Classical Conditioning: How It Works With Examples

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Classical conditioning (also known as Pavlovian or respondent conditioning) is learning through association and was discovered by Pavlov , a Russian physiologist. In simple terms, two stimuli are linked together to produce a new learned response in a person or animal.

John B. Watson proposed that the process of classical conditioning (based on Pavlov’s observations) was able to explain all aspects of human psychology.

If you pair a neutral stimulus (NS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US) that already triggers an unconditioned response (UR) that neutral stimulus will become a conditioned stimulus (CS), triggering a conditioned response (CR) similar to the original unconditioned response.

Everything from speech to emotional responses was simply patterns of stimulus and response. Watson completely denied the existence of the mind or consciousness. Watson believed that all individual differences in behavior were due to different learning experiences.

Watson (1924, p. 104) famously said:

Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I’ll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select – doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations and the race of his ancestors.

How Classical Conditioning Works

There are three stages of classical conditioning. At each stage, the stimuli and responses are given special scientific terms:

Stage 1: Before Conditioning:

In this stage, the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) produces an unconditioned response (UCR) in an organism.

In basic terms, this means that a stimulus in the environment has produced a behavior/response that is unlearned (i.e., unconditioned) and, therefore, is a natural response that has not been taught. In this respect, no new behavior has been learned yet.

For example, a stomach virus (UCS) would produce a response of nausea (UCR). In another example, a perfume (UCS) could create a response of happiness or desire (UCR).

This stage also involves another stimulus that has no effect on a person and is called the neutral stimulus (NS). The NS could be a person, object, place, etc.

The neutral stimulus in classical conditioning does not produce a response until it is paired with the unconditioned stimulus.

Stage 2: During Conditioning:

During this stage, a stimulus which produces no response (i.e., neutral) is associated with the unconditioned stimulus, at which point it now becomes known as the conditioned stimulus (CS).

For example, a stomach virus (UCS) might be associated with eating a certain food such as chocolate (CS). Also, perfume (UCS) might be associated with a specific person (CS).

For classical conditioning to be effective, the conditioned stimulus should occur before the unconditioned stimulus, rather than after it, or during the same time. Thus, the conditioned stimulus acts as a type of signal or cue for the unconditioned stimulus.

In some cases, conditioning may take place if the NS occurs after the UCS (backward conditioning), but this normally disappears quite quickly. The most important aspect of the conditioning stimulus is the it helps the organism predict the coming of the unconditional stimulus.

Often during this stage, the UCS must be associated with the CS on a number of occasions, or trials, for learning to take place.

However, one trial learning can happen on certain occasions when it is not necessary for an association to be strengthened over time (such as being sick after food poisoning or drinking too much alcohol).

Stage 3: After Conditioning:

The conditioned stimulus (CS) has been associated with the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) to create a new conditioned response (CR).

For example, a person (CS) who has been associated with nice perfume (UCS) is now found attractive (CR). Also, chocolate (CS) which was eaten before a person was sick with a virus (UCS) now produces a response of nausea (CR).

Classical Conditioning Examples

Pavlov’s dogs.

The most famous example of classical conditioning was Ivan Pavlov’s experiment with dogs , who salivated in response to a bell tone. Pavlov showed that when a bell was sounded each time the dog was fed, the dog learned to associate the sound with the presentation of the food.

Pavlovs Dogs Experiment

He first presented the dogs with the sound of a bell; they did not salivate so this was a neutral stimulus. Then he presented them with food, they salivated. The food was an unconditioned stimulus, and salivation was an unconditioned (innate) response.

He then repeatedly presented the dogs with the sound of the bell first and then the food (pairing) after a few repetitions, the dogs salivated when they heard the sound of the bell. The bell had become the conditioned stimulus and salivation had become the conditioned response.

Fear Response

Watson & Rayner (1920) were the first psychologists to apply the principles of classical conditioning to human behavior by looking at how this learning process may explain the development of phobias.

They did this in what is now considered to be one of the most ethically dubious experiments ever conducted – the case of Little Albert . Albert B.’s mother was a wet nurse in a children’s hospital. Albert was described as ‘healthy from birth’ and ‘on the whole stolid and unemotional’.

When he was about nine months old, his reactions to various stimuli (including a white rat, burning newspapers, and a hammer striking a four-foot steel bar just behind his head) were tested.

Little Albert Classical Conditioning

Only the last of these frightened him, so this was designated the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and fear the unconditioned response (UCR). The other stimuli were neutral because they did not produce fear.

When Albert was just over eleven months old, the rat and the UCS were presented together: as Albert reached out to stroke the animal, Watson struck the bar behind his head.

This occurred seven times in total over the next seven weeks. By this time, the rat, the conditioned stimulus (CS), on its own frightened Albert, and fear was now a conditioned response (CR).

The CR transferred spontaneously to the rabbit, the dog, and other stimuli that had been previously neutral. Five days after conditioning, the CR produced by the rat persisted. After ten days, it was ‘much less marked’, but it was still evident a month later.

Carter and Tiffany (1999) support the cue reactivity theory, they carried out a meta-analysis reviewing 41 cue-reactivity studies that compared responses of alcoholics, cigarette smokers, cocaine addicts and heroin addicts to drug-related versus neutral stimuli.

They found that dependent individuals reacted strongly to the cues presented and reported craving and physiological arousal.

Panic Disorder

Classical conditioning is thought to play an important role in the development of Pavlov (Bouton et al., 2002).

Panic disorder often begins after an initial “conditioning episode” involving an early panic attack. The panic attack serves as an unconditioned stimulus (US) that gets paired with neutral stimuli (conditioned stimuli or CS), allowing those stimuli to later trigger anxiety and panic reactions (conditioned responses or CRs).

The panic attack US can become associated with interoceptive cues (like increased heart rate) as well as external situational cues that are present during the attack. This allows those cues to later elicit anxiety and possibly panic (CRs).

Through this conditioning process, anxiety becomes focused on the possibility of having another panic attack. This anticipatory anxiety (a CR) is seen as a key step in the development of panic disorder, as it leads to heightened vigilance and sensitivity to bodily cues that can trigger future attacks.

The presence of conditioned anxiety can serve to potentiate or exacerbate future panic attacks. Anxiety cues essentially lower the threshold for panic. This helps explain how panic disorder can spiral after the initial conditioning episode.

Evidence suggests most patients with panic disorder recall an initial panic attack or conditioning event that preceded the disorder. Prospective studies also show conditioned anxiety and panic reactions can develop after an initial panic episode.

Classical conditioning processes are believed to often occur outside of conscious awareness in panic disorder, reflecting the operation of emotional neural systems separate from declarative knowledge systems.

Cue reactivity is the theory that people associate situations (e.g., meeting with friends)/ places (e.g., pub) with the rewarding effects of nicotine, and these cues can trigger a feeling of craving (Carter & Tiffany, 1999).

These factors become smoking-related cues. Prolonged use of nicotine creates an association between these factors and smoking based on classical conditioning.

Nicotine is the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), and the pleasure caused by the sudden increase in dopamine levels is the unconditioned response (UCR). Following this increase, the brain tries to lower the dopamine back to a normal level.

The stimuli that have become associated with nicotine were neutral stimuli (NS) before “learning” took place but they became conditioned stimuli (CS), with repeated pairings. They can produce the conditioned response (CR).

However, if the brain has not received nicotine, the levels of dopamine drop, and the individual experiences withdrawal symptoms therefore is more likely to feel the need to smoke in the presence of the cues that have become associated with the use of nicotine.

Classroom Learning

The implications of classical conditioning in the classroom are less important than those of operant conditioning , but there is still a need for teachers to try to make sure that students associate positive emotional experiences with learning.

If a student associates negative emotional experiences with school, then this can obviously have bad results, such as creating a school phobia.

For example, if a student is bullied at school they may learn to associate the school with fear. It could also explain why some students show a particular dislike of certain subjects that continue throughout their academic career. This could happen if a student is humiliated or punished in class by a teacher.

Principles of Classical Conditioning

Neutral stimulus.

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus (NS) is a stimulus that initially does not evoke a response until it is paired with the unconditioned stimulus.

For example, in Pavlov’s experiment, the bell was the neutral stimulus, and only produced a response when paired with food.

Unconditioned Stimulus

Unconditioned response.

In classical conditioning, an unconditioned response is an innate response that occurs automatically when the unconditioned stimulus is presented.

Pavlov showed the existence of the unconditioned response by presenting a dog with a bowl of food and measuring its salivary secretions.

Conditioned Stimulus

Conditioned response.

In classical conditioning, the conditioned response (CR) is the learned response to the previously neutral stimulus.

In Ivan Pavlov’s experiments in classical conditioning, the dog’s salivation was the conditioned response to the sound of a bell.

Acquisition

The process of pairing a neutral stimulus with an unconditioned stimulus to produce a conditioned response.

In the initial learning period, acquisition describes when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus.

In psychology, extinction refers to the gradual weakening of a conditioned response by breaking the association between the conditioned and the unconditioned stimuli.

The weakening of a conditioned response occurs when the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus.

For example, when the bell repeatedly rang, and no food was presented, Pavlov’s dog gradually stopped salivating at the sound of the bell.

Spontaneous Recovery

Spontaneous recovery is a phenomenon of Pavlovian conditioning that refers to the return of a conditioned response (in a weaker form) after a period of time following extinction.

It is the reappearance of an extinguished conditioned response after a rest period when the conditioned stimulus is presented alone.

For example, when Pavlov waited a few days after extinguishing the conditioned response, and then rang the bell once more, the dog salivated again.

Generalization

In psychology, generalization is the tendency to respond in the same way to stimuli similar (but not identical) to the original conditioned stimulus.

For example, in Pavlov’s experiment, if a dog is conditioned to salivate to the sound of a bell, it may later salivate to a higher-pitched bell.

Discrimination

In classical conditioning, discrimination is a process through which individuals learn to differentiate among similar stimuli and respond appropriately to each one.

For example, eventually, Pavlov’s dog learns the difference between the sound of the 2 bells and no longer salivates at the sound of the non-food bell.

Higher-Order Conditioning

Higher-order conditioning is when a conditioned stimulus is paired with a new neutral stimulus to create a second conditioned stimulus. For example, a bell (CS1) is paired with food (UCS) so that the bell elicits salivation (CR). Then, a light (NS) is paired with the bell.

Eventually, the light alone will elicit salivation, even without the presence of food. This demonstrates higher-order conditioning, where the conditioned stimulus (bell) serves as an unconditioned stimulus to condition a new stimulus (light).

Critical Evaluation

Practical applications.

The principles of classical conditioning have been widely and effectively applied in fields like behavioral therapy, education, and advertising. Therapies like systematic desensitization use classical conditioning to help eliminate phobias and anxiety.

The behaviorist approach has been used in the treatment of phobias, and systematic desensitization . The individual with the phobia is taught relaxation techniques and then makes a hierarchy of fear from the least frightening to the most frightening features of the phobic object.

He then is presented with the stimuli in that order and learns to associate (classical conditioning) the stimuli with a relaxation response. This is counter-conditioning.

Explaining involuntary behaviors

Classical conditioning helps explain some reflexive or involuntary behaviors like phobias, emotional reactions, and physiological responses. The model shows how these can be acquired through experience.

The process of classical conditioning can probably account for aspects of certain other mental disorders. For example, in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), sufferers tend to show classically conditioned responses to stimuli present at the time of the traumatizing event (Charney et al., 1993).

However, since not everyone exposed to the traumatic event develops PTSD, other factors must be involved, such as individual differences in people’s appraisal of events as stressors and the recovery environment, such as family and support groups.

Supported by substantial experimental evidence

There is a wealth of experimental support for basic phenomena like acquisition, extinction, generalization, and discrimination. Pavlov’s original experiments on dogs and subsequent studies have demonstrated classical conditioning in animals and humans.

There have been many laboratory demonstrations of human participants acquiring behavior through classical conditioning. It is relatively easy to classically condition and extinguish conditioned responses, such as the eye-blink and galvanic skin responses.

A strength of classical conditioning theory is that it is scientific . This is because it’s based on empirical evidence carried out by controlled experiments . For example, Pavlov (1902) showed how classical conditioning could be used to make a dog salivate to the sound of a bell.

Supporters of a reductionist approach say that it is scientific. Breaking complicated behaviors down into small parts means that they can be scientifically tested. However, some would argue that the reductionist view lacks validity . Thus, while reductionism is useful, it can lead to incomplete explanations.

Ignores biological predispositions

Organisms are biologically prepared to associate certain stimuli over others. However, classical conditioning does not sufficiently account for innate predispositions and biases.

Classical conditioning emphasizes the importance of learning from the environment, and supports nurture over nature.

However, it is limiting to describe behavior solely in terms of either nature or nurture , and attempts to do this underestimate the complexity of human behavior. It is more likely that behavior is due to an interaction between nature (biology) and nurture (environment).

Lacks explanatory power

Classical conditioning provides limited insight into the cognitive processes underlying the associations it describes.

However, applying classical conditioning to our understanding of higher mental functions, such as memory, thinking, reasoning, or problem-solving, has proved more problematic.

Even behavior therapy, one of the more successful applications of conditioning principles to human behavior, has given way to cognitive–behavior therapy (Mackintosh, 1995).

Questionable ecological validity

While lab studies support classical conditioning, some question how well it holds up in natural settings. There is debate about how automatic and inevitable classical conditioning is outside the lab.

In normal adults, the conditioning process can be overridden by instructions: simply telling participants that the unconditioned stimulus will not occur causes an instant loss of the conditioned response, which would otherwise extinguish only slowly (Davey, 1983).

Most participants in an experiment are aware of the experimenter’s contingencies (the relationship between stimuli and responses) and, in the absence of such awareness often fail to show evidence of conditioning (Brewer, 1974).

Evidence indicates that for humans to exhibit classical conditioning, they need to be consciously aware of the connection between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). This contradicts traditional theories that humans have two separate learning systems – one conscious and one unconscious – that allow conditioning to occur without conscious awareness (Lovibond & Shanks, 2002).

There are also important differences between very young children or those with severe learning difficulties and older children and adults regarding their behavior in a variety of operant conditioning and discrimination learning experiments.

