).
  • Brown, E.L.; Phillippo, K.L.; Weston, K.; Rodger, S. United States and Canada pre-service teacher certification standards for student mental health: A comparative case study. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2019 , 80 , 71–82. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ball, A.; Iachini, A.L.; Bohnenkamp, J.H.; Togno, N.M.; Brown, E.L.; Hoffman, J.A.; George, M.W. School mental health content in state in-service K-12 teaching standards in the United States. Teach. Teach. Educ. 2016 , 60 , 312–320. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • State, T.M.; Kern, L.; Starosta, K.M.; Mukherjee, A.D. Elementary Pre-service Teacher Preparation in the Area of Social, Emotional, and Behavioral Problems. Sch. Ment. Health 2011 , 3 , 13–23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Andrews, A.; McCabe, M.; Wideman-Johnston, T. Mental health issues in the schools: Are educators prepared? J. Ment. Health Train. Educ. Pract. 2014 , 9 , 261–272. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Moon, J.; Williford, A.; Mendenhall, A. Educators’ perceptions of youth mental health: Implications for training and the promotion of mental health services in schools. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2017 , 73 , 384–391. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Roeser, R.W.; Midgley, C. Teachers’ views of issues involving students’ mental health. Elem. Sch. J. 1997 , 98 , 115–133. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Walter, H.J.; Gouze, K.; Lim, K.G. Teachers’ beliefs about mental health needs in inner city elementary schools. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2006 , 45 , 61–68. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kidger, J.; Gunnell, D.; Biddle, L.; Campbell, R.; Donovan, J. Part and parcel of teaching? Secondary school staff’s views on supporting student emotional health and well-being. Br. Educ. Res. J. 2010 , 36 , 919–935. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Phillippo, K.L.; Kelly, M.S. On the Fault Line: A Qualitative Exploration of High School Teachers’ Involvement with Student Mental Health Issues. Sch. Ment. Health 2014 , 6 , 184–200. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chinman, M.; Hannah, G.; Wandersman, A.; Ebener, P.; Hunter, S.B.; Imm, P.; Sheldon, J. Developing a community science research agenda for building community capacity for effective preventive interventions. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2005 , 35 , 143–157. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Dalton, J.H.; Elias, M.J.; Wandersman, A. Prevention and Promotion: Implementing Programs. In Community Psychology: Linking Individuals and Communities , 2nd ed.; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 360–397. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Franks, R.P.; Schroeder, J. Implementation Science: Defining the New Frontier. In Applying Implementation Science in Early Childhood Programs and Systems ; Halle, T., Metz, A., Martinez-Beck, I., Eds.; Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2013; pp. 5–19. Available online: https://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=psyc10&NEWS=N&AN=2013-17206-000 (accessed on 25 July 2019).
  • Wandersman, A.; Duffy, J.; Flaspohler, P.; Noonan, R.; Lubell, K.; Stillman, L.; Blachman, M.; Dunville, R.; Saul, J. Bridging the gap between prevention research and practice: The Interactive Systems Framework for dissemination and implementation. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2008 , 41 , 171–181. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Chinman, M.; Hunter, S.B.; Ebener, P.; Paddock, S.M.; Stillman, L.; Imm, P.; Wandersman, A. Illustration of the Prevention Support System. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2008 , 41 , 206–224. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Greenberg, M.T.; Weissberg, R.P.; Brien, M.U.O.; Zins, J.E.; Fredericks, L.; Resnik, H.; Elias, M.J. Enhancing school-based prevention and youth development through coordinated social, emotional, and academic learning. Am. Psychol. 2003 , 58 , 466–474. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Forman, S.G.; Olin, S.S.; Hoagwood, K.E.; Crowe, M.; Saka, N. Evidence-based interventions in schools: Developers’ views of implementation barriers and facilitators. Sch. Ment. Health 2009 , 1 , 26–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Darling-Hammond, L.; Hyler, M.E.; Gardner, M. Effective Teacher Professional Development ; Learning Policy Institute: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 2017. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durlak, J.A.; DuPre, E.P. Implementation matters: A review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. Am. J. Community Psychol. 2008 , 41 , 327–350. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Blank, R.K.; de las Alas, N. Effects of Teacher Professional Development on Gains in Student Achievement ; Council of Chief State School Officers: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wei, R.C.; Darling-Hammond, L.; Andree, A.; Richardson, N.; Orphanos, S. Professional Learning in the Learning Profession: A Status Report on Teacher Development in the United States and Abroad ; National Staff Development Council: Dallas, TX, USA, 2009. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED504168 (accessed on 21 August 2019).
