• DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80067-7
  • Corpus ID: 143654248

Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures

  • Published 1 February 1967
  • Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

1,611 Citations

Perception and memory for pictures: single-trial learning of 2500 visual stimuli, recognition memory for words and pictures at short and long retention intervals, visual recognition memory for complex configurations, recognition memory for common sounds, recognition memory for sentence meaning and wording, changes in picture recognition memory over time using an exclusion set paradigm, coding processes for pictures and words, children’s recognition and recall of picture and word stimuli, remembering trivia, memory span, organization, and transformations in recall of pictures and words, 21 references, short-term memory for complex meaningful visual configurations: a demonstration of capacity., forced-choice tests of recognition memory under steady-state conditions, the magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information., recall as a search process, eidetic imagery: i. frequency, a threshold theory for simple detection experiments., what is memory, on memory and recall., some mechanisms of consciousness discovered during electrical stimulation of the brain., the effect of length of series upon recognition memory., related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

You must turn off your ad blocker to use Psych Web; however, we are taking pains to keep advertising minimal and unobtrusive (one ad at the top of each page) so interference to your reading should be minimal.

If you need instructions for turning off common ad-blocking programs, click here .

If you already know how to turn off your ad blocker, just hit the refresh icon or F5 after you do it, to see the page.

Psi man mascot

Book Table of Contents

Chapter Contents

Recognition Testing

Serial learning and free recall are examples of recall testing . The participant in a memory experiment is exposed to information, waits for a period of time (the retention interval), then tries to remember it.

A different approach is to present a stimulus, wait for a period of time, then present it again to see if the research participant recognizes it. Typically, in recognition testing, the subject sees a series of test items and must indicate whether each item is "old" (from the earlier list) or "new" (not shown earlier).

In recognition testing, What is an "old" item? A "new" item?

Notice this does not correspond to what we usually call recognition. Usually if we do not recognize an item, that means we do not remember ever seeing it before in our lives.

In recognition memory research, a subject is typically asked to recognize an item seen earlier in the experiment . It may be a familiar word the subject has known since childhood.

To experience a simple recognition test in the role of a subject (or "participant" as researchers are more likely to say today) see if you can detect the old items in the following list. Three of the syllables appeared on the previous page.

BOF   WUJ   YOZ   VAF    MIB   DUR   RIQ   SOZ   ZYQ

Could you recognize the items you saw before? Probably so, because (as a rule) recognition memory is more sensitive than recall. Chances are you recognize the three "old" syllables, even if you might not recall them in a free recall test.

(Perhaps you would recall them in a free recall test, if you studied the "mibery" example. Natural language mediation is very helpful.)

Recognition succeeds when recall may fail. Most people can recognize the names of their grade-school teachers, even if they cannot recall them in a free recall situation.

Of all the memory testing procedures, relearning is the most sensitive. It can show the effects of memory when recognition fails. Recognition is next most sensitive; it can show the effects of memory when recall fails. Free recall is the least sensitive or most demanding measure of memory.

How do relearning, recognition, and recall compare in sensitivity, as measures of memory?

Most psychologists agree recall is harder than recognition because retrieval is more of a problem in recall testing. Recognition testing makes retrieval easy by presenting you with a stimulus.

As long as you encode the stimulus the same way during testing as you did during the acquisition (learning) phase, recognition is virtually automatic. This is particularly true when to-be-remembered- items are distinctive (easy to tell apart from other items).

Why is recognition more sensitive than recall?

Recognition failure does occur some­times. People do not always encode stimuli the same way on two different occasions.

Recognition failures take place if your current encoding of a stimulus does not match your previous encoding of the same stimulus. Then you may think you did not see it previously, when actually you did (a false negative).

For example, the nonsense syllable ZYQ might look unfamiliar to you. So you say you did not see it earlier, even though it was on the list you were asked to memorize (a false negative error).

If you think you saw something before, but you did not, that would be the other type of error (a false positive). For example, you might think the syllable BIF looked familiar from the earlier list of nonsense syllables, although the actual stimulus was BOF.

When does recognition failure take place?

A change of context can cause partial recognition failure, where you have a sense of familiarity but cannot quite "place" where you saw something. This sometimes happens when you see a person in an unusual setting.

For example, you might recognize some­body at a sporting event, but be unable to recall where you have seen that person previously. It might turn out the person helped you several times at a retail store. The context is different enough to prevent you from recalling that fact.

Similarly, you might recognize a person's face but be unable to recall the name. That would be a recognition success and a recall failure.