These seem largely attributable to language development (Dugdale & Lowe, 1990). This suggests that people have rather more efficient, language-based forms of learning at their disposal than just the laborious formation of associations between a conditioned stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus.

Ethical concerns

The principles of classical conditioning raise ethical concerns about manipulating behavior without consent. This is especially true in advertising and politics.

  • Manipulation of preferences – Classical conditioning can create positive associations with certain brands, products, or political candidates. This can manipulate preferences outside of a person’s rational thought process.
  • Encouraging impulsive behaviors – Conditioning techniques may encourage behaviors like impulsive shopping, unhealthy eating, or risky financial choices by forging positive associations with these behaviors.
  • Preying on vulnerabilities – Advertisers or political campaigns may exploit conditioning techniques to target and influence vulnerable demographic groups like youth, seniors, or those with mental health conditions.
  • Reduction of human agency – At an extreme, the use of classical conditioning techniques reduces human beings to automata reacting predictably to stimuli. This is ethically problematic.

Deterministic theory

A final criticism of classical conditioning theory is that it is deterministic . This means it does not allow the individual any degree of free will. Accordingly, a person has no control over the reactions they have learned from classical conditioning, such as a phobia.

The deterministic approach also has important implications for psychology as a science. Scientists are interested in discovering laws that can be used to predict events.

However, by creating general laws of behavior, deterministic psychology underestimates the uniqueness of human beings and their freedom to choose their destiny.

The Role of Nature in Classical Conditioning

Behaviorists argue all learning is driven by experience, not nature. Classical conditioning exemplifies environmental influence. However, our evolutionary history predisposes us to learn some associations more readily than others. So nature also plays a role.

For example, PTSD develops in part due to strong conditioning during traumatic events. The emotions experienced during trauma lead to neural activity in the amygdala , creating strong associative learning between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli (Milad et al., 2009).

Individuals with PTSD show enhanced fear conditioning, reflected in greater amygdala reactivity to conditioned threat cues compared to trauma-exposed controls. In addition to strong initial conditioning, PTSD patients exhibit slower extinction to conditioned fear stimuli.

During extinction recall tests, PTSD patients fail to show differential skin conductance responses to extinguished versus non-extinguished cues, indicating impaired retention of fear extinction. Deficient extinction retention corresponds to reduced activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus and heightened dorsal anterior cingulate cortex response during extinction recall in PTSD patients.

In influential research on food conditioning, John Garcia found that rats easily learned to associate a taste with nausea from drugs, even if illness occurred hours later.

However, conditioning nausea to a sight or sound was much harder. This showed that conditioning does not occur equally for any stimulus pairing. Rather, evolution prepares organisms to learn some associations that aid survival more easily, like linking smells to illness.

The evolutionary significance of taste and nutrition ensures robust and resilient classical conditioning of flavor preferences, making them difficult to reverse (Hall, 2002).

Forming strong and lasting associations between flavors and nutrition aids survival by promoting the consumption of calorie-rich foods. This makes flavor conditioning very robust.

Repeated flavor-nutrition pairings in these studies lead to overlearning of the association, making it more resistant to extinction.

The learning is overtrained, context-specific, and subject to recovery effects that maintain the conditioned behavior despite extinction training.

Classical vs. operant condioning

In summary, classical conditioning is about passive stimulus-response associations, while operant conditioning is about actively connecting behaviors to consequences. Classical works on reflexes and operant on voluntary actions.

  • Stimuli vs consequences : Classical conditioning focuses on associating two stimuli together. For example, pairing a bell (neutral stimulus) with food (reflex-eliciting stimulus) creates a conditioned response of salivation to the bell. Operant conditioning is about connecting behaviors with the consequences that follow. If a behavior is reinforced, it will increase. If it’s punished, it will decrease.
  • Passive vs. active : In classical conditioning, the organism is passive and automatically responds to the conditioned stimulus. Operant conditioning requires the organism to perform a behavior that then gets reinforced or punished actively. The organism operates on the environment.
  • Involuntary vs. voluntary : Classical conditioning works with involuntary, reflexive responses like salivation, blinking, etc. Operant conditioning shapes voluntary behaviors that are controlled by the organism, like pressing a lever.
  • Association vs. reinforcement : Classical conditioning relies on associating stimuli in order to create a conditioned response. Operant conditioning depends on using reinforcement and punishment to increase or decrease voluntary behaviors.

Learning Check

  • In Ivan Pavlov’s famous experiment, he rang a bell before presenting food powder to dogs. Eventually, the dogs salivated at the mere sound of the bell. Identify the neutral stimulus, unconditioned stimulus, unconditioned response, conditioned stimulus, and conditioned response in Pavlov’s experiment.
  • A student loves going out for pizza and beer with friends on Fridays after class. Whenever one friend texts the group about Friday plans, the student immediately feels happy and excited. The friend starts texting the group on Thursdays when she wants the student to feel happier. Explain how this is an example of classical conditioning. Identify the UCS, UCR, CS, and CR.
  • A college student is traumatized after a car accident. She now feels fear every time she gets into a car. How could extinction be used to eliminate this acquired fear?
  • A professor always slams their book on the lectern right before giving a pop quiz. Students now feel anxiety whenever they hear the book slam. Is this classical conditioning? If so, identify the NS, UCS, UCR, CS, and CR.
  • Contrast classical conditioning and operant conditioning. How are they similar and different? Provide an original example of each type of conditioning.
  • How could the principles of classical conditioning be applied to help students overcome test anxiety?
  • Explain how taste aversion learning is an adaptive form of classical conditioning. Provide an original example.
  • What is second-order conditioning? Give an example and identify the stimuli and responses.
  • What is the role of extinction in classical conditioning? How could extinction be used in cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders?

Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder .  Psychological Review ,  108 (1), 4.

Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S. M., Johnson, D. R., Yehuda, R., & Charney, D. S. (1993). Childhood physical abuse and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans.  The American journal of psychiatry .

Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans.

Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta‐analysis of cue‐reactivity in addiction research.  Addiction, 94 (3), 327-340.

Davey, B. (1983). Think aloud: Modeling the cognitive processes of reading comprehension.  Journal of Reading, 27 (1), 44-47.

Dugdale, N., & Lowe, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence.

Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5 (3), 121–122.

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation.  Science, 122 , 157–158.

Hall, G. (2022). Extinction of conditioned flavor preferences.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition .

Logan, C. A. (2002). When scientific knowledge becomes scientific discovery: The disappearance of classical conditioning before Pavlov .  Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences ,  38 (4), 393-403.

Lovibond, P. F., & Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes ,  28 (1), 3.

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B.,…Rauch, S. L. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biological Psychiatry, 66 (12), 1075–82.

Pavlov, I. P. (1897/1902).  The work of the digestive glands . London: Griffin.

Thanellou, A., & Green, J. T. (2011). Spontaneous recovery but not reinstatement of the extinguished conditioned eyeblink response in the rat.  Behavioral Neuroscience ,  125 (4), 613.

Watson, J. B. (1913).  Psychology as the behaviorist views it .  Psychological Review, 20 , 158–177.

Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist Views It.  Psychological Review, 20 , 158-177.

Watson, J. B. (1924).  Behaviorism . New York: People’s Institute Publishing Company.

Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920).  Conditioned emotional reactions .  Journal of experimental psychology, 3 (1), 1.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Pavlov’s Dog: The Psychology Experiment That Changed Everything

Pavlov’s Dog is a well-known experiment in psychology that has been taught for decades. Ivan Pavlov , a Russian physiologist, discovered classical conditioning through his experiments with dogs. He found that dogs could be trained to associate a sound with food, causing them to salivate at the sound alone.

The experiment began with Pavlov ringing a bell every time he fed his dogs. After a while, the dogs began to associate the sound of the bell with food and would salivate at the sound alone, even if no food was present. This became known as a conditioned response, where a previously neutral stimulus (the bell) became associated with a natural response (salivating).

The experiment has been used to explain many psychological phenomena, including addiction, phobias, and anxiety. It has also been applied in therapy, where patients can learn to associate positive experiences with previously negative stimuli. The Pavlov’s Dog experiment is a crucial part of psychology’s history and continues to be studied today.

pavlov experiment conditioning

Pavlov’s Life and Career

Ivan Pavlov was a Russian physiologist who lived from 1849 to 1936. He is best known for his work in classical conditioning, a type of learning that occurs when a neutral stimulus is consistently paired with a stimulus that elicits a response. Pavlov was born in Ryazan, Russia, and studied at the University of St. Petersburg, where he received his doctorate in 1879.

Pavlov’s early research focused on the digestive system, and he discovered that the secretion of gastric juice was not a passive process but rather a response to stimuli. This led him to develop the concept of the conditioned reflex, which he explored in detail in his famous experiments with dogs.

In these experiments, Pavlov trained dogs to associate the sound of a bell with food presentation. Over time, the dogs began to salivate at the sound of the bell, even when no food was present. This demonstrated that a neutral stimulus (the bell) could become associated with a natural response (salivation) through repeated pairings with a stimulus that elicits that response (food).

Pavlov’s work had a profound impact on the field of psychology, and his ideas continue to influence research today. He was awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1904 for his work on the physiology of digestion. Still, his legacy is best remembered for his contributions to the study of learning and behavior.

Classical Conditioning

Classical conditioning is a type of learning that occurs when a neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with a stimulus that naturally elicits a response. Over time, the neutral stimulus becomes associated with the natural stimulus and begins to produce the same response. Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov first studied this type of learning in the late 1800s.

One of the most famous examples of classical conditioning is Pavlov’s experiment with dogs. In this experiment, Pavlov rang a bell every time he fed the dogs. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate at the sound of the bell, even when no food was present. The sound of the bell had become associated with the food, and the dogs had learned to associate the two stimuli.

Classical conditioning can be used to explain a variety of behaviors and responses. For example, a person who has been in a car accident may develop a fear of driving. The sound of screeching tires or the sight of a car may become associated with the traumatic experience, causing the person to feel anxious or fearful when driving.

Classical conditioning can also be used to treat certain types of phobias and anxiety disorders. By gradually exposing a person to the feared stimulus in a safe and controlled environment, the person can learn to associate the stimulus with safety and relaxation rather than fear and anxiety.

Classical conditioning is a powerful tool for understanding how we learn and respond to environmental stimuli. By understanding the principles of classical conditioning, we can better understand our behaviors and emotions, as well as those of others around us.

Pavlov’s Experiments

Pavlov’s experiments with dogs revolutionized the field of psychology and laid the foundation for the study of classical conditioning. In this section, we will explore two aspects of his experiments: salivating dogs and conditioned responses.

Salivating Dogs

Pavlov observed that dogs would salivate when presented with food. However, he also noticed that the dogs would start salivating before the food was presented. This led him to hypothesize that the dogs were responding not just to the food but to other associated stimuli, such as the sound of the food being prepared or the sight of the person who fed them.

To test his hypothesis, Pavlov began a series of experiments where he would ring a bell before presenting the dogs with food. After a few repetitions, the dogs began to salivate at the sound of the bell alone, even when no food was present. This demonstrated that the dogs had learned to associate the sound of the bell with the presence of food and were responding accordingly.

Conditioned Response

Pavlov’s experiments with dogs led to the discovery of the conditioned response, the learned response to a previously neutral stimulus. In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, the sound of the bell was originally a neutral stimulus. Still, it became associated with food and, therefore, elicited a response (salivation) from the dogs.

The conditioned response is an essential concept in psychology, as it helps to explain how we learn to respond to various stimuli in our environment. For example, if we have a positive experience with a particular food, we may develop a conditioned response to the sight or smell of that food, even if we are not hungry.

Pavlov’s experiments with dogs were groundbreaking in psychology and led to the discovery of classical conditioning and the conditioned response. By demonstrating that animals (and humans) can learn to respond to previously neutral stimuli, Pavlov paved the way for further research into the mechanisms of learning and behavior.

Significance in Psychology

Pavlov’s dog experiment has been a significant discovery in psychology. It has paved the way for developing various theories and has been instrumental in understanding human behavior. In this section, we will discuss the significance of Pavlov’s dog experiment in the context of behaviorism and learning theories.

Behaviorism

Pavlov’s dog experiment has been a cornerstone in the development of behaviorism. Behaviorism is a school of thought in psychology that emphasizes the importance of observable behavior rather than internal mental states. Pavlov’s experiment demonstrated how a stimulus-response connection could be formed through conditioning. This concept has been used to explain various behaviors, such as phobias and addictions.

Learning Theories

Pavlov’s dog experiment has also been significant in developing learning theories . Learning theories are concerned with how people acquire new knowledge and skills. Pavlov’s experiment demonstrated how classical conditioning could teach animals new behaviors. This concept has been used to explain various learning phenomena, such as the acquisition of language and the development of social skills.

In conclusion, Pavlov’s dog experiment has been a significant discovery in psychology. It has been instrumental in the development of behaviorism and learning theories. By understanding the principles of classical conditioning, we can better understand human behavior and how we learn new skills and behaviors.

Implications in Modern Psychology

Pavlov’s dog experiments have had a significant impact on modern psychology. His theory of classical conditioning has become a cornerstone of behaviorism, a school of thought that dominated psychology in the early 20th century. Today, it continues to influence psychologists and researchers in various fields.

One of the most significant implications of Pavlov’s work is the understanding of how learning takes place. His experiments showed that animals, including humans, can learn through association. This concept has been applied in many areas of modern psychology, including education, advertising, and even politics.

For example, in education, classical conditioning can improve students’ learning by associating positive experiences with specific subjects or activities. In advertising, classical conditioning can create positive associations between a product and a particular emotion or experience, influencing consumers’ purchasing decisions.

Moreover, Pavlov’s work has also contributed to developing other learning theories, such as operant conditioning, which focuses on the consequences of behavior rather than the stimuli that precede it. These theories have been used to explain various human behaviors, from addiction to language acquisition.

Pavlov’s dog experiments have had a lasting impact on modern psychology. His theory of classical conditioning has contributed to our understanding of how learning takes place and has been applied in various fields, from education to advertising. His work has also influenced the development of other learning theories, making it a crucial part of studying human behavior.