  • Bayar, A. The components of effective professional development activities in terms of teachers’ perspective. Int. Online J. Educ. Sci. 2014 , 6 , 319–327. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Borko, H. Professional development and teacher learning: Mapping the terrain. Educ. Res. 2004 , 33 , 3–15. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Penuel, W.R.; Fishman, B.J.; Yamaguchi, R.; Gallagher, L.P. What makes professional development effective? Strategies that foster curriculum implementation. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2007 , 44 , 921–958. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Katz, J.; Wandersman, A. Technical assistance to enhance prevention capacity: A research synthesis of the evidence base. Prev. Sci. 2016 , 17 , 417–428. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Weiss, C.A. Evaluation , 2nd ed.; Prentice Hall: Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chafouleas, S.M.; Johnson, A.H.; Overstreet, S.; Santos, N.M. Toward a blueprint for trauma-informed service delivery in schools. Sch. Ment. Health 2016 , 8 , 144–162. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Elias, M.J. From Model Implementation to Sustainability: A Multisite Study of Pathways to Excellence in Social-Emotional Learning and Related School Programs. In Sustaining Professional Learning Communities ; Blankstein, A.M., Houston, P.D., Cole, R.W., Eds.; Corwin Press: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 59–95. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eppler-Wolff, N.; Martin, A.; Homayoonfar, S. The School-Based Mental Health Collaboration (SBMHC): A multi-level university-school partnership. J. Infant Child Adolesc. Psychother. 2019 , 18 , 13–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hemmeter, M.L.; Snyder, P.; Fox, L. Using the Teaching Pyramid Observation Tool (TPOT) to support implementation of social–emotional teaching practices. Sch. Ment. Health 2018 , 10 , 202–213. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Becker, K.D.; Darney, D.; Domitrovich, C.; Keperling, J.P.; Ialongo, N.S. Supporting universal prevention programs: A two-phased coaching model. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2013 , 16 , 213–228. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Hough, D.L. Characteristics of effective professional development: An examination of the Developmental Designs character education classroom management approach in middle grades schools. Middle Grades Res. J. 2011 , 6 , 129–143. Available online: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eue&AN=66179257&site=ehost-live (accessed on 11 July 2019).
  • Kutcher, S.; Wei, Y.; McLuckie, A.; Bullock, L. Educator mental health literacy: A programme evaluation of the teacher training education on the mental health & high school curriculum guide. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2013 , 6 , 83–93. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Edgar, S.N. Introducing social emotional learning to music education professional development. Update Appl. Res. Music Educ. 2013 , 31 , 28–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hamre, B.K.; Downer, J.T.; Jamil, F.M.; Pianta, R.C. Enhancing Teachers’ Intentional use of Effective Interactions with Children: Designing and Testing Professional Development Interventions. In Handbook of Early Childhood Education ; Pianta, R.C., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 507–532. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pas, E.T.; Bradshaw, C.P.; Cash, A.H. Coaching Classroom-Based Preventive Interventions. In Handbook of School Mental Health: Research, Training, Practice, and Policy , 2nd ed.; Weist, M.D., Lever, N.A., Bradshaw, C.P., Owens, J.S., Eds.; Springer Science: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 255–267. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Silva, D.Y.; Gimbert, B.G. Character education and teacher inquiry: A promising partnership for enhancing children’s classrooms. Int. J. Soc. Educ. 2001 , 16 , 18–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker, K.D.; Bohnenkamp, J.; Domitrovich, C.; Keperling, J.P.; Ialongo, N.S. Online training for teachers delivering evidence-based preventive interventions. Sch. Ment. Health 2014 , 6 , 225–236. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Cappella, E.; Jackson, D.R.; Bilal, C.; Hamre, B.K.; Soulé, C. Bridging mental health and education in urban elementary schools: Participatory research to inform intervention development. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2011 , 40 , 486–508. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Cunningham, C.E.; Barwick, M.; Short, K.; Chen, Y.; Rimas, H.; Ratcliffe, J.; Mielko, S. Modeling the mental health practice change preferences of educators: A discrete-choice conjoint experiment. Sch. Ment. Health 2014 , 6 , 1–14. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Berkowitz, M.W. What works in values education. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2011 , 50 , 153–158. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vetter, J.B. A Leadership Team Approach to Sustaining Social and Emotional Learning. In Sustaining Professional Learning Communities ; Blankstein, A.M., Houston, P.D., Cole, R.W., Eds.; Corwin Press: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2008; pp. 97–119. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Domitrovich, C.E.; Moore, J.E.; Thompson, R.A.; CASEL Preschool to Elementary School Social and Emotional Learning Assessment Workgroup. Interventions that Promote Social-Emotional Learning in Young Children. In Handbook of Early Childhood Education ; Pianta, R.C., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 393–415. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Erchul, W.P. Put me in, coach: Observations on selected studies implementing supportive interventions to teachers. Sch. Ment. Health 2015 , 7 , 74–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gibson, J.E.; Stephan, S.; Brandt, N.E.; Lever, N.A. Supporting Teachers Through Consultation and Training in Mental Health. In Handbook of School Mental Health: Research, Training, Practice, and Policy , 2nd ed.; Weist, M.D., Lever, N.A., Bradshaw, C.P., Owens, J.S., Eds.; Springer Science: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 269–282. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pas, E.T.; Bradshaw, C.P.; Becker, K.D.; Domitrovich, C.; Berg, J.; Musci, R.; Ialongo, N.S. Identifying patterns of coaching to support the implementation of the good behavior game: The role of teacher characteristics. Sch. Ment. Health 2015 , 7 , 61–73. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Schultz, B.K.; Arora, P.; Mautone, J.A. Consultation and coaching to increase the uptake of evidence-based practices: Introduction to the special issue. Sch. Ment. Health 2015 , 7 , 1–5. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Livet, M.; Yannayon, M.; Sheppard, K.; Kocher, K.; Upright, J.; McMillen, J. Exploring provider use of a digital implementation support system for school mental health: A pilot study. Adm. Policy Ment. Health Ment. Health Serv. Res. 2018 , 45 , 362–380. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Sutherland, K.S.; Conroy, M.A.; Vo, A.; Ladwig, C. Implementation Integrity of Practice-Based Coaching: Preliminary Results from the BEST in CLASS Efficacy Trial. Sch. Ment. Health 2015 , 7 , 21–33. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bradshaw, C.P.; Pas, E.T.; Goldweber, A.; Rosenberg, M.S.; Leaf, P.J. Integrating school-wide positive behavioral interventions and supports with tier 2 coaching to student support teams: The PBISplus model. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2012 , 5 , 177–193. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hamre, B.K.; Pianta, R.C.; Mashburn, A.J.; Downer, J.T. Promoting young children’s social competence through the preschool PATHS curriculum and MyTeachingPartner professional development resources. Early Educ. Dev. 2012 , 23 , 809–832. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wanless, S.B.; Patton, C.L.; Rimm-Kaufman, S.E.; Deutsch, N.L. Setting-level influences on implementation of the responsive classroom approach. Prev. Sci. 2013 , 14 , 40–51. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ashworth, E.; Demkowicz, O.; Lendrum, A.; Frearson, K. Coaching models of school-based prevention and promotion programmes: A qualitative exploration of UK teachers’ perceptions. Sch. Ment. Health 2018 , 10 , 287–300. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Owens, J.S.; Coles, E.K.; Evans, S.W.; Himawan, L.K.; Girio-Herrera, E.; Holdaway, A.S.; Zoromski, A.K.; Schamberg, T.; Schulte, A.C. Using multi-component consultation to increase the integrity with which teachers implement behavioral classroom interventions: A pilot study. Sch. Ment. Health 2017 , 9 , 218–234. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Frey, A.J.; Lee, J.; Small, J.W.; Seeley, J.R.; Walker, H.M.; Feil, E.G. The motivational interviewing navigation guide: A process for enhancing teachers’ motivation to adopt and implement school-based interventions. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2013 , 6 , 158–173. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Steed, E.A.; Durand, V.M. Optimistic teaching: Improving the capacity for teachers to reduce young children’s challenging behavior. Sch. Ment. Health 2013 , 5 , 15–24. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Reinke, W.M.; Herman, K.C.; Darney, D.; Pitchford, J.; Becker, K.; Domitrovich, C.; Ialongo, N. Using the Classroom Check-Up model to support implementation of PATHS to PAX. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2012 , 5 , 220–232. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Scaccia, J.P.; Cook, S.B.; Lamont, A.; Wandersman, A.; Castellow, J.; Katz, J.; Beidas, R.S. A practical implementation science heuristic for organizational readiness: R = MC 2 . J. Community Psychol. 2015 , 43 , 484–501. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]