Even when the criterion of success is clear and simple, like deciding whether you have seen a face before, recog­nition memory can be probabilistic in nature. You might be "70% sure" you recognize a face.

This would be like saying, "When I feel this level of confidence in my recog­nition, I might be wrong 3 out of 10 times."

How is recognition "probabilistic" in nature?

Subjective judgments do not always correspond with reality, of course. You could be 90% sure a stimulus was familiar when viewing it for the first time. That would be an example of a false positive recognition error.

A person who claims to be guessing, in a recognition task, usually performs at better than random levels of accuracy. This is a typical finding.

Perhaps some sort of unconscious, implicit learning is at work, when a person "knows more than they know." The person has begun to grasp a pattern unconsciously, but the results of this computational activity are not yet available to consciousness.

Because people usually guess at better than random levels in a recognition test, students taking multiple-choice tests should guess rather than leave items blank. The only time this is might be a bad strategy is when students are penalized more heavily for wrong answers than for leaving items blank.

Why does a student who guesses an answer have a good chance of getting it right?

The probabilistic nature of recognition puts it in the same category as other processes in which a person picks a signal out of background noise . The Theory of Signal Detection was designed for situations like that.

The Theory of Signal Detection was discussed in chapter 4. It is a mathematical description of decision-making in uncertain environments.

That framework works well with recognition testing. The d-prime statistic can provide a bias-free measure of the true sensitivity of an organism's recognition abilities.

Forced Choice Recognition Testing

The simplest way to test recognition is to present a stimulus and ask, "Do you recognize this?" Unfortun­ately, this yes/no method does not take into account the probabilistic nature of recognition.

On a true/false quiz, some people are very conservative and will say False if they are not absolutely sure of their recognition judgment. They want to avoid false positives, and that is their bias.

What is the drawback of the yes/no method?

Others have a bias toward saying True. Perhaps they want to be agreeable. This can vary between ethnic groups.

One pollster found that people of Japanese ancestry had a bias toward saying Yes in opinion polls, perhaps because it was con­sidered more polite. This threw off the accuracy of results.

In general, people differ in their bias toward saying Yes or No on a recognition test. One person might require 70% confidence before saying Yes (or True), another might only say Yes when 90% sure.

Experimenters cannot compare the recognition accuracy of different people unless they know the confidence level each requires for a Yes judgment, and that is something that could change even within an experiment.

One solution is to use the Theory of Signal Detection. A person could be presented with a range of biasing conditions so the D prime statistic could be calculated.

In the forced-choice method, what is the target? The distracter?

However, a simpler solution is to use a forced-choice method of recognition testing. In a forced-choice procedure, subjects are faced with two or more stimuli, only one of which is "old."

The old item is called the target item; the other items are called distracters . To give yourself a forced choice recognition test, decide which of the following items was used earlier in this chapter.

VEW   DOX   XAJ

What are advantages of the forced-choice method?

In a forced choice test, you must guess even if you are not sure. This solves the problem of response bias, because every­body must say Yes to one and only one item.

This also makes forced-choice testing a very sensitive measure of recognition memory, because people are forced to make a guess even if they are not sure about the answer. Usually they will guess at a better-than-chance level.

The "Almost Limitless" Capacity of Recognition Memory

How sensitive is human recognition memory? Standing (1973) tried to find its upper limits. He gave people a single exposure to pictures cut from magazines then tested them in a forced-choice situation using similar pictures as distracters.

Standing found that subjects had no difficulty learning 10,000 pictures in one sitting. They could recognize them the next day and tell them apart from the distracters. Animals like chimps and pigeons proved to have equally impressive recognition memory for images.

He concluded, "The capacity of recognition memory for pictures is almost limitless." In similar research, Shepard (1967) found that word recognition was over 90% accurate in forced choice situations.

What did Standing conclude, and how did he arrive at this conclusion?

The excellence of recognition memory creates a potential problem for teach­ers. A poorly constructed multiple-choice test resembles a forced-choice recognition test.

Suppose a teacher puts familiar vocabulary words from lectures into correct answers on a multiple choice test, but not in any of the distractors. According to Shepard's research above, a student who simply recognizes the words (without understanding the concepts) could pick out the right answer nine out of ten times.

Students are efficiency experts. If they learn they can ace tests simply by familiarizing themselves with words, some will do that instead of studying.

Therefore a teacher who con­structs tests carelessly, making it easy to pass a test using recognition memory, subverts the goal of encouraging in-depth understanding. No wonder "multiple guess" tests have such a bad reputation among teachers with high standards.

How can a multiple-choice test be constructed to measure more than recognition?