Criticism and Controversies

While Pavlov’s experiments have been foundational in psychology, they have also been subject to criticism and controversy. Here are a few examples:

  • Animal cruelty:  Some critics argue that Pavlov’s experiments on dogs were cruel and unethical. The dogs were often subjected to painful surgeries and kept in small cages for long periods. While these practices were common in the early 20th century, they would not be acceptable by today’s ethical standards.
  • Oversimplification of behavior:  Pavlov’s experiments focused on classical conditioning, which suggests that behavior is determined solely by external stimuli. However, this oversimplifies the complex nature of human behavior, which is influenced by various factors, including genetics, environment, and personal experience.
  • Limited generalizability:  Pavlov’s experiments were conducted on dogs, which may not accurately reflect human behavior. While some of the principles of classical conditioning may apply to humans, it is essential to recognize that there are also significant differences between species.
  • Misinterpretation of results:  Pavlov’s work has been subject to misinterpretation over the years. For example, many people believe that Pavlov’s dogs learned to salivate at the sound of a bell because they associated it with food. However, this is only partially accurate. The dogs learned to associate the sound of the bell with the experimenter’s presence, who would then provide the food.

Frequently Asked Questions

What were the basic features of classical conditioning discovered by pavlov.

Classical conditioning is a type of learning in which a neutral stimulus becomes associated with a meaningful stimulus, resulting in a behavioral response. Pavlov discovered that when a neutral stimulus (such as a bell) was repeatedly paired with a meaningful stimulus (such as food), the neutral stimulus alone could elicit the same response (such as salivation) as the meaningful stimulus.

What was the purpose of Pavlov’s dog experiment?

Pavlov’s dog experiment was designed to study the process of classical conditioning. He wanted to understand how dogs learn to associate a neutral stimulus (such as a bell) with a meaningful stimulus (such as food) and how this association leads to a behavioral response (such as salivation).

How did Pavlov’s experiments contribute to the development of psychology?

Pavlov’s experiments were groundbreaking in the field of psychology. They provided evidence for the concept of classical conditioning, which has since been used to explain a wide range of human and animal behaviors. Pavlov’s work also paved the way for the development of behaviorism, a school of psychology that emphasizes the importance of observable behavior in understanding human and animal psychology.

What is the Pavlovian response and how does it work?

The Pavlovian response is a learned response to a previously neutral stimulus. It works by pairing the neutral stimulus with a meaningful stimulus, which leads to the formation of an association between the two. Once the association is formed, the neutral stimulus alone can elicit the same response as the meaningful stimulus.

How is Pavlovian conditioning used in dog training?

Pavlovian conditioning is often used in dog training to teach dogs new behaviors or to modify existing ones. For example, a trainer might use a clicker (a neutral stimulus) to signal to a dog that it has performed a desired behavior (a meaningful stimulus), and then reward the dog with a treat. Over time, the dog will learn to associate the clicker with the reward and will perform the desired behavior without the need for a treat.

What is the Pavlovian response in humans and how is it studied?

The Pavlovian response in humans is similar to that in dogs: it involves the formation of an association between a neutral stimulus and a meaningful stimulus, resulting in a learned response. This response has been studied in a variety of contexts, including addiction, phobias, and taste aversions. Researchers use a variety of methods to study the Pavlovian response in humans, including brain imaging techniques and behavioral experiments.

Leave a Comment Cancel reply

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

  • Mental Health

Classical Conditioning: How It Works and Examples

pavlov experiment conditioning

What Is Classical Conditioning?

Classical conditioning, also called Pavlovian conditioning or respondent conditioning, is learning through association. This behavioral learning method was first studied in the late 19th century by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov. 

Pavlov’s dog experiment

In the 1890s, Pavlov was experimenting with dogs, ringing a bell whenever they were fed. Over time, the dogs learned to associate a neutral stimulus (bell ringing) with a positive one (food). Pavlov also noticed that his dogs would often begin to salivate whenever they heard the footsteps of his assistant bringing them the food. This is called a conditioned response. Pavlov's experiment and its association between positive and neutral stimuli became the foundation of classical conditioning theory.

Eventually, Pavlov linked these behavioral associations to humans. He spent the remainder of his career studying the phenomenon.

Terms to Know

To understand how classical conditioning works, it's helpful to understand the following terms.

  • Neutral stimulus. A stimulus is something that triggers a physical or behavioral change. A neutral stimulus produces no response. At first, Pavlov's dogs had no response to the bell.
  • Unconditioned stimulus. This is what leads to an automatic response. In Pavlov’s experiment, it's the food.
  • Unconditioned response . A normal process, like salivating when you smell food, is an unconditioned response.
  • Conditioned stimulus. This is when a formerly neutral stimulus, like the bell in Pavlov's experiment, mimics an unconditioned response, as when the dogs began to associate the bell with food and salivate.
  • Conditioned response. The learned behavior, such as relating the bell to food, is called a conditioned response.  

What Is Classical Conditioning Theory?

Classical conditioning theory says that behaviors are learned by connecting a neutral stimulus with a positive one, such as when Pavlov's dogs heard a bell (neutral) and expected food (positive).  

There are essentially three stages in classical conditioning:.

Before conditioning. Something in the environment triggers a natural response in the subject. During this stage, no new behavior has been learned yet. This stage also includes a neutral stimulus, which doesn't affect the subject. To create a response to a neutral stimulus, it must be linked to an unconditioned stimulus -- like the bell to food. 

During conditioning. This is the stage in which the subject starts to associate the neutral stimulus with the positive stimulus that caused the response during the first stage. In Pavlov's experiment, this stage involved ringing a bell when the dogs were fed. Over time, the dogs began to associate the bell with food.

For this to work, the neutral stimulus should come before the positive (unconditioned) stimulus. It creates a cue for what comes next. Doing this over and over makes the conditioning stick. But sometimes it only takes one time to make an association, such as a hangover after too much drinking. 

After conditioning. During the final stage of conditioning, the subject firmly associates the neutral stimulus with the unconditioned response. This creates a new behavior, or what's known as the conditioned response. If the link between the two weakens or breaks, this leads to what's called extinction. When Pavlov's dogs no longer got food after hearing the bell, they eventually stopped associating the bell with food.

What Is the Little Albert Experiment?

Considered one of the "most ethically dubious experiments ever conducted," the Little Albert experiment was developed by psychologists John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner, who first applied Pavlov's classical conditioning principles to human behavior.

In 1920, Watson and Rayner began their behavioral learning experiment with a 9-month-old boy named Albert. They tested his reactions to various things in his environment, including a white rat, burning newspapers, and a hammer striking a 4-foot steel bar just behind Albert's head. Because the sound of the hammer frightened Albert, it became the unconditioned stimulus, and fear became the unconditioned response.

When Albert was 11 months old, he was presented with the white rat. When he tried to pet it, the pipe was struck with the hammer, causing him to feel fear. The researchers did this over the next few weeks and eventually Albert saw the rat and showed a fearful response. 

They reproduced these results with a rabbit, a dog, and several other stimuli that were previously neutral. At the end of the experiment, Albert showed a fear response for all of them.

Classical Conditioning vs. Operant Conditioning

Classical conditioning relies on associating one stimulus with another, such as the sound of a bell with food. Learning through operant conditioning relies on what comes after behaviors. These are the consequences that reinforce or punish behaviors.

In operant conditioning, either positive or negative reinforcement is used to affect whether a behavior is likely to happen again.  

When you give your dog a treat after they follow a command, that's positive reinforcement. It encourages them to repeat the behavior. When you yell (punishment) after your dog grabs food off the counter, that's punishment or negative reinforcement. Like classical conditioning, operant conditioning requires repetition for learning to take place. 

Classical Conditioning Principles

Classical conditioning includes several steps:

Acquisition. The point at which the neutral stimulus and unconditioned stimulus become linked. In other words, the dog learns to relate the sound of the bell with food.  

Extinction. Extinction breaks the conditioned bonds between the stimuli. If the dog no longer sees food after hearing the bell, it will gradually stop associating the bell with food.

Spontaneous recovery. If, after extinction, the conditioned stimulus and neutral stimulus again appear in relationship to one another, the conditioned response will return. After the extinction of the conditioned response in his dogs, Pavlov rang the bell before producing the food a few days later. His dogs began to salivate at the sound of the bell again.

Generalization. A conditioned response may be produced with stimuli that are similar but not the same. For example, if Pavlov's dogs heard a bell that rang at a lower pitch and still salivated, that's generalization.

Discrimination. Discrimination is the ability to understand that two or more stimuli are different from one another. In Pavlov's experiment, he later introduced the dogs to two bell sounds. Food appeared only after one. The dogs soon learned the difference. 

Classical Conditioning Examples

Classical conditioning isn't just related to food or fear. You see examples of this type of conditioning every day, though you may not know it or consciously think about it. Here are some examples of classical conditioning in daily life.

  • Every time you put on your shoes, your dog gets excited and runs to the front door. Your dog associates you putting on shoes with a walk, or maybe going for a car ride.
  • You always buy the same type of crackers for your baby's morning snack. When you pull the box of crackers out of the cupboard, your baby gets excited and reaches toward the box because they associate that box with snack time.
  • A certain perfume reminds you of your late grandmother. After her passing, smelling that perfume or similar scents make you sad because of its association with your grandmother.
  • Your demanding boss occasionally berates underperforming employees in his office. You feel nervous or agitated whenever your boss asks one of your co-workers into his office and closes the door because that's what he does whenever someone's in trouble.
  • You listen to your favorite music when you exercise. You don't generally enjoy working out, but eventually, you begin to relate the positive feelings you get from your playlist to working out.
  • Advertising. You see an ad showing a cold, wet can of soda while pumping your gas. You start feeling thirsty and think about running inside and buying this soft drink. 

Classical Conditioning Uses

Psychologists consider classical conditioning a key type of learning. It can create changes in mental and physical health, emotions, and drive. Its uses include: 

  • Phobias. Repeated exposure to the object of a phobia, such as frequently flying when you're afraid of planes, can reduce fears.
  • Drug use. Counselors often urge former addicts to stay away from people and places associated with their drug use.
  • Classroom learning. Teachers might use classical conditioning to associate learning with positive emotions rather than negative ones like fear or shame.
  • Pet training. Classical conditioning taught Pavlov's dogs what to expect after they heard the bell: food. Your dog also learns to positively associate actions like picking up a leash with going for a walk or going out to pee.
  • Food aversions. We're born favoring certain tastes more than others (like sweet vs. bitter). If you eat something and become sick, you might learn to avoid the food and even feel sick at the sight of it. 
  • PTSD  For people with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) , classical conditioning may not cure their condition but contribute to it. PTSD is a type of anxiety that comes from associating certain triggers with fearful experiences. For example, loud noises may remind a veteran of the sounds of war. 

Criticisms of Classical Conditioning

Classical conditioning stresses outward learning over traits we're born with. Some criticisms of classical conditioning include:

  • It fails to consider complex human actions like thinking, reason, and memory that produce learning, too. 
  • It takes a long time to make the associations that create learning.
  • It assumes a lack of free will -- that people have no control over their reactions to stimuli.

Classical conditioning is a type of learning by association. It takes several steps to associate a neutral stimulus with a positive outcome. Classical conditioning is used to treat psychological problems such as drug addiction and phobias. But it's also the basis for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Classical conditioning appears in everyday life in advertising and in our sensory associations with good and bad events. 

Classical Conditioning FAQs

  • What is the simple definition of classical conditioning? Classical conditioning is learning through association.  
  • What is an example of classical conditioning? Listening to your favorite music during workouts is an example of associating exercise with a positive neutral stimulus.  
  • What are the five elements of classical conditioning?  Elements of classical conditioning include acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination.

person getting diagnosis from doctor

Top doctors in ,

Find more top doctors on, related links.

  • Mental Health Home
  • Mental Health News & Features
  • Mental Health Reference
  • Mental Health Quizzes
  • Mental Health Slideshows
  • Mental Health Blogs
  • Mental Health Videos
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Anxiety & Panic Disorders
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Crisis Assistance
  • Eating Disorders
  • Health & Balance
  • Personality Disorders
  • Schizophrenia
  • Social Media and Mental Health
  • Stress Management
  • Substance Abuse & Addiction
  • More Related Topics

pavlov experiment conditioning

Psychologist World

Learn More Psychology

  • Behavioral Psychology

Pavlov's Dogs and Classical Conditioning

How pavlov's experiments with dogs demonstrated that our behavior can be changed using conditioning..

Permalink Print   |  

Pavlov's Dogs and Classical Conditioning

One of the most revealing studies in behavioral psychology was carried out by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936) in a series of experiments today referred to as 'Pavlov's Dogs'. His research would become renowned for demonstrating the way in classical conditioning (also referred to as Pavlovian conditioning ) could be used to cultivate a particular association between the occurrence of one event in the anticipation of another.

  • Conditioning
  • Stimulus-Response Theory
  • Reductionism in Psychology
  • What Factors Affect Classical Conditioning?
  • Imprinting and Relationships

Pavlov's Dog Experiments

Pavlov came across classical conditioning unintentionally during his research into animals' gastric systems. Whilst measuring the salivation rates of dogs, he found that they would produce saliva when they heard or smelt food in anticipation of feeding. This is a normal reflex response which we would expect to happen as saliva plays a role in the digestion of food.

Did You Know?

Psychologist Edwin Twitmyer at the University of Pennsylvania in the U.S. discovered classical conditioning at approximately the same time as Pavlov was conducting his research ( Coon, 1982 ). 1 However, the two were unaware of each other's research in this case of simultaneous discovery , and Pavlov received credit for the findings.

However, the dogs also began to salivate when events occurred which would otherwise be unrelated to feeding. By playing sounds to the dogs prior to feeding them, Pavlov showed that they could be conditioned to unconsciously associate neutral, unrelated events with being fed 2 .

Experiment Procedure

Pavlov's dogs were each placed in an isolated environment and restrained in a harness, with a food bowl in front of them and a device was used to measure the rate at which their saliva glands made secretions. These measurements would then be recorded onto a revolving drum so that Pavlov could monitor salivation rates throughout the experiments.

He found that the dogs would begin to salivate when a door was opened for the researcher to feed them.