Click here to enlarge figure

ComponentDescription
TrainingWorkshop to provide introduction to innovation and build needed knowledge, skills, and motivation
Staff inputGather input (e.g., whole staff survey) regarding topics to be covered and alignment of innovation with school mission
InteractiveEmploy active approaches to skill development (e.g., role playing, observation, analysis of models) and include opportunity for discussion and reflection (e.g., connect to current practices)
Technical AssistanceOngoing professional development or guidance to improve the implementation of a program or practice, often coaching
GoalsThe overall purpose of TA as part of teacher professional development for SMH is generally to improve implementation
Developing teacher skillsEnhance and refine skills acquired in training
Building motivationAddress teacher beliefs and attitudes toward the intervention, self-efficacy, commitment, and resistance to increase likelihood of behavior change
Assisting with generalization and adaptationHelp teachers adapt innovation to fit local needs and challenges and to integrate with existing curriculum, routines, schedule, and teaching style
ApproachThe TA providers’ approach underlies effective delivery of TA activities
CollaborativeWork with teachers as partners by building rapport and shared understanding of goals, challenges, and progress
IndividualizedTailor frequency, intensity, and focus of support to address unique needs and challenges of different teachers
Data-driveUse formal and informal data to identify needs, strengths, and values to best prioritize goals and assess implementation quality
Strengths-basedAcknowledge and celebrate strengths, effort, and success; build on strengths before providing feedback on growth areas
ActivitiesThe activities of TA, undertaken using the above approaches, help to achieve TA goals
ModelingDirectly model innovation-specific skills, or provide and analyze video models of these skills
Observation and performance feedbackRepeatedly observe implementation in person or using video recording and provide detailed, targeted feedback
ReflectionIncorporate reflection throughout TA, using collaborative reflection to identify areas of focus for coaching and encourage teacher insight into progress and areas for improvement
Problem solvingIdentify and address barriers through a systematic problem-solving process
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Dinnen, H.L.; Litvitskiy, N.S.; Flaspohler, P.D. Effective Teacher Professional Development for School-Based Mental Health Promotion: A Review of the Literature. Behav. Sci. 2024 , 14 , 780. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090780

Dinnen HL, Litvitskiy NS, Flaspohler PD. Effective Teacher Professional Development for School-Based Mental Health Promotion: A Review of the Literature. Behavioral Sciences . 2024; 14(9):780. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090780

Dinnen, Hannah L., Nicole S. Litvitskiy, and Paul D. Flaspohler. 2024. "Effective Teacher Professional Development for School-Based Mental Health Promotion: A Review of the Literature" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 9: 780. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090780

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Here's how you know

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

Home

  •   Facebook
  •   Twitter
  •   Linkedin
  •   Digg
  •   Reddit
  •   Pinterest
  •   Email

Latest Earthquakes |    Chat Share Social Media  

Self-determination theory as an alternate conceptual foundation for motivation in natural resource research

Motivation is a topic that receives substantial interest across the social sciences. However, in the human dimensions of natural resource literature, scholars have primarily treated motivation as a construct narrowly defined by the individual’s desired goal state. In contrast, self-determination theory (SDT) suggests that multiple forms of motivation can influence levels of self-determination, integration of identities, and subsequent behavioral intentions, and has been utilized in multiple realms to understand human behavior. SDT forms a consistent and well-understood mechanism for human psychological development and optimal function and allows for the formulation of out-of-sample prediction, a cornerstone of science. In this manuscript, we review the basic theories that make up SDT and provide insight for its application to human dimensions of natural resource research.