However, it is not too difficult to create a multiple-choice test that measures comprehension rather than familiarity with words. The test writer must construct distracters that trigger a recognition response. More than one answer must "sound right" to a student who has merely skimmed and does not understand the concepts.

That is easy to accomplish by using familiar words from the text in the wrong answers. Of course, the instructor must take care that only one answer is actually correct, and the correct answer should be easy to select for a student who understands the concept targeted by the question.

Also desirable: the correct response might not repeat the exact language of the lecture or book. It might use paraphrasing (expressing the same idea in different words).

The goal is to encourage a correct understanding of the subject matter. Recognition memory alone should not be enough to guide a student to a correct answer.

Recognition Errors

In an article titled "Attributes of Memory" (1969) the influential verbal learning researcher Benton Underwood summarized evidence about false recognition errors. He pointed out that they occur when words have similar attributes .

Even opposites can be falsely mistaken for each other. If you see the word UP in a list of words, you might falsely recog­nize the word DOWN on a recognition test the next day. Both are directions, and both involve the vertical dimension, so they have many features in common, despite being opposites.

Underwood tested other relationships that might provoke false recognition errors. He obtained the results shown in the following table.

RelationshipList word(s)Test wordError rate
Oppositesbottomtop.32
Associatesbread, buttercrumbs  .28
Categories/ maple, elm, oaktree.25
Sense Impressionsdoughnut, globeround.08 (not signif.)

It shows, for example, that people who saw the word "bottom" on a list would falsely recognize the word "top" about 32% of the time. Conceptual features (related to meaning) proved more likely to produce recognition errors than per­ceptual features (related to appear­ance).

What sorts of relationships did Underwood discover to provoke false recognition?

Underwood summarized his conclusions as follows:

A memory is...a collection of at­tributes that serve to discrim­inate one memory from another and to act as retrieval mech­anisms for a target memory.... Encoding represents the process by which the attributes of a memory are established.

Why was Underwood's conversion to the encoding perspective considered important?

When Underwood wrote that in 1969, other researchers took note. Under­wood was a grand old man of memory research, highly respected. He had always accepted the assumptions of S-R theory that went back to the days of Ebbinghaus.

Underwood was joining the encoding revolution. The old S-R (stimulus-re­sponse) theories were being replaced. Now researchers had to consider how subjects interpreted, represented, and manipulated information.

How did changes in memory research foreshadow changes in other areas of experimental psychology?

Memory researchers were among the first psychologists to use information processing concepts. Before long, the encoding revolution in memory research was followed by the cognitive revolution in experi­mental psychology as a whole.

--------------------- References:

Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6 , 156-163.

Standing, L (1973). Learning 10,000 pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25 , 207-222.

Underwood, B. J. (1969). Attributes of memory. Psychological Review, 76 , 559-573.

Prev page    |    Page top    |    Chapter Contents    |    Next page

Write to Dr. Dewey at [email protected].

Don't see what you need? Psych Web has over 1,000 pages, so it may be elsewhere on the site. Do a site-specific Google search using the box below.

Copyright © 2007-2018 Russ Dewey

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Recognition memory for words and pictures at short and long retention intervals

Profile image of Michael Toglia

1976, Memory & Cognition

Related Papers

Memory & Cognition

When the context accompanying a to-be-remembered word is changed between study and test, recognition memory is impaired. The deleterious effect of context change on recognition memory can be viewed as support for encoding specificity theory, semantic theory, or the existence of two bases for recognition. A fourth possible interpretation, examined here, is that the effect of context change on recognition memory is due to an accompanying change in response bias, rather than a “true” decrease in sensitivity to old and new items. In two experiments, the effect of context change on discrimination and bias in recognition of simple line drawings and their names was examined. Bias was measured using two measures shown by Snodgrass and Corwin (1988) to be theoretically independent of their associated discrimination measures. Context change produced marked conservatism in response bias in both experiments but demonstrated an effect on discrimination in the second experiment only. The shift from a neutral to a conservative response strategy as a result of context change may also be seen in other experiments, in which the same experimental paradigm was used with a variety of stimulus materials- We suggest that the major effect of context manipulation is to produce a change in-bias.A stimulus, in a.familiar environment appears to be more familiar than a stimulus in a strange environment, regardless of its old/new status. In addition, there appears to be a true decrease in discrimination with context change, but this is more difficult to detect. The finding that pictures, which are less polysemous than words, are as affected by context change as words are supports encoding-Specificity theory over semantic theory.