This response demonstrated the basic principle of classical conditioning . A neutral event, such as opening a door (a neutral stimulus , NS) could be associated with another event that followed - in this case, being fed (known as the unconditioned stimulus , UCS). This association could be created through repeating the neutral stimulus along with the unconditioned stimulus, which would become a conditioned stimulus , leading to a conditioned response : salivation.

Pavlov continued his research and tested a variety of other neutral stimuli which would otherwise be unlinked to the receipt of food. These included precise tones produced by a buzzer, the ticking of a metronome and electric shocks .

The dogs would demonstrate a similar association between these events and the food that followed.

NEUTRAL STIMULUS (NS, eg. tone) > UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS (UCS, eg. receiving food)

when repeated leads to:

CONDITIONED STIMULUS (CS, eg. tone) > CONDITIONED RESPONSE (CR, eg. salivation)

The implications for Pavlov's findings are significant as they can be applied to many animals, including humans.

For example, when you first saw someone holding a balloon and a pin close to it, you may have watched in anticipation as they burst the balloon. After this had happened multiple times, you would associate holding the pin to the balloon with the 'bang' that followed. Like Pavlov's dogs, classical conditioning was leading you to associate a neutral stimulus (the pin approaching a balloon) with bursting of the balloon, leading to a conditioned response (flinching, wincing or plugging one's ears) to this now conditioned stimulus.

  • Craik & Lockhart (1972) Levels of Processing Theory

Let us look now at some of the nuances of Pavlov's findings in relation to classical conditioning.

'Unconditioning' through experimental extinction

Once an animal has been inadvertently conditioned to produce a response to a stimulus, can this association ever be broken?

Pavlov presented the dogs with a tone which they would come to associate with food. He then played the tone but did not follow that by rewarding the dogs with food.

After he made the sound without food numerous times, the dogs' produced less saliva as the conditioning underwent experimental extinction - a case of 'unlearning' the association.

When experimental extinction occurs, is the association permanently broken?

Pavlov's research would suggest that it remains but is inactive after extinction, and can be re-activated by reinstating, for example, the food reward, as it was given during the original conditioning. This phenomenon is known as spontaneous recovery .

Forward Conditioning vs Backward Conditioning

During conditioning, it is important that the neutral stimulus (NS) is presented before the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) in order for learning to take place. This forward conditioning is more likely to lead to a conditioned response than when the neutral stimulus is presented after the conditioned stimulus has been provided ( backward conditioning ).

In the case of Pavlov's dogs, the tone must be played to the subject prior to the food being provided. Making a sound after the dogs have been fed may not lead to a conditioned association being made between the events.

Carr and Freeman (1919) attempted both forward and backward conditioning in rats, between a buzzer sound and closed doors in a maze. They found backward conditioning to be ineffective when compared to forward conditioning. 4

Delay Conditioning vs Trace Conditioning

We may use forward conditioning in one of two forms:

Delay Conditioning - when the unconditioned stimulus is provided prior to and during the unconditioned stimulus - there is a period of overlap where the neutral and unconditioned stimulus are given simultaneously, e.g. a buzzer sound begins, and after 10 seconds, food is given whilst the buzzer continues.

Trace Conditioning - when there is a delay after the unconditioned stimulus has been provided before the unconditioned stimulus is presented to the subject, e.g. buzzer sounds for 10 seconds, stops and after 10 seconds of silence (the trace interval ), food is presented.

Discussing delay conditioning, Pavlov (1927) asserted that the longer the delay between the stimuli, the more delayed the response would be 5 .

Temporal Conditioning

So far, we have looked at conditioning in which a neutral stimulus is key to eliciting a desired response. However, if an unconditioned stimulus is provided at regular intervals, even without a preceding neutral stimulus, animals' sense of timing will enable conditioning to take place, and a response may occur in time with the intervals.

For example, in a study in which rats were fed at either random or regular intervals, Kirkpatrick and Church (2003) found that the subjects underwent temporal conditioning in the anticipation of food when they were fed at set intervals. 6

Generalisation

Pavlov noticed that once neutral stimulus had been associated with an unconditioned stimulus, the conditioned stimulus could vary and the dogs would still generate a similar response. For example, once specific tone of buzzer sound was associated with food, differing toned buzzer sounds would solicit a conditioned response.

Nonetheless, the closer the stimulus was to the original stimulus used in conditioning, the clearer the response would be. This correlation between stimulus accuracy and response is referred to as a generalisation gradient , and has been demonstrated in studies such as Meulders et al (2013) . 7

Modern Classical Conditioning

Pavlov's dog experiments are still discussed today and have influenced many later ideas in psychology. The U.S. psychologist John B. Watson was impressed by Pavlov's findings and reproduced classical conditioning in the Little Albert Experiment (Watson, 1920), in which a subject was unethically conditioned to associate furry stimuli such as rabbits with a loud noise, and subsequently developed a fear of rats. 8

  • Behavioral Approach

The numerous studies following the experiments, which have demonstrated classical conditioning using a variety of methods, also show the replicability of Pavlov's research, helping it to be recognised as an important unconscious influence of human behavior. This has helped the theory to be recognised and applied in many real life situations, from training dogs to creating associations in today's product advertisements.

Continue Reading

  • Coon, D.J. (1982). Eponymy, obscurity, Twitmyer, and Pavlov. Journal of the History of Behavioral Science . 18 (3). 255-62.
  • Pavlov, I.P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Retrieved from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Pavlov/ .
  • Craik, F.I.M. and Lockhart, R.S. (1972). Levels of Processing: A Framework for Memory Research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Visual Behavior . 11 (6). 671-684.
  • Carr, H. and Freeman A. (1919). Time relationships in the formation of associations. Psychology Review . 26 (6). 335-353.
  • Pavlov, I.P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Retrieved from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Pavlov/lecture6.htm .
  • Kirkpatrick, K and Church, R.M. (2003). Tracking of the expected time to reinforcement in temporal conditioning processes. Learning & Behavior . 31 (1). 3-21.
  • Meulders A, Vandebroek, N. Vervliet, B. and Vlaeyen, J.W.S. (2013). Generalization Gradients in Cued and Contextual Pain-Related Fear: An Experimental Study in Health Participants. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience , 7 (345). 1-12.
  • Watson, J.B. and Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned Emotional Reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology . 3 (1). 1-14.
  • Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It. Psychological Review . (Watson, 1913). 20 . 158-177.

Which Archetype Are You?

Which Archetype Are You?

Are You Angry?

Are You Angry?

Windows to the Soul

Windows to the Soul

Are You Stressed?

Are You Stressed?

Attachment & Relationships

Attachment & Relationships

Memory Like A Goldfish?

Memory Like A Goldfish?

31 Defense Mechanisms

31 Defense Mechanisms

Slave To Your Role?

Slave To Your Role?

Which Archetype Are You?

Are You Fixated?

Are You Fixated?

Interpret Your Dreams

Interpret Your Dreams

How to Read Body Language

How to Read Body Language

How to Beat Stress and Succeed in Exams

pavlov experiment conditioning

More on Behavioral Psychology

Dressing To Impress

The psychology driving our clothing choices and how fashion affects your dating...

Fashion Psychology: What Clothes Say About You

Shock Therapy

Aversion therapy uses the principle that new behavior can be 'learnt' in order...

Immerse To Overcome?

If you jumped out of a plane, would you overcome your fear of heights?

Imprinting: Why First Impressions Matter

How first impressions from birth influence our relationship choices later in...

Bats & Goodwill

Why do we help other people? When Darwin introduced his theory of natural...

Sign Up for  Unlimited Access

Psychologist World

  • Psychology approaches, theories and studies explained
  • Body Language Reading Guide
  • How to Interpret Your Dreams Guide
  • Self Hypnosis Downloads
  • Plus More Member Benefits

You May Also Like...

Master body language, why do we dream, nap for performance, dark sense of humor linked to intelligence, making conversation, psychology of color, persuasion with ingratiation, brainwashed, psychology  guides.

Learn Body Language Reading

Learn Body Language Reading

How To Interpret Your Dreams

How To Interpret Your Dreams

Overcome Your Fears and Phobias

Overcome Your Fears and Phobias

Psychology topics, learn psychology.

Sign Up

  • Access 2,200+ insightful pages of psychology explanations & theories
  • Insights into the way we think and behave
  • Body Language & Dream Interpretation guides
  • Self hypnosis MP3 downloads and more
  • Eye Reading
  • Stress Test
  • Cognitive Approach
  • Fight-or-Flight Response
  • Neuroticism Test

© 2024 Psychologist World. Home About Contact Us Terms of Use Privacy & Cookies Hypnosis Scripts Sign Up

Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Identify the NS, UCS, UCR, CS, and CR in classical conditioning situations

Does the name Ivan Pavlov ring a bell? Even if you are new to the study of psychology, chances are that you have heard of Pavlov and his famous dogs.

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning (Figure 1). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

A portrait shows Ivan Pavlov.

Figure 1 . Ivan Pavlov’s research on the digestive system of dogs unexpectedly led to his discovery of the learning process now known as classical conditioning.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants’ footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs’ “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder (Figure 2). The tone was the neutral stimulus (NS), which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs. Quite simply this pairing means:

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behavior caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Two illustrations are labeled “before conditioning” and show a dog salivating over a dish of food, and a dog not salivating while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “during conditioning” shows a dog salivating over a bowl of food while a bell is rung. An illustration labeled “after conditioning” shows a dog salivating while a bell is rung.

Figure 2 . Before conditioning, an unconditioned stimulus (food) produces an unconditioned response (salivation), and a neutral stimulus (bell) does not produce a response. During conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus (food) is presented repeatedly just after the presentation of the neutral stimulus (bell). After conditioning, the neutral stimulus alone produces a conditioned response (salivation), thus becoming a conditioned stimulus.

View the following video to learn more about Pavlov and his dogs:

You can view the transcript for “Classical Conditioning – Ivan Pavlov” here (opens in new window) .

Real World Application of Classical Conditioning

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Consider the case of Moisha, who was diagnosed with cancer. When she received her first chemotherapy treatment, she vomited shortly after the chemicals were injected. In fact, every trip to the doctor for chemotherapy treatment shortly after the drugs were injected, she vomited. Moisha’s treatment was a success and her cancer went into remission. Now, when she visits her oncologist’s office every 6 months for a check-up, she becomes nauseous. In this case, the chemotherapy drugs are the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), vomiting is the unconditioned response (UCR), the doctor’s office is the conditioned stimulus (CS) after being paired with the UCS, and nausea is the conditioned response (CR). Let’s assume that the chemotherapy drugs that Moisha takes are given through a syringe injection. After entering the doctor’s office, Moisha sees a syringe, and then gets her medication. In addition to the doctor’s office, Moisha will learn to associate the syringe with the medication and will respond to syringes with nausea. This is an example of higher-order (or second-order) conditioning, when the conditioned stimulus (the doctor’s office) serves to condition another stimulus (the syringe). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if someone rang a bell every time Moisha received a syringe injection of chemotherapy drugs in the doctor’s office, Moisha likely will never get sick in response to the bell.

Consider another example of classical conditioning. Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called higher-order conditioning, or second-order conditioning. This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet (Figure 3). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if you ring a bell, open the cabinet (“squeak”), use the can opener (“zzhzhz”), and then feed Tiger, Tiger will likely never get excited when hearing the bell alone.

A diagram is labeled “Higher-Order / Second-Order Conditioning” and has three rows. The first row shows an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus (CS)” followed by a plus sign and then a dish of food labeled “unconditioned stimulus (UCS)” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “unconditioned response (UCR).” The second row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by a plus sign and then an electric can opener labeled “conditioned stimulus (CS)” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response (CR).” The third row shows a squeaky cabinet door labeled “second-order stimulus” followed by an equal sign and a picture of a salivating cat labeled “conditioned response (CR).”

Figure 3 . In higher-order conditioning, an established conditioned stimulus is paired with a new neutral stimulus (the second-order stimulus), so that eventually the new stimulus also elicits the conditioned response, without the initial conditioned stimulus being presented.

Everyday Connection: Classical Conditioning at Stingray City

A photograph shows a woman standing in the ocean holding a stingray.

Figure 4 . Kate holds a southern stingray at Stingray City in the Cayman Islands. These stingrays have been classically conditioned to associate the sound of a boat motor with food provided by tourists. (credit: Kathryn Dumper)

Kate and her husband Scott recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, fishermen began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the fishermen threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as Kate and Scott reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats (Figure 4). Kate and Scott were able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Sara buys formula in blue canisters for her six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Sara takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider our earlier example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries.

For a humorous look at conditioning, you can watch an example from the television show  The Office . Jim conducts an experiment in which he offers Dwight a breath mint every time Jim’s computer makes a specific sound. After repeating this several times, he eventually conditions Dwight to automatically expect a breath mint upon hearing that sound.   See if you can identify the NS, UCS, UCR, CS, and CR.

Review the classical conditioning concepts yet again by walking through Pavlov’s research in the following interactive:

Think It Over

Can you think of an example in your life of how classical conditioning has produced a positive emotional response, such as happiness or excitement? How about a negative emotional response, such as fear, anxiety, or anger?

  • Modification and adaptation, addition of tutorial. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Classical conditioning interactive. Authored by : Jessica Traylor for Lumen Learning. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Classical Conditioning. Authored by : OpenStax College. Located at : https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/6-2-classical-conditioning . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • Classical Conditioning - Ivan Pavlov. Authored by : BullyingNewsVideos. Located at : https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hhqumfpxuzI . License : Other . License Terms : Standard YouTube License
  • The Office Classical Conditioning. Authored by : Susann Stanley. Provided by : Vimeo. Located at : https://vimeo.com/35754924 . License : All Rights Reserved

Footer Logo Lumen Waymaker

6.2 Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain how classical conditioning occurs
  • Summarize the processes of acquisition, extinction, spontaneous recovery, generalization, and discrimination

Does the name Ivan Pavlov ring a bell? Even if you are new to the study of psychology, chances are that you have heard of Pavlov and his famous dogs.