Citation Information

Publication Year 2024
Title Self-determination theory as an alternate conceptual foundation for motivation in natural resource research
DOI
Authors Kyle Smith, Adam Landon, David C. Fulton, Gerard Kyle
Publication Type Article
Publication Subtype Journal Article
Series Title Human Dimensions of Wildlife: An International Journal
Index ID
Record Source
USGS Organization Coop Res Unit Leetown

Related Content

David fulton, phd, research ecologist.

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Motivation to Use IS: A Literature Review

    literature review motivation theory

  2. Literature Review Motivation Theory

    literature review motivation theory

  3. (PDF) The Role of Motivational Theories in Shaping Teacher Motivation

    literature review motivation theory

  4. ⇉Theory of Motivation and Literature Review of Sales Management Essay

    literature review motivation theory

  5. (PDF) LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES MOTIVATION

    literature review motivation theory

  6. Literature review of ensuring motivation and job commitment Essay

    literature review motivation theory

VIDEO

  1. McClelland's Human Motivation Theory

  2. Literature Review 101

  3. Taylor’s Motivation Theory

  4. Motivation In Conducting Research || Research Methodology || Ph.D 2020 || By Amandeep Lamba

  5. What is Drive Theory of Motivation

  6. Motivation Concepts || Motivational Theories || Organizational Behaviour || Md. Azim

COMMENTS

  1. Theories of motivation: A comprehensive analysis of human behavior

    Theories of motivation: A comprehensive analysis ...

  2. A literature review on motivation

    Research on motivation has attracted academic and corporate entities over the last two decades. In the present study, authors have reviewed the intense literature to extract all possible dimensions of motivation, having direct and indirect impact on motivation techniques. This has examined the multidimensionality of motivation from the existing literature and present a conceptual framework ...

  3. Motivation-Achievement Cycles in Learning: a Literature Review and

    While an exhaustive review of the literature is beyond the scope of the current paper (see the Special Issue on Prominent Motivation Theories: The Past, Present, and Future on Contemporary Educational Psychology, edited by Wigfield and Koenka, 2020), we start with a summary of how influential theories of academic motivation have conceptualized ...

  4. LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES MOTIVATION

    A motive is what pr ompts a person to act in a. certain way or at least dev elop an inclination for. specific behavior (Kast and Rosenzweig, 1985), "Motivation" can be defined as those forces ...

  5. Theories of Motivation in Education: an Integrative Framework

    Theories of Motivation in Education: an Integrative ...

  6. Motivation to learn: an overview of contemporary theories

    Motivation to learn: an overview of contemporary theories

  7. (PDF) Theories of motivation: A comprehensive analysis of human

    This paper explores theories of motivation, including instinct theory, arousal theory, incentive theory, intrinsic. theory, extrinsic theory, the ARCS model, self-determination theory, expectancy ...

  8. (PDF) Contemporary Motivation Learning Theories: A Review

    Abstract: A systematic review was conducted to highlight the current trends in contemporary motivation theory in the. context of education. It compares and contrasts m otivation theories, such as ...

  9. PDF A Review of Classical Motivation Theories: Understanding the Value of

    LITERATURE REVIEW Motivation is a valuable construct in life that determines the degree of success in outcomes (i.e. students in school, professionals at work, potentially the well-being of personal and professional relationships, etc.) and is defined in a number of different ways. Jones and George (2017) defined motivation as

  10. Motivation Theories of Maslow, Herzberg, McGregor & McClelland. A

    Job satisfaction, motivation, and reward systems are included in one area of organizational theory. The strongest influence in this area is motivation because it overlaps into both of the other two components. A review of the classical literature on motivation reveals four major theory areas: (1) Maslow's Hierarchy of Needs; (2) Herzberg's Motivation/Hygiene (two factor) Theory; (3) McGregor's ...

  11. Motivation-achievement cycles in learning: A literature review and

    The question of how learners' motivation influences their academic achievement and vice versa has been the subject of intensive research due to its theoretical relevance and important implications for the field of education. Here, we present our understanding of how influential theories of academic motivation have conceptualized reciprocal interactions between motivation and achievement and ...

  12. Achievement Goal Theory Review: An Application to School Psychology

    Achievement Goal Theory Review: An Application to School ...