shepard 1967 experiment

Angela Boldini

John D Bain

Journal of Memory and Language

Douglas Hintzman

Charles Clifton

Gordon Bower

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

Barbara Tversky

Journal of Experimental Psychology-human Perception and Performance

Janina Rado

Memory & Cognition

Lynne Reder

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Itamar Gati

Cognitive Psychology

James Juola

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

Donald Thomson

Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

Thomas Toppino

James L. (Jay) McClelland

Journal of Experimental Psychology

Aimee Surprenant

Jean Pierce

Psychonomic Science

Frank Wicker

Robert M. Nosofsky

Psychological Review

Mark Chappell

Graham Hitch

Natalie Frankel

Perception & Psychophysics

Object Recognition

Valeria Carlini

Greg Schwartz

James Nairne

Anne Cleary

Neuropsychology

Christine Bastin , Martial Van der Linden

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

Larry Jacoby

Cathy McEvoy

Hannah Scruggs

nelson ukere

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

IMAGES

  1. Accessibilities from Shepard (1967) and Nickerson (1968) compared with

    shepard 1967 experiment

  2. Comparison between the present experiment, Shepard (1967) and Nickerson

    shepard 1967 experiment

  3. Comparison between the present experiment, Shepard (1967) and Nickerson

    shepard 1967 experiment

  4. Accessibilities from Shepard (1967) and Nickerson (1968) compared with

    shepard 1967 experiment

  5. APOLLO 14: NASA "RED NUMBER" PHOTOGRAPH SHEPARD AND MITCHELL EXPERIMENT

    shepard 1967 experiment

  6. Solved QUESTION 37 Shepard (1967) found that memory for

    shepard 1967 experiment

COMMENTS

  1. PDF Memory for pictures: Sometimes a picure is not worth a single word

    Smythe, 1968; Shepard, 1967). In addition, the number of pictures that can be ... We conducted an experiment whose goal was not to test the PSE, but the results frequently elicited comments from memory experts who were surprised by the superior memory for words than for photographs and pictures. Reder et al. (2006) conducted a recognition ...

  2. Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures

    In order to secure a more precise estimate, however, it is necessary to take account of the de- parture from 100% to be expected in a yes- no experiment of a design more suitable for the direct application of information theory. 162 SHEPARD Now, since recognition performance is gener- ally better for short than for long inspection se- quences ...

  3. Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures.

    ss looked through a series of about 600 stimuli selected at random from an initially larger population. they were then tested for their ability to recognize these "old" stimuli in pairs in which the alternative was always a "new" stimulus selected at random from the stimuli remaining in the original population. depending upon whether this original population consisted solely of words ...

  4. PDF The fidelity of visual and auditory memory

    Using this paradigm, Shepard (1967) presented over 600 pictures during the study phase and found that participants were 98% accurate on the memory test. In a similar study, Standing, Conezio, and Haber ( 1970)presented ... Experiment 1 was conducted to ensure that perceptual dis-criminability between old items and exemplars were similar.

  5. PDF Learning 10,000 Pictures

    Shepard (1967), extending a similar study by Nickerson (1965), has found that immediately following a single exposure of ... Experiment I It is desirable first to examine picture memory capacity in more detail than has been achieved by Nickerson, Shepard or Standing et al. in order to find the

  6. PDF Memory Cognition Vol. 256-260 Recognition memoryfor wordsand ...

    Deno, 1967; Shepard, 1967). One purpose of the present studies was to obtain information concerning' pictorial andverbal recognition memory over a longer period oftime. To this end, picture and word recog­ nition were compared for intervals ranging from 10 min to 3 months in Experiment I and from 15 min to 2 months in Experiment II.

  7. PDF Recognition memory for words, pictures, and words pictures

    tasks, pictorial imagery variables play an important role. For example, words rated higher in imagery are remembered better than those rated low in imagery (Paivio, 1969), and recognition memory. or pictures is better than that for words (Shepard, 1967).One would also expect that presenting two cues in a recognition memory paradigm, such as the ...

  8. Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures

    Recognition memory for common sounds. James D. Miller D. Tanis. Psychology. 1971. Recognition memory was measured separately for printed words, spoken words, and common sounds. Ss in each group inspected 350 stimuli and then were tested with 100 pairs of "old" and "new" stimuli in…. Expand.

  9. Verbal and pictorial similarity in recognition of pictures

    This experiment explored the role of pictorial and verbal coding in memory for pictures. Ss viewed slides for either 3 or 10 sec each. ... Shepard, 1967; Standing, Conezio, & Haber, 1970). Shepard ...