Pavlov (1849–1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning ( Figure 6.3 ). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

Pavlov came to his conclusions about how learning occurs completely by accident. Pavlov was a physiologist, not a psychologist. Physiologists study the life processes of organisms, from the molecular level to the level of cells, organ systems, and entire organisms. Pavlov’s area of interest was the digestive system (Hunt, 2007). In his studies with dogs, Pavlov measured the amount of saliva produced in response to various foods. Over time, Pavlov (1927) observed that the dogs began to salivate not only at the taste of food, but also at the sight of food, at the sight of an empty food bowl, and even at the sound of the laboratory assistants' footsteps. Salivating to food in the mouth is reflexive, so no learning is involved. However, dogs don’t naturally salivate at the sight of an empty bowl or the sound of footsteps.

These unusual responses intrigued Pavlov, and he wondered what accounted for what he called the dogs' “psychic secretions” (Pavlov, 1927). To explore this phenomenon in an objective manner, Pavlov designed a series of carefully controlled experiments to see which stimuli would cause the dogs to salivate. He was able to train the dogs to salivate in response to stimuli that clearly had nothing to do with food, such as the sound of a bell, a light, and a touch on the leg. Through his experiments, Pavlov realized that an organism has two types of responses to its environment: (1) unconditioned (unlearned) responses, or reflexes, and (2) conditioned (learned) responses.

In Pavlov’s experiments, the dogs salivated each time meat powder was presented to them. The meat powder in this situation was an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) : a stimulus that elicits a reflexive response in an organism. The dogs’ salivation was an unconditioned response (UCR) : a natural (unlearned) reaction to a given stimulus. Before conditioning, think of the dogs’ stimulus and response like this:

In classical conditioning, a neutral stimulus is presented immediately before an unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov would sound a tone (like ringing a bell) and then give the dogs the meat powder ( Figure 6.4 ). The tone was the neutral stimulus (NS) , which is a stimulus that does not naturally elicit a response. Prior to conditioning, the dogs did not salivate when they just heard the tone because the tone had no association for the dogs.

When Pavlov paired the tone with the meat powder over and over again, the previously neutral stimulus (the tone) also began to elicit salivation from the dogs. Thus, the neutral stimulus became the conditioned stimulus (CS) , which is a stimulus that elicits a response after repeatedly being paired with an unconditioned stimulus. Eventually, the dogs began to salivate to the tone alone, just as they previously had salivated at the sound of the assistants’ footsteps. The behavior caused by the conditioned stimulus is called the conditioned response (CR) . In the case of Pavlov’s dogs, they had learned to associate the tone (CS) with being fed, and they began to salivate (CR) in anticipation of food.

Link to Learning

View this video about Pavlov and his dogs to learn more.

Real World Application of Classical Conditioning

How does classical conditioning work in the real world? Consider the case of Moisha, who was diagnosed with cancer. When she received her first chemotherapy treatment, she vomited shortly after the chemicals were injected. In fact, every trip to the doctor for chemotherapy treatment shortly after the drugs were injected, she vomited. Moisha’s treatment was a success and her cancer went into remission. Now, when she visits her oncologist's office every 6 months for a check-up, she becomes nauseous. In this case, the chemotherapy drugs are the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), vomiting is the unconditioned response (UCR), the doctor’s office is the conditioned stimulus (CS) after being paired with the UCS, and nausea is the conditioned response (CR). Let's assume that the chemotherapy drugs that Moisha takes are given through a syringe injection. After entering the doctor's office, Moisha sees a syringe, and then gets her medication. In addition to the doctor's office, Moisha will learn to associate the syringe with the medication and will respond to syringes with nausea. This is an example of higher-order (or second-order) conditioning, when the conditioned stimulus (the doctor's office) serves to condition another stimulus (the syringe). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if someone rang a bell every time Moisha received a syringe injection of chemotherapy drugs in the doctor's office, Moisha likely will never get sick in response to the bell.

Consider another example of classical conditioning. Let’s say you have a cat named Tiger, who is quite spoiled. You keep her food in a separate cabinet, and you also have a special electric can opener that you use only to open cans of cat food. For every meal, Tiger hears the distinctive sound of the electric can opener (“zzhzhz”) and then gets her food. Tiger quickly learns that when she hears “zzhzhz” she is about to get fed. What do you think Tiger does when she hears the electric can opener? She will likely get excited and run to where you are preparing her food. This is an example of classical conditioning. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR?

What if the cabinet holding Tiger’s food becomes squeaky? In that case, Tiger hears “squeak” (the cabinet), “zzhzhz” (the electric can opener), and then she gets her food. Tiger will learn to get excited when she hears the “squeak” of the cabinet. Pairing a new neutral stimulus (“squeak”) with the conditioned stimulus (“zzhzhz”) is called higher-order conditioning , or second-order conditioning . This means you are using the conditioned stimulus of the can opener to condition another stimulus: the squeaky cabinet ( Figure 6.5 ). It is hard to achieve anything above second-order conditioning. For example, if you ring a bell, open the cabinet (“squeak”), use the can opener (“zzhzhz”), and then feed Tiger, Tiger will likely never get excited when hearing the bell alone.

Everyday Connection

Classical conditioning at stingray city.

Kate and her spouse recently vacationed in the Cayman Islands, and booked a boat tour to Stingray City, where they could feed and swim with the southern stingrays. The boat captain explained how the normally solitary stingrays have become accustomed to interacting with humans. About 40 years ago, people began to clean fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a particular sandbar near a barrier reef, and large numbers of stingrays would swim in to eat (unconditioned response) what the people threw into the water; this continued for years. By the late 1980s, word of the large group of stingrays spread among scuba divers, who then started feeding them by hand. Over time, the southern stingrays in the area were classically conditioned much like Pavlov’s dogs. When they hear the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus that becomes a conditioned stimulus), they know that they will get to eat (conditioned response).

As soon as they reached Stingray City, over two dozen stingrays surrounded their tour boat. The couple slipped into the water with bags of squid, the stingrays’ favorite treat. The swarm of stingrays bumped and rubbed up against their legs like hungry cats ( Figure 6.6 ). Kate was able to feed, pet, and even kiss (for luck) these amazing creatures. Then all the squid was gone, and so were the stingrays.

Classical conditioning also applies to humans, even babies. For example, Elan buys formula in blue canisters for their six-month-old daughter, Angelina. Whenever Elan takes out a formula container, Angelina gets excited, tries to reach toward the food, and most likely salivates. Why does Angelina get excited when she sees the formula canister? What are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR here?

So far, all of the examples have involved food, but classical conditioning extends beyond the basic need to be fed. Consider our earlier example of a dog whose owners install an invisible electric dog fence. A small electrical shock (unconditioned stimulus) elicits discomfort (unconditioned response). When the unconditioned stimulus (shock) is paired with a neutral stimulus (the edge of a yard), the dog associates the discomfort (unconditioned response) with the edge of the yard (conditioned stimulus) and stays within the set boundaries. In this example, the edge of the yard elicits fear and anxiety in the dog. Fear and anxiety are the conditioned response.

Watch this video clip from the television show, The Office , for a humorous look at conditioning in which Jim conditions Dwight to expect a breath mint every time Jim’s computer makes a specific sound.

General Processes in Classical Conditioning

Now that you know how classical conditioning works and have seen several examples, let’s take a look at some of the general processes involved. In classical conditioning, the initial period of learning is known as acquisition , when an organism learns to connect a neutral stimulus and an unconditioned stimulus. During acquisition, the neutral stimulus begins to elicit the conditioned response, and eventually the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of eliciting the conditioned response by itself. Timing is important for conditioning to occur. Typically, there should only be a brief interval between presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus. Depending on what is being conditioned, sometimes this interval is as little as five seconds (Chance, 2009). However, with other types of conditioning, the interval can be up to several hours.

Taste aversion is a type of conditioning in which an interval of several hours may pass between the conditioned stimulus (something ingested) and the unconditioned stimulus (nausea or illness). Here’s an example. Harry went to the carnival. He ate a lot of cotton candy and later that night was very sick and threw up. The next day, his friend offered him a piece of candy. He put it into his mouth and started to feel sick and had to spit it out. The unconditioned stimulus is eating too much cotton candy. The unconditioned response is getting sick and throwing up. The conditioned stimulus is the sugary flavor and the conditioned response is Harry feeling nauseous at the taste of sugar.

How does this occur—conditioning based on a single instance and involving an extended time lapse between the event and the negative stimulus? Research into taste aversion suggests that this response may be an evolutionary adaptation designed to help organisms quickly learn to avoid harmful foods (Garcia & Rusiniak, 1980; Garcia & Koelling, 1966). Not only may this contribute to species survival via natural selection, but it may also help us develop strategies for challenges such as helping cancer patients through the nausea induced by certain treatments (Holmes, 1993; Jacobsen et al., 1993; Hutton, Baracos, & Wismer, 2007; Skolin et al., 2006). Garcia and Koelling (1966) showed not only that taste aversions could be conditioned, but also that there were biological constraints to learning. In their study, separate groups of rats were conditioned to associate either a flavor with illness, or lights and sounds with illness. Results showed that all rats exposed to flavor-illness pairings learned to avoid the flavor, but none of the rats exposed to lights and sounds with illness learned to avoid lights or sounds. This added evidence to the idea that classical conditioning could contribute to species survival by helping organisms learn to avoid stimuli that posed real dangers to health and welfare.

Robert Rescorla demonstrated how powerfully an organism can learn to predict the UCS from the CS. Take, for example, the following two situations. Ari’s dad always has dinner on the table every day at 6:00. Soraya’s mom switches it up so that some days they eat dinner at 6:00, some days they eat at 5:00, and other days they eat at 7:00. For Ari, 6:00 reliably and consistently predicts dinner, so Ari will likely start feeling hungry every day right before 6:00, even if he's had a late snack. Soraya, on the other hand, will be less likely to associate 6:00 with dinner, since 6:00 does not always predict that dinner is coming. Rescorla, along with his colleague at Yale University, Allan Wagner, developed a mathematical formula that could be used to calculate the probability that an association would be learned given the ability of a conditioned stimulus to predict the occurrence of an unconditioned stimulus and other factors; today this is known as the Rescorla-Wagner model (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972)

Once we have established the connection between the unconditioned stimulus and the conditioned stimulus, how do we break that connection and get the dog, cat, or child to stop responding? In Tiger’s case, imagine what would happen if you stopped using the electric can opener for her food and began to use it only for human food. Now, Tiger would hear the can opener, but she would not get food. In classical conditioning terms, you would be giving the conditioned stimulus, but not the unconditioned stimulus. Pavlov explored this scenario in his experiments with dogs: sounding the tone without giving the dogs the meat powder. Soon the dogs stopped responding to the tone. Extinction is the decrease in the conditioned response when the unconditioned stimulus is no longer presented with the conditioned stimulus. When presented with the conditioned stimulus alone, the dog, cat, or other organism would show a weaker and weaker response, and finally no response. In classical conditioning terms, there is a gradual weakening and disappearance of the conditioned response.

What happens when learning is not used for a while—when what was learned lies dormant? As we just discussed, Pavlov found that when he repeatedly presented the bell (conditioned stimulus) without the meat powder (unconditioned stimulus), extinction occurred; the dogs stopped salivating to the bell. However, after a couple of hours of resting from this extinction training, the dogs again began to salivate when Pavlov rang the bell. What do you think would happen with Tiger’s behavior if your electric can opener broke, and you did not use it for several months? When you finally got it fixed and started using it to open Tiger’s food again, Tiger would remember the association between the can opener and her food—she would get excited and run to the kitchen when she heard the sound. The behavior of Pavlov’s dogs and Tiger illustrates a concept Pavlov called spontaneous recovery : the return of a previously extinguished conditioned response following a rest period ( Figure 6.7 ).

Of course, these processes also apply in humans. For example, let’s say that every day when you walk to campus, an ice cream truck passes your route. Day after day, you hear the truck’s music (neutral stimulus), so you finally stop and purchase a chocolate ice cream bar. You take a bite (unconditioned stimulus) and then your mouth waters (unconditioned response). This initial period of learning is known as acquisition, when you begin to connect the neutral stimulus (the sound of the truck) and the unconditioned stimulus (the taste of the chocolate ice cream in your mouth). During acquisition, the conditioned response gets stronger and stronger through repeated pairings of the conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. Several days (and ice cream bars) later, you notice that your mouth begins to water (conditioned response) as soon as you hear the truck’s musical jingle—even before you bite into the ice cream bar. Then one day you head down the street. You hear the truck’s music (conditioned stimulus), and your mouth waters (conditioned response). However, when you get to the truck, you discover that they are all out of ice cream. You leave disappointed. The next few days you pass by the truck and hear the music, but don’t stop to get an ice cream bar because you’re running late for class. You begin to salivate less and less when you hear the music, until by the end of the week, your mouth no longer waters when you hear the tune. This illustrates extinction. The conditioned response weakens when only the conditioned stimulus (the sound of the truck) is presented, without being followed by the unconditioned stimulus (chocolate ice cream in the mouth). Then the weekend comes. You don’t have to go to class, so you don’t pass the truck. Monday morning arrives and you take your usual route to campus. You round the corner and hear the truck again. What do you think happens? Your mouth begins to water again. Why? After a break from conditioning, the conditioned response reappears, which indicates spontaneous recovery.

Acquisition and extinction involve the strengthening and weakening, respectively, of a learned association. Two other learning processes—stimulus discrimination and stimulus generalization—are involved in determining which stimuli will trigger learned responses. Animals (including humans) need to distinguish between stimuli—for example, between sounds that predict a threatening event and sounds that do not—so that they can respond appropriately (such as running away if the sound is threatening). When an organism learns to respond differently to various stimuli that are similar, it is called stimulus discrimination . In classical conditioning terms, the organism demonstrates the conditioned response only to the conditioned stimulus. Pavlov’s dogs discriminated between the basic tone that sounded before they were fed and other tones (e.g., the doorbell), because the other sounds did not predict the arrival of food. Similarly, Tiger, the cat, discriminated between the sound of the can opener and the sound of the electric mixer. When the electric mixer is going, Tiger is not about to be fed, so she does not come running to the kitchen looking for food. In our other example, Moisha, the cancer patient, discriminated between oncologists and other types of doctors. She learned not to feel ill when visiting doctors for other types of appointments, such as her annual physical.