  13. [Pdf] a Review of Motivation Theories, Models and Instruments in

    A literature review to identify motivational theories, models, and motivational questionnaires that can support a technology-based learning environment found that there are several theories for measuring motivation, intrinsic theory, and extrinsic motivation theory, self-determination theory, ARCS model, social cognitive theory,and expectancy theory. Motivation is vital for students to face ...

  14. The Future of Work Motivation Theory

    MOTIVATION THEORY The earliest approaches to understanding hu man motivation date from the time of the Greek We are indebted to the staff of AMR and to the editorial philosophers and focus on the concept of hedo review panel for their time and effort on behalf of this spe cial forum. 1 For recent reviews of the research literature on work

  15. On what motivates us: a detailed review of intrinsic

    Behavioral research primarily supports the view that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are partially distinct, interacting processes. For example, if the motivation for intrinsic and extrinsic goals were independent constructs, they might demonstrate an additive or subtractive effect on each other (Woodworth, 1921).

  16. Autonomy and motivation a literature review

    This paper will review the literature on motivation and suggest that there is an important link between autonomy and some educational theories of motivation which could account for the claimed power of autonomy. ... The second theory reviewed--the attribution theory of motivation --relates motivation to the reasons the learner believes are ...

  17. PDF A literature review on motivation

    A literature review on motivation Chandra Sekhar • Manoj Patwardhan • Rohit Kr. Singh Published online: 14 November 2013 International Network of Business and Management 2013 Abstract Research on motivation has attracted academic and corporate entities over the last two decades. In the present study, authors have reviewed the intense

  18. PDF Understanding the Theories and Interventions of Motivation in

    Understanding the Theories and Interventions of Motivation ...

  19. PDF AUTHOR Pardee, Ronald L. TITLE PUB DATE NOTE 24p.

    Motivation Theories of Maslow, Herzberg, McGregor & ...

  20. LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES MOTIVATION

    LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES MOTIVATION. Alex Acquah, Takyi Kwabena Nsiah, +1 author. B. Otoo. Published 21 May 2021. Business, Psychology. People possess something that machines don't - human spirit and inspiration. It is the spirit and inspiration that leads associates to apply their human capital to meet the goals of the organization ...

  21. Literature Review on Theories of Motivation

    LITERATURE REVIEW ON THEORIES OF MOTIVATIONneeds will dete. mine what rewards will satisfy an employee.According to Dessler, most psychologists believe that all motivation is ultimately derived from a tension that results when one or more of our impo. IEW OF THEORIESMaslow's Hierarchy of NeedsThis is the earliest and most widely known theory of ...

  22. Can't or Needn't? Study on the Factors Influencing the Research

    Literature Review Museum Personnel. ... Graham S. (1991). A review of attribution theory in achievement contexts. Educational Psychology Review, 3(1), 5-39. Crossref. ... An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548-573. Crossref. PubMed. Web of Science. Google Scholar.

  23. PDF Motivation-Achievement Cycles in Learning: a Literature Review and

    Our objective is to summarize the current understanding of motivation-achievement inter-actions (drawing mainly from the academic motivation literature) and to identify the theoretical and methodological challenges that could inspire further advances to understand such specific features of this reciprocal relationship.

  24. Motivation and anxiety in Chinese EFL students' listening process: An

    II Literature review 1 Motivation and anxiety in second language acquisition. ... The implications of expectancy-value theory of motivation in language education. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 992372. Crossref. PubMed. Web of Science. Google Scholar. Wu X., Yang H., Liu J., Liu Z. (2022). English use anxiety, motivation, self-efficacy, and their ...

  25. (PDF) A literature review on motivation

    A literature review on motivation. December 2013; Global Business Perspectives 1(4) December 2013; ... theory which has direct influence on employee motivation. The novelty of this study.

  26. Behavioral Sciences

    To address teacher motivation, the descriptive literature reviewed suggested that coaches might specifically target teachers' beliefs and attitudes toward the intervention (e.g., acceptability, alignment with values, appropriateness, expectations of success), self-efficacy, commitment, and resistance [14,53,68,69,71].

  27. Self-determination theory as an alternate conceptual foundation for

    Motivation is a topic that receives substantial interest across the social sciences. However, in the human dimensions of natural resource literature, scholars have primarily treated motivation as a construct narrowly defined by the individual's desired goal state. In contrast, self-determination theory (SDT) suggests that multiple forms of motivation can influence levels of self ...