  10. Long-term memory for a common object

    EXPERIMENT II The results of Experiment I indicate that the information that appears on a U.S. penny is not trivially easy for many of us to recall, or at least to draw on request. ... 1968), Shepard (1967), and Standing et al. (1970). The pictures that were used in those studies were complex and diverse. On the average, they probably differed ...

  11. PDF Does visual expertise improve visual recognition memory?

    Standing, 1973). Shepard (1967) showed observers 612 different pictures, and obtained 98% correct recognition performance. Standing (1973) went even further, presenting ... Fig. 1 Examples of stimuli used in different blocks of the experiment. a Objects, b scenes, c Pap smears, d mammograms. Cytologists and their

  12. PDF Recognition Memory for Briefly Presented Pictures: The Time Course of

    Shepard, 1967; Standing, 1973). But when pictured scenes are presented more rapidly, in a sequence that simulates successive fixations with durations of 125-333 ms, most of the pictures ... Experiment 3. In Experiment 4, 10 rather than 5 pictures were presented, and, in Experiment 5, 20 pictures were presented. ...

  13. Recognition Testing

    The participant in a memory experiment is exposed to information, waits for a period of time (the retention interval), then tries to remember it. ... Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 156-163.

  14. The Picture Superiority Effect in Conceptual Implicit Memory: A ...

    1968) and recognition (Madigan, 1983; Shepard, 1967). These tests are considered tests of explicit memory because they require participants to explicitly refer back to the study episode and indicate whether a target ... (see Experiment 1 of Nicolas, 1995). However, as Weldon and Coyote suggested, performance on this test might have been influenced.

  15. Recognition memory for sentence meaning and wording

    Eight served in the memory experiment, and eight rated the sentences on imagery. 178 BEGG Materials Two parallel lists of 502 sentences, A and B, were constructed. ... judgments was similar to the results of Sachs (1967a,b), while the meaning judgments approximated the results of Shepard (1967). Thus, while Ss have a remarkable capacity for ...

  16. Accessibilities from Shepard (1967) and Nickerson (1968

    Accessibilities from Shepard (1967) and Nickerson (1968) compared with the present experiment. The functions fit to these data are Equation 4 with f(t) = 1/t, which adjusts for interference from ...

  17. (PDF) Recognition memory for words and pictures at short and long

    Retention intervals varied from several minutes to a few months. The complicated testing procedures in Experiment I required the use of a traditional correction for guessing to obtain estimates of subjects' memory performance. A comparable, but simpler, design in Experiment II permitted the calculation of sensitivity and bias measures.

  18. Comparison between the present experiment, Shepard (1967

    Download scientific diagram | Comparison between the present experiment, Shepard (1967) and Nickerson (1965 & 1968). from publication: FORGETTING TRACKED BY RECOGNITION OF PICTURES | Thirty-three ...

  19. PDF Increasing word distinctiveness eliminates the picture ...

    example, Shepard (1967) tested memory for 612 color pic-tures and 540 words. At test, he showed two pictures (or words) simultaneously, one of which had been in the study phase and one of which was new. Whereas subjects correctly identified the old words 88.4% of the time, they correctly identified the old pictures 96.7% of the time. Snodgrass,

  20. Picture superiority effect

    Allan Paivio's dual-coding theory is a basis of picture superiority effect. Paivio claims that pictures have advantages over words with regards to coding and retrieval of stored memory because pictures are coded more easily and can be retrieved from symbolic mode, while the dual coding process using words is more difficult for both coding and retrieval.

  21. Memory & Cognition Exam 2 Flashcards

    Memory & Cognition Exam 2. In a pioneering study, Shepard (1967, Experiment 1) had participants study a deck of 540 index cards, with one randomly selected word printed on each; half the words were high frequency (e.g. office), and the other half were very low frequency (e.g. , ferule). Participants studied the words, and were given a forced ...

  22. Chapter 7: Representation of Knowledge Flashcards

    Shepard (1967) Experiment -Compared memory for sentences vs. memory for pictures -Some participants were shown a photo, then after the photo was taken away they were asked to choose which photo from a series of photos was the original one.

  23. Nth Country Experiment

    The experiment ended on April 10, 1967, after three person-years of work over two and a half calendar years. According to a heavily redacted declassified version of the summary, lab weapons experts apparently judged that the team had come up with a credible design for an implosion-style nuclear weapon.

  24. 55 Years Ago: Apollo 11 Astronauts End Quarantine, Feted from ...

    The main goals of this second lunar landing included a precision touchdown near the Surveyor 3 spacecraft that landed there in April 1967, and an expanded science program conducted during two spacewalks, including the deployment of the first Apollo Lunar Surface Experiment Package (ALSEP), a suite of science instruments.