On the other hand, when an organism demonstrates the conditioned response to stimuli that are similar to the condition stimulus, it is called stimulus generalization , the opposite of stimulus discrimination. The more similar a stimulus is to the condition stimulus, the more likely the organism is to give the conditioned response. For instance, if the electric mixer sounds very similar to the electric can opener, Tiger may come running after hearing its sound. But if you do not feed her following the electric mixer sound, and you continue to feed her consistently after the electric can opener sound, she will quickly learn to discriminate between the two sounds (provided they are sufficiently dissimilar that she can tell them apart). In our other example, Moisha continued to feel ill whenever visiting other oncologists or other doctors in the same building as her oncologist.

Behaviorism

John B. Watson , shown in Figure 6.8 , is considered the founder of behaviorism. Behaviorism is a school of thought that arose during the first part of the 20th century, which incorporates elements of Pavlov’s classical conditioning (Hunt, 2007). In stark contrast with Freud, who considered the reasons for behavior to be hidden in the unconscious, Watson championed the idea that all behavior can be studied as a simple stimulus-response reaction, without regard for internal processes. Watson argued that in order for psychology to become a legitimate science, it must shift its concern away from internal mental processes because mental processes cannot be seen or measured. Instead, he asserted that psychology must focus on outward observable behavior that can be measured.

Watson’s ideas were influenced by Pavlov’s work. According to Watson, human behavior, just like animal behavior, is primarily the result of conditioned responses. Whereas Pavlov’s work with dogs involved the conditioning of reflexes, Watson believed the same principles could be extended to the conditioning of human emotions (Watson, 1919).

In 1920, while chair of the psychology department at Johns Hopkins University, Watson and his graduate student, Rosalie Rayner, conducted research on a baby nicknamed Little Albert. Rayner and Watson’s experiments with Little Albert demonstrated how fears can be conditioned using classical conditioning. Through these experiments, Little Albert was exposed to and conditioned to fear certain things. Initially he was presented with various neutral stimuli, including a rabbit, a dog, a monkey, masks, cotton wool, and a white rat. He was not afraid of any of these things. Then Watson, with the help of Rayner, conditioned Little Albert to associate these stimuli with an emotion—fear. For example, Watson handed Little Albert the white rat, and Little Albert enjoyed playing with it. Then Watson made a loud sound, by striking a hammer against a metal bar hanging behind Little Albert’s head, each time Little Albert touched the rat. Little Albert was frightened by the sound—demonstrating a reflexive fear of sudden loud noises—and began to cry. Watson repeatedly paired the loud sound with the white rat. Soon Little Albert became frightened by the white rat alone. In this case, what are the UCS, CS, UCR, and CR? Days later, Little Albert demonstrated stimulus generalization—he became afraid of other furry things: a rabbit, a furry coat, and even a Santa Claus mask ( Figure 6.9 ). Watson had succeeded in conditioning a fear response in Little Albert, thus demonstrating that emotions could become conditioned responses. It had been Watson’s intention to produce a phobia—a persistent, excessive fear of a specific object or situation— through conditioning alone, thus countering Freud’s view that phobias are caused by deep, hidden conflicts in the mind. However, there is no evidence that Little Albert experienced phobias in later years. While Watson’s research provided new insight into conditioning, it would be considered unethical by today’s standards.

View scenes from this video on John Watson’s experiment in which Little Albert was conditioned to respond in fear to furry objects to learn more.

As you watch the video, look closely at Little Albert’s reactions and the manner in which Watson and Rayner present the stimuli before and after conditioning. Based on what you see, would you come to the same conclusions as the researchers?

Advertising and Associative Learning

Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement for the same car minus the model.

Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with their product (the conditioned stimulus).

Now that you are aware of how associative learning works, see if you can find examples of these types of advertisements on television, in magazines, or on the Internet.

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • Authors: Rose M. Spielman, William J. Jenkins, Marilyn D. Lovett
  • Publisher/website: OpenStax
  • Book title: Psychology 2e
  • Publication date: Apr 22, 2020
  • Location: Houston, Texas
  • Book URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • Section URL: https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/6-2-classical-conditioning

© Jun 26, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.

Logo for M Libraries Publishing

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

7.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

  • Describe how Pavlov’s early work in classical conditioning influenced the understanding of learning.
  • Review the concepts of classical conditioning, including unconditioned stimulus (US), conditioned stimulus (CS), unconditioned response (UR), and conditioned response (CR).
  • Explain the roles that extinction, generalization, and discrimination play in conditioned learning.

Pavlov Demonstrates Conditioning in Dogs

In the early part of the 20th century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) was studying the digestive system of dogs when he noticed an interesting behavioral phenomenon: The dogs began to salivate when the lab technicians who normally fed them entered the room, even though the dogs had not yet received any food. Pavlov realized that the dogs were salivating because they knew that they were about to be fed; the dogs had begun to associate the arrival of the technicians with the food that soon followed their appearance in the room.

Figure 7.2 Ivan Pavlov

Ivan Pavlov

Ivan Pavlov’s research made substantial contributions to our understanding of learning.

LIFE Photo Archive – Wikimedia Commons – public domain.

With his team of researchers, Pavlov began studying this process in more detail. He conducted a series of experiments in which, over a number of trials, dogs were exposed to a sound immediately before receiving food. He systematically controlled the onset of the sound and the timing of the delivery of the food, and recorded the amount of the dogs’ salivation. Initially the dogs salivated only when they saw or smelled the food, but after several pairings of the sound and the food, the dogs began to salivate as soon as they heard the sound. The animals had learned to associate the sound with the food that followed.

Pavlov had identified a fundamental associative learning process called classical conditioning . Classical conditioning refers to learning that occurs when a neutral stimulus (e.g., a tone) becomes associated with a stimulus (e.g., food) that naturally produces a behavior . After the association is learned, the previously neutral stimulus is sufficient to produce the behavior.

As you can see in Figure 7.3 “4-Panel Image of Whistle and Dog” , psychologists use specific terms to identify the stimuli and the responses in classical conditioning. The unconditioned stimulus (US) is something (such as food) that triggers a natural occurring response , and the unconditioned response (UR) is the naturally occurring response (such as salivation) that follows the unconditioned stimulus . The conditioned stimulus (CS) is a neutral stimulus that, after being repeatedly presented prior to the unconditioned stimulus, evokes a similar response as the unconditioned stimulus . In Pavlov’s experiment, the sound of the tone served as the conditioned stimulus that, after learning, produced the conditioned response (CR) , which is the acquired response to the formerly neutral stimulus . Note that the UR and the CR are the same behavior—in this case salivation—but they are given different names because they are produced by different stimuli (the US and the CS, respectively).

Figure 7.3 4-Panel Image of Whistle and Dog

Top left: Before conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus (US) naturally produces the unconditioned response (UR). Top right: Before conditioning, the neutral stimulus (the whistle) does not produce the salivation response. Bottom left: The unconditioned stimulus (US), in this case the food, is repeatedly presented immediately after the neutral stimulus. Bottom right: After learning, the neutral stimulus (now known as the conditioned stimulus or CS), is sufficient to produce the conditioned responses (CR).

Top left: Before conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus (US) naturally produces the unconditioned response (UR). Top right: Before conditioning, the neutral stimulus (the whistle) does not produce the salivation response. Bottom left: The unconditioned stimulus (US), in this case the food, is repeatedly presented immediately after the neutral stimulus. Bottom right: After learning, the neutral stimulus (now known as the conditioned stimulus or CS), is sufficient to produce the conditioned responses (CR).

Conditioning is evolutionarily beneficial because it allows organisms to develop expectations that help them prepare for both good and bad events. Imagine, for instance, that an animal first smells a new food, eats it, and then gets sick. If the animal can learn to associate the smell (CS) with the food (US), then it will quickly learn that the food creates the negative outcome, and not eat it the next time.

The Persistence and Extinction of Conditioning

After he had demonstrated that learning could occur through association, Pavlov moved on to study the variables that influenced the strength and the persistence of conditioning. In some studies, after the conditioning had taken place, Pavlov presented the sound repeatedly but without presenting the food afterward. Figure 7.4 “Acquisition, Extinction, and Spontaneous Recovery” shows what happened. As you can see, after the intial acquisition (learning) phase in which the conditioning occurred, when the CS was then presented alone, the behavior rapidly decreased—the dogs salivated less and less to the sound, and eventually the sound did not elicit salivation at all. Extinction refers to the reduction in responding that occurs when the conditioned stimulus is presented repeatedly without the unconditioned stimulus .

Figure 7.4 Acquisition, Extinction, and Spontaneous Recovery

Acquisition: The CS and the US are repeatedly paired together and behavior increases. Extinction: The CS is repeatedly presented alone, and the behavior slowly decreases. Spontaneous recovery: After a pause, when the CS is again presented alone, the behavior may again occur and then again show extinction.

Acquisition: The CS and the US are repeatedly paired together and behavior increases. Extinction: The CS is repeatedly presented alone, and the behavior slowly decreases. Spontaneous recovery: After a pause, when the CS is again presented alone, the behavior may again occur and then again show extinction.

Although at the end of the first extinction period the CS was no longer producing salivation, the effects of conditioning had not entirely disappeared. Pavlov found that, after a pause, sounding the tone again elicited salivation, although to a lesser extent than before extinction took place. The increase in responding to the CS following a pause after extinction is known as spontaneous recovery . When Pavlov again presented the CS alone, the behavior again showed extinction until it disappeared again.

Although the behavior has disappeared, extinction is never complete. If conditioning is again attempted, the animal will learn the new associations much faster than it did the first time.

Pavlov also experimented with presenting new stimuli that were similar, but not identical to, the original conditioned stimulus. For instance, if the dog had been conditioned to being scratched before the food arrived, the stimulus would be changed to being rubbed rather than scratched. He found that the dogs also salivated upon experiencing the similar stimulus, a process known as generalization . Generalization refers to the tendency to respond to stimuli that resemble the original conditioned stimulus . The ability to generalize has important evolutionary significance. If we eat some red berries and they make us sick, it would be a good idea to think twice before we eat some purple berries. Although the berries are not exactly the same, they nevertheless are similar and may have the same negative properties.

Lewicki (1985) conducted research that demonstrated the influence of stimulus generalization and how quickly and easily it can happen. In his experiment, high school students first had a brief interaction with a female experimenter who had short hair and glasses. The study was set up so that the students had to ask the experimenter a question, and (according to random assignment) the experimenter responded either in a negative way or a neutral way toward the students. Then the students were told to go into a second room in which two experimenters were present, and to approach either one of them. However, the researchers arranged it so that one of the two experimenters looked a lot like the original experimenter, while the other one did not (she had longer hair and no glasses). The students were significantly more likely to avoid the experimenter who looked like the earlier experimenter when that experimenter had been negative to them than when she had treated them more neutrally. The participants showed stimulus generalization such that the new, similar-looking experimenter created the same negative response in the participants as had the experimenter in the prior session.

The flip side of generalization is discrimination — the tendency to respond differently to stimuli that are similar but not identical . Pavlov’s dogs quickly learned, for example, to salivate when they heard the specific tone that had preceded food, but not upon hearing similar tones that had never been associated with food. Discrimination is also useful—if we do try the purple berries, and if they do not make us sick, we will be able to make the distinction in the future. And we can learn that although the two people in our class, Courtney and Sarah, may look a lot alike, they are nevertheless different people with different personalities.

In some cases, an existing conditioned stimulus can serve as an unconditioned stimulus for a pairing with a new conditioned stimulus —a process known as second-order conditioning . In one of Pavlov’s studies, for instance, he first conditioned the dogs to salivate to a sound, and then repeatedly paired a new CS, a black square, with the sound. Eventually he found that the dogs would salivate at the sight of the black square alone, even though it had never been directly associated with the food. Secondary conditioners in everyday life include our attractions to things that stand for or remind us of something else, such as when we feel good on a Friday because it has become associated with the paycheck that we receive on that day, which itself is a conditioned stimulus for the pleasures that the paycheck buys us.

The Role of Nature in Classical Conditioning

As we have seen in Chapter 1 “Introducing Psychology” , scientists associated with the behavioralist school argued that all learning is driven by experience, and that nature plays no role. Classical conditioning, which is based on learning through experience, represents an example of the importance of the environment. But classical conditioning cannot be understood entirely in terms of experience. Nature also plays a part, as our evolutionary history has made us better able to learn some associations than others.

Clinical psychologists make use of classical conditioning to explain the learning of a phobia — a strong and irrational fear of a specific object, activity, or situation . For example, driving a car is a neutral event that would not normally elicit a fear response in most people. But if a person were to experience a panic attack in which he suddenly experienced strong negative emotions while driving, he may learn to associate driving with the panic response. The driving has become the CS that now creates the fear response.

Psychologists have also discovered that people do not develop phobias to just anything. Although people may in some cases develop a driving phobia, they are more likely to develop phobias toward objects (such as snakes, spiders, heights, and open spaces) that have been dangerous to people in the past. In modern life, it is rare for humans to be bitten by spiders or snakes, to fall from trees or buildings, or to be attacked by a predator in an open area. Being injured while riding in a car or being cut by a knife are much more likely. But in our evolutionary past, the potential of being bitten by snakes or spiders, falling out of a tree, or being trapped in an open space were important evolutionary concerns, and therefore humans are still evolutionarily prepared to learn these associations over others (Öhman & Mineka, 2001; LoBue & DeLoache, 2010).

Another evolutionarily important type of conditioning is conditioning related to food. In his important research on food conditioning, John Garcia and his colleagues (Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Koelling, 1955; Garcia, Ervin, & Koelling, 1966) attempted to condition rats by presenting either a taste, a sight, or a sound as a neutral stimulus before the rats were given drugs (the US) that made them nauseous. Garcia discovered that taste conditioning was extremely powerful—the rat learned to avoid the taste associated with illness, even if the illness occurred several hours later. But conditioning the behavioral response of nausea to a sight or a sound was much more difficult. These results contradicted the idea that conditioning occurs entirely as a result of environmental events, such that it would occur equally for any kind of unconditioned stimulus that followed any kind of conditioned stimulus. Rather, Garcia’s research showed that genetics matters—organisms are evolutionarily prepared to learn some associations more easily than others. You can see that the ability to associate smells with illness is an important survival mechanism, allowing the organism to quickly learn to avoid foods that are poisonous.

Classical conditioning has also been used to help explain the experience of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), as in the case of P. K. Philips described in the chapter opener. PTSD is a severe anxiety disorder that can develop after exposure to a fearful event, such as the threat of death (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). PTSD occurs when the individual develops a strong association between the situational factors that surrounded the traumatic event (e.g., military uniforms or the sounds or smells of war) and the US (the fearful trauma itself). As a result of the conditioning, being exposed to, or even thinking about the situation in which the trauma occurred (the CS), becomes sufficient to produce the CR of severe anxiety (Keane, Zimering, & Caddell, 1985).

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) represents a case of classical conditioning to a severe trauma that does not easily become extinct. In this case the original fear response, experienced during combat, has become conditioned to a loud noise. When the person with PTSD hears a loud noise, she experiences a fear response even though she is now far from the site of the original trauma.

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) represents a case of classical conditioning to a severe trauma that does not easily become extinct. In this case the original fear response, experienced during combat, has become conditioned to a loud noise. When the person with PTSD hears a loud noise, she experiences a fear response even though she is now far from the site of the original trauma.

Marc Wathieu – Luigi Coppola – CC BY-NC 2.0.

PTSD develops because the emotions experienced during the event have produced neural activity in the amygdala and created strong conditioned learning. In addition to the strong conditioning that people with PTSD experience, they also show slower extinction in classical conditioning tasks (Milad et al., 2009). In short, people with PTSD have developed very strong associations with the events surrounding the trauma and are also slow to show extinction to the conditioned stimulus.

Key Takeaways

  • In classical conditioning, a person or animal learns to associate a neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, or CS) with a stimulus (the unconditioned stimulus, or US) that naturally produces a behavior (the unconditioned response, or UR). As a result of this association, the previously neutral stimulus comes to elicit the same response (the conditioned response, or CR).
  • Extinction occurs when the CS is repeatedly presented without the US, and the CR eventually disappears, although it may reappear later in a process known as spontaneous recovery.
  • Stimulus generalization occurs when a stimulus that is similar to an already-conditioned stimulus begins to produce the same response as the original stimulus does.
  • Stimulus discrimination occurs when the organism learns to differentiate between the CS and other similar stimuli.
  • In second-order conditioning, a neutral stimulus becomes a CS after being paired with a previously established CS.
  • Some stimuli—response pairs, such as those between smell and food—are more easily conditioned than others because they have been particularly important in our evolutionary past.

Exercises and Critical Thinking

  • A teacher places gold stars on the chalkboard when the students are quiet and attentive. Eventually, the students start becoming quiet and attentive whenever the teacher approaches the chalkboard. Can you explain the students’ behavior in terms of classical conditioning?
  • Recall a time in your life, perhaps when you were a child, when your behaviors were influenced by classical conditioning. Describe in detail the nature of the unconditioned and conditioned stimuli and the response, using the appropriate psychological terms.
  • If posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a type of classical conditioning, how might psychologists use the principles of classical conditioning to treat the disorder?

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5 (3), 121–122.

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science, 122 , 157–158.

Keane, T. M., Zimering, R. T., & Caddell, J. M. (1985). A behavioral formulation of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. The Behavior Therapist, 8 (1), 9–12.

Lewicki, P. (1985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances on subsequent judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 , 563–574.

LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13 (1), 221–228.

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B.,…Rauch, S. L. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66 (12), 1075–82.

Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108 (3), 483–522.

Introduction to Psychology Copyright © 2015 by University of Minnesota is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Ivan Pavlov and the Theory of Classical Conditioning

Ivan Pavlov and the Theory of Classical Conditioning

The components of classical conditioning

We can divide classical conditioning into four main components. These components are the unconditioned and conditioned stimuli and the unconditioned and conditioned response. If we understand the relationships between these components, we’ll be able to understand classical conditioning better.

We’re now going to briefly explain each of these components and the relationship between them:

  • Unconditioned stimulus: This is the stimulus that is already significant and meaningful enough for the subject . By this, we mean that it’s a stimulus that’s capable of provoking a response on its own. In Pavlov’s experiment, the unconditioned stimulus was the food.
  • Unconditioned response: This is the subject’s response in the presence of the unconditioned stimulus. In the case of the aforementioned experiment, the unconditioned response was the salivation when the dogs saw the food.
  • Conditioned stimulus: This is the initially neutral stimulus which doesn’t generate any significant response in the subject on its own. However, through association with the unconditioned stimulus, it’s capable of provoking a new response. In the case of Pavlov’s experiment, this stimulus was the sound of the bell.
  • Conditioned response: This is the response after introducing the conditioned stimulus . In the case of this experiment, it was the dogs salivating when they heard the sound of the bell.

Dog with tongue hanging out.

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

Chapter 8. Learning

8.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

Learning objectives.

  • Describe how Pavlov’s early work in classical conditioning influenced the understanding of learning.
  • Review the concepts of classical conditioning, including unconditioned stimulus (US), conditioned stimulus (CS), unconditioned response (UR), and conditioned response (CR).
  • Explain the roles that extinction, generalization, and discrimination play in conditioned learning.

Pavlov Demonstrates Conditioning in Dogs

In the early part of the 20th century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936), shown in Figure 8.2, was studying the digestive system of dogs when he noticed an interesting behavioural phenomenon: the dogs began to salivate when the lab technicians who normally fed them entered the room, even though the dogs had not yet received any food. Pavlov realized that the dogs were salivating because they knew that they were about to be fed; the dogs had begun to associate the arrival of the technicians with the food that soon followed their appearance in the room.

With his team of researchers, Pavlov began studying this process in more detail. He conducted a series of experiments in which, over a number of trials, dogs were exposed to a sound immediately before receiving food. He systematically controlled the onset of the sound and the timing of the delivery of the food, and recorded the amount of the dogs’ salivation. Initially the dogs salivated only when they saw or smelled the food, but after several pairings of the sound and the food, the dogs began to salivate as soon as they heard the sound. The animals had learned to associate the sound with the food that followed.

Pavlov had identified a fundamental associative learning process called classical conditioning. Classical conditioning refers to learning that occurs when a neutral stimulus (e.g., a tone) becomes associated with a stimulus (e.g., food) that naturally produces a behaviour . After the association is learned, the previously neutral stimulus is sufficient to produce the behaviour.

As you can see in Figure 8.3, “4-Panel Image of Whistle and Dog,” psychologists use specific terms to identify the stimuli and the responses in classical conditioning. The unconditioned stimulus (US)  is something (such as food) that triggers a naturally occurring response , and the unconditioned response (UR)  is the naturally occurring response (such as salivation) that follows the unconditioned stimulus . The conditioned stimulus (CS)  is a neutral stimulus that, after being repeatedly presented prior to the unconditioned stimulus, evokes a similar response as the unconditioned stimulus . In Pavlov’s experiment, the sound of the tone served as the conditioned stimulus that, after learning, produced the conditioned response (CR) , which is the acquired response to the formerly neutral stimulus . Note that the UR and the CR are the same behaviour — in this case salivation — but they are given different names because they are produced by different stimuli (the US and the CS, respectively).

Conditioning is evolutionarily beneficial because it allows organisms to develop expectations that help them prepare for both good and bad events. Imagine, for instance, that an animal first smells a new food, eats it, and then gets sick. If the animal can learn to associate the smell (CS) with the food (US), it will quickly learn that the food creates the negative outcome and will not eat it the next time.

The Persistence and Extinction of Conditioning

After he had demonstrated that learning could occur through association, Pavlov moved on to study the variables that influenced the strength and the persistence of conditioning. In some studies, after the conditioning had taken place, Pavlov presented the sound repeatedly but without presenting the food afterward. Figure 8.4, “Acquisition, Extinction, and Spontaneous Recovery,” shows what happened. As you can see, after the initial acquisition (learning) phase in which the conditioning occurred, when the CS was then presented alone, the behaviour rapidly decreased — the dogs salivated less and less to the sound, and eventually the sound did not elicit salivation at all. Extinction  refers to the reduction in responding that occurs when the conditioned stimulus is presented repeatedly without the unconditioned stimulus .

Although at the end of the first extinction period the CS was no longer producing salivation, the effects of conditioning had not entirely disappeared. Pavlov found that, after a pause, sounding the tone again elicited salivation, although to a lesser extent than before extinction took place. The increase in responding to the CS following a pause after extinction is known as spontaneous recovery . When Pavlov again presented the CS alone, the behaviour again showed extinction until it disappeared again.

Although the behaviour has disappeared, extinction is never complete. If conditioning is again attempted, the animal will learn the new associations much faster than it did the first time.

Pavlov also experimented with presenting new stimuli that were similar, but not identical, to the original conditioned stimulus. For instance, if the dog had been conditioned to being scratched before the food arrived, the stimulus would be changed to being rubbed rather than scratched. He found that the dogs also salivated upon experiencing the similar stimulus, a process known as generalization . Generalization  refers to the tendency to respond to stimuli that resemble the original conditioned stimulus . The ability to generalize has important evolutionary significance. If we eat some red berries and they make us sick, it would be a good idea to think twice before we eat some purple berries. Although the berries are not exactly the same, they nevertheless are similar and may have the same negative properties.

Lewicki (1985) conducted research that demonstrated the influence of stimulus generalization and how quickly and easily it can happen. In his experiment, high school students first had a brief interaction with a female experimenter who had short hair and glasses. The study was set up so that the students had to ask the experimenter a question, and (according to random assignment) the experimenter responded either in a negative way or a neutral way toward the students. Then the students were told to go into a second room in which two experimenters were present and to approach either one of them. However, the researchers arranged it so that one of the two experimenters looked a lot like the original experimenter, while the other one did not (she had longer hair and no glasses). The students were significantly more likely to avoid the experimenter who looked like the earlier experimenter when that experimenter had been negative to them than when she had treated them more neutrally. The participants showed stimulus generalization such that the new, similar-looking experimenter created the same negative response in the participants as had the experimenter in the prior session.

The flip side of generalization is discrimination — the tendency to respond differently to stimuli that are similar but not identical . Pavlov’s dogs quickly learned, for example, to salivate when they heard the specific tone that had preceded food, but not upon hearing similar tones that had never been associated with food. Discrimination is also useful — if we do try the purple berries, and if they do not make us sick, we will be able to make the distinction in the future. And we can learn that although two people in our class, Courtney and Sarah, may look a lot alike, they are nevertheless different people with different personalities.

In some cases, an existing conditioned stimulus can serve as an unconditioned stimulus for a pairing with a new conditioned stimulus — a process known as second-order conditioning . In one of Pavlov’s studies, for instance, he first conditioned the dogs to salivate to a sound and then repeatedly paired a new CS, a black square, with the sound. Eventually he found that the dogs would salivate at the sight of the black square alone, even though it had never been directly associated with the food. Secondary conditioners in everyday life include our attractions to things that stand for or remind us of something else, such as when we feel good on a Friday because it has become associated with the paycheque that we receive on that day, which itself is a conditioned stimulus for the pleasures that the paycheque buys us.

The Role of Nature in Classical Conditioning

As we have seen in Chapter 1, “Introducing Psychology,” scientists associated with the behaviourist school argued that all learning is driven by experience, and that nature plays no role. Classical conditioning, which is based on learning through experience, represents an example of the importance of the environment. But classical conditioning cannot be understood entirely in terms of experience. Nature also plays a part, as our evolutionary history has made us better able to learn some associations than others.

Clinical psychologists make use of classical conditioning to explain the learning of a phobia — a strong and irrational fear of a specific object, activity, or situation . For example, driving a car is a neutral event that would not normally elicit a fear response in most people. But if a person were to experience a panic attack in which he or she suddenly experienced strong negative emotions while driving, that person may learn to associate driving with the panic response. The driving has become the CS that now creates the fear response.

Psychologists have also discovered that people do not develop phobias to just anything. Although people may in some cases develop a driving phobia, they are more likely to develop phobias toward objects (such as snakes and spiders) or places (such as high locations and open spaces) that have been dangerous to people in the past. In modern life, it is rare for humans to be bitten by spiders or snakes, to fall from trees or buildings, or to be attacked by a predator in an open area. Being injured while riding in a car or being cut by a knife are much more likely. But in our evolutionary past, the potential for being bitten by snakes or spiders, falling out of a tree, or being trapped in an open space were important evolutionary concerns, and therefore humans are still evolutionarily prepared to learn these associations over others (Öhman & Mineka, 2001; LoBue & DeLoache, 2010).

Another evolutionarily important type of conditioning is conditioning related to food. In his important research on food conditioning, John Garcia and his colleagues (Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Koelling, 1955; Garcia, Ervin, & Koelling, 1966) attempted to condition rats by presenting either a taste, a sight, or a sound as a neutral stimulus before the rats were given drugs (the US) that made them nauseous. Garcia discovered that taste conditioning was extremely powerful — the rat learned to avoid the taste associated with illness, even if the illness occurred several hours later. But conditioning the behavioural response of nausea to a sight or a sound was much more difficult. These results contradicted the idea that conditioning occurs entirely as a result of environmental events, such that it would occur equally for any kind of unconditioned stimulus that followed any kind of conditioned stimulus. Rather, Garcia’s research showed that genetics matters — organisms are evolutionarily prepared to learn some associations more easily than others. You can see that the ability to associate smells with illness is an important survival mechanism, allowing the organism to quickly learn to avoid foods that are poisonous.

Classical conditioning has also been used to help explain the experience of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) , as in the case of P. K. Philips described in the chapter opener. PTSD is a severe anxiety disorder that can develop after exposure to a fearful event, such as the threat of death (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). PTSD occurs when the individual develops a strong association between the situational factors that surrounded the traumatic event (e.g., military uniforms or the sounds or smells of war) and the US (the fearful trauma itself). As a result of the conditioning, being exposed to or even thinking about the situation in which the trauma occurred (the CS) becomes sufficient to produce the CR of severe anxiety (Keane, Zimering, & Caddell, 1985).

PTSD develops because the emotions experienced during the event have produced neural activity in the amygdala and created strong conditioned learning. In addition to the strong conditioning that people with PTSD experience, they also show slower extinction in classical conditioning tasks (Milad et al., 2009). In short, people with PTSD have developed very strong associations with the events surrounding the trauma and are also slow to show extinction to the conditioned stimulus.

Key Takeaways

  • In classical conditioning, a person or animal learns to associate a neutral stimulus (the conditioned stimulus, or CS) with a stimulus (the unconditioned stimulus, or US) that naturally produces a behaviour (the unconditioned response, or UR). As a result of this association, the previously neutral stimulus comes to elicit the same response (the conditioned response, or CR).
  • Extinction occurs when the CS is repeatedly presented without the US, and the CR eventually disappears, although it may reappear later in a process known as spontaneous recovery.
  • Stimulus generalization occurs when a stimulus that is similar to an already-conditioned stimulus begins to produce the same response as the original stimulus does.
  • Stimulus discrimination occurs when the organism learns to differentiate between the CS and other similar stimuli.
  • In second-order conditioning, a neutral stimulus becomes a CS after being paired with a previously established CS.
  • Some stimuli — response pairs, such as those between smell and food — are more easily conditioned than others because they have been particularly important in our evolutionary past.

Exercises and Critical Thinking

  • A teacher places gold stars on the chalkboard when the students are quiet and attentive. Eventually, the students start becoming quiet and attentive whenever the teacher approaches the chalkboard. Can you explain the students’ behaviour in terms of classical conditioning?
  • Recall a time in your life, perhaps when you were a child, when your behaviours were influenced by classical conditioning. Describe in detail the nature of the unconditioned and conditioned stimuli and the response, using the appropriate psychological terms.
  • If post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a type of classical conditioning, how might psychologists use the principles of classical conditioning to treat the disorder?

American Psychiatric Association. (2000).  Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders  (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement.  Psychonomic Science, 5 (3), 121–122.

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., & Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation.  Science, 122 , 157–158.

Keane, T. M., Zimering, R. T., & Caddell, J. M. (1985). A behavioral formulation of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans.  The Behavior Therapist, 8 (1), 9–12.

Lewicki, P. (1985). Nonconscious biasing effects of single instances on subsequent judgments.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 , 563–574.

LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy.  Developmental Science, 13 (1), 221–228.

Milad, M. R., Pitman, R. K., Ellis, C. B., Gold, A. L., Shin, L. M., Lasko, N. B.,…Rauch, S. L. (2009). Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder.  Biological Psychiatry, 66 (12), 1075–82.

Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.  Psychological Review, 108 (3), 483–522.

Image Attributions

Figure 8.2: Ivan Pavlov (http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Ivan_Pavlov_LIFE.jpg) is in the public domain.

Introduction to Psychology - 1st Canadian Edition Copyright © 2014 by Jennifer Walinga and Charles Stangor is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

pavlov experiment conditioning

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • Games & Quizzes
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center
  • Introduction & Top Questions

Laws of conditioned reflex

  • Opposition to communism

Ivan Pavlov

What did Ivan Pavlov study?

What was ivan pavlov best known for, what were ivan pavlov’s contributions, what was ivan pavlov’s first job.

Aspirin pills.

Ivan Pavlov

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • History Today - Death of Ivan Pavlov
  • Athabasca University - Psychology Learning Resources - Biography of Ivan Pavlov (1849 - 1936)
  • The International Journal of Indian Psychology - Person of the Issue: Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849–1936)
  • The Nobel Prize - Biography of Ivan Pavlov
  • Age of the Sage - Transmitting the Wisdoms of the Ages - Biography of Ivan Pavlov
  • Verywell Mind - Ivan Pavlov and His Discovery of Classical Conditioning
  • Academia - Theory of Ivan Petrovich Pavlov and application in ELT
  • GoodTherapy - Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936)
  • Ivan Pavlov - Student Encyclopedia (Ages 11 and up)
  • Table Of Contents

Ivan Pavlov gave up studying theology to enter the University of St. Petersburg , where he studied chemistry and physiology . After receiving an M.D. at the Imperial Medical Academy in St. Petersburg , he studied in Germany under the direction of the cardiovascular physiologist Carl Ludwig and the gastrointestinal physiologist Rudolf Heidenhain.

Ivan Pavlov developed an experiment testing the concept of the conditioned reflex . He trained a hungry dog to salivate at the sound of a metronome or buzzer, which was previously associated with the sight of food . He later developed an approach that emphasized the importance of conditioning in studies relating human behaviour to the nervous system .

In addition to his conditioning work, Ivan Pavlov devised an operation to prepare a miniature stomach , which was isolated from ingested foods but retained its vagal nerve supply. The procedure allowed him to study the gastrointestinal secretions in animals . For his efforts he received the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in 1904.

Having worked with Carl Ludwig , Ivan Pavlov’s first independent research was on the physiology of the circulatory system . From 1888 to 1890, in St. Petersburg, he investigated cardiac physiology and blood pressure regulation. He became so skillful as a surgeon that he could introduce a catheter into a dog’s femoral artery almost painlessly.

Ivan Pavlov (born September 14 [September 26, New Style], 1849, Ryazan, Russia—died February 27, 1936, Leningrad [now St. Petersburg]) was a Russian physiologist known chiefly for his development of the concept of the conditioned reflex . In a now-classic experiment, he trained a hungry dog to salivate at the sound of a metronome or buzzer, which was previously associated with the sight of food. He developed a similar conceptual approach, emphasizing the importance of conditioning , in his pioneering studies relating human behaviour to the nervous system . He was awarded the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in 1904 for his work on digestive secretions.

Pavlov, the first son of a priest and the grandson of a sexton, spent his youth in Ryazan in central Russia . There, he attended a church school and theological seminary, where his seminary teachers impressed him by their devotion to imparting knowledge. In 1870 he abandoned his theological studies to enter the University of St. Petersburg , where he studied chemistry and physiology. After receiving the M.D. at the Imperial Medical Academy in St. Petersburg (graduating in 1879 and completing his dissertation in 1883), he studied during 1884–86 in Germany under the direction of the cardiovascular physiologist Carl Ludwig (in Leipzig) and the gastrointestinal physiologist Rudolf Heidenhain (in Breslau).

Having worked with Ludwig, Pavlov’s first independent research was on the physiology of the circulatory system . From 1888 to 1890, in the laboratory of Botkin in St. Petersburg, he investigated cardiac physiology and the regulation of blood pressure .

He became so skillful a surgeon that he was able to introduce a catheter into the femoral artery of a dog almost painlessly without anesthesia and to record the influence on blood pressure of various pharmacological and emotional stimuli. By careful dissection of the fine cardiac nerves , he was able to demonstrate the control of the strength of the heartbeat by nerves leaving the cardiac plexus; by stimulating the severed ends of the cervical nerves, he showed the effects of the right and left vagal nerves on the heart.

Michael Faraday (L) English physicist and chemist (electromagnetism) and John Frederic Daniell (R) British chemist and meteorologist who invented the Daniell cell.

Pavlov married a pedagogical student in 1881, a friend of the author Fyodor Dostoyevsky , but he was so impoverished that at first they had to live separately. He attributed much of his eventual success to his wife, a domestic, religious, and literary woman, who devoted her life to his comfort and work. In 1890 he became professor of physiology in the Imperial Medical Academy, where he remained until his resignation in 1924. At the newly founded Institute of Experimental Medicine, he initiated precise surgical procedures for animals, with strict attention to their postoperative care and facilities for the maintenance of their health .

During the years 1890–1900 especially, and to a lesser extent until about 1930, Pavlov studied the secretory activity of digestion . While working with Heidenhain, he had devised an operation to prepare a miniature stomach , or pouch; he isolated the stomach from ingested foods, while preserving its vagal nerve supply. The surgical procedure enabled him to study the gastrointestinal secretions in a normal animal over its life span. This work culminated in his book Lectures on the Work of the Digestive Glands in 1897.

pavlov experiment conditioning

By observing irregularities of secretions in normal unanesthetized animals, Pavlov was led to formulate the laws of the conditioned reflex, a subject that occupied his attention from about 1898 until 1930. He used the salivary secretion as a quantitative measure of the psychical, or subjective, activity of the animal, in order to emphasize the advantage of objective, physiological measures of mental phenomena and higher nervous activity. He sought analogies between the conditional (commonly though incorrectly translated as “conditioned”) reflex and the spinal reflex.

According to the English physiologist Sir Charles Sherrington , the spinal reflex is composed of integrated actions of the nervous system involving such complex components as the excitation and inhibition of many nerves , induction (i.e., the increase or decrease of inhibition brought on by previous excitation), and the irradiation of nerve impulses to many nerve centres. To these components, Pavlov added cortical and subcortical influences, the mosaic action of the brain , the effect of sleep on the spread of inhibition, and the origin of neurotic disturbances principally through a collision, or conflict, between cortical excitation and inhibition.

Beginning about 1930, Pavlov tried to apply his laws to the explanation of human psychoses . He assumed that the excessive inhibition characteristic of a psychotic person was a protective mechanism—shutting out the external world—in that it excluded injurious stimuli that had previously caused extreme excitation. In Russia this idea became the basis for treating psychiatric patients in quiet and nonstimulating external surroundings. During this period Pavlov announced the important principle of the language function in human beings as based on long chains of conditioned reflexes involving words. The function of language involves not only words, he held, but an elaboration of generalizations not possible in animals lower than humans.

pavlov experiment conditioning

Classical Conditioning (Pavlov)

Classical conditioning is a reflexive or automatic type of learning in which a stimulus acquires the capacity to evoke a response that was originally evoked by another stimulus.

Contributors Key Concepts Resources and References

Contributors

  • Ivan Pavlov (1849 – 1936)
  • John B. Watson (1878 – 1958)

Key Concepts

Several types of learning exist. The most basic form is associative learning, i.e., making a new association between events in the environment [1] . There are two forms of associative learning: classical conditioning (made famous by Ivan Pavlov’s experiments with dogs) and operant conditioning.

Pavlov’s Dogs

In the early twentieth century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov did Nobel prize-winning work on digestion [2] . While studying the role of saliva in dogs’ digestive processes, he stumbled upon a phenomenon he labeled “psychic reflexes.” While an accidental discovery, he had the foresight to see the importance of it. Pavlov’s dogs, restrained in an experimental chamber, were presented with meat powder and they had their saliva collected via a surgically implanted tube in their saliva glands. Over time, he noticed that his dogs who begin salivation before the meat powder was even presented, whether it was by the presence of the handler or merely by a clicking noise produced by the device that distributed the meat powder.

Fascinated by this finding, Pavlov paired the meat powder with various stimuli such as the ringing of a bell. After the meat powder and bell (auditory stimulus) were presented together several times, the bell was used alone. Pavlov’s dogs, as predicted, responded by salivating to the sound of the bell (without the food). The bell began as a neutral stimulus (i.e. the bell itself did not produce the dogs’ salivation). However, by pairing the bell with the stimulus that did produce the salivation response, the bell was able to acquire the ability to trigger the salivation response. Pavlov therefore demonstrated how stimulus-response bonds (which some consider as the basic building blocks of learning) are formed. He dedicated much of the rest of his career further exploring this finding.

In technical terms, the meat powder is considered an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and the dog’s salivation is the unconditioned response (UCR). The bell is a neutral stimulus until the dog learns to associate the bell with food. Then the bell becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS) which produces the conditioned response (CR) of salivation after repeated pairings between the bell and food.

Pavlov’s Dogs

John B. Watson: Early Classical Conditioning with Humans

John B. Watson further extended Pavlov’s work and applied it to human beings [3] . In 1921, Watson studied Albert, an 11 month old infant child. The goal of the study was to condition Albert to become afraid of a white rat by pairing the white rat with a very loud, jarring noise (UCS). At first, Albert showed no sign of fear when he was presented with rats, but once the rat was repeatedly paired with the loud noise (UCS), Albert developed a fear of rats. It could be said that the loud noise (UCS) induced fear (UCR). The implications of Watson’s experiment suggested that classical conditioning could cause some phobias in humans.

Additional Resources and References

  • McSweeney and Murphy: The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning. : This brand new book contains an up-to-date, inclusive account of a core field of psychology research, with in-depth coverage of operant and classical conditioning theory, its applications, and current topics including behavioral economics.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1983). Conditioning and associative learning (p. 316). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Pavlov, I. P., & Anrep, G. V. (2003). Conditioned reflexes. Courier Corporation.
  • Watson, J. B. (2013). Behaviorism. Read Books Ltd.

Leave a Reply

You must be logged in to post a comment.

Related Posts

pavlov experiment conditioning

Situated Learning Theory (Lave)

pavlov experiment conditioning

Stereotype Threat (Steele, Aronson)

Albert bandura, erik erikson.

Ivan Pavlov: His Dogs and Conditioning Theory

Lesson introduction.

The lesson on Ivan Pavlov highlights his significant contributions to science, particularly through his experiments with dogs that led to the discovery of conditioned reflexes, a foundational concept in classical conditioning. Born in 1849, Pavlov shifted his focus from religious studies to physiology, ultimately earning a Nobel Prize for his innovative research methods and insights into behavior. His work has profoundly influenced psychology and behaviorism, establishing a legacy that continues to shape our understanding of learning processes today.

Lesson Article

Meet ivan pavlov.

Ivan Pavlov was a famous Russian scientist known for his groundbreaking experiments with dogs. He discovered something called “conditioned reflexes,” which earned him a Nobel Prize in Physiology. However, many people misunderstand his work, so let’s dive into the real story of Ivan Pavlov and his fascinating discoveries.

Early Life and Influences

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov was born on September 26, 1849, in Ryazan, part of the Russian Empire. His father, Petr, was a local Orthodox priest who also grew vegetables and brewed liquor to support the family. As a child, Ivan was often sick and found it hard to concentrate on his studies. Instead of doing homework, he preferred helping his father with farm work.

When Ivan was eight, he had a bad fall and needed time to recover. During this period, his godfather decided Ivan needed strict discipline and took him to a monastery. Surprisingly, Ivan began to enjoy reading there, which helped him focus better. This change made his father hopeful that Ivan could attend a seminary to become a priest.

Another important influence in Ivan’s life was his uncle, who was also a priest. Despite being expelled from his village for bad behavior, Ivan’s uncle shared many family stories with him, forming a strong bond. This family connection helped shape Ivan’s moral values.

Eventually, Ivan was sent to study at a seminary, but he soon realized his true passion was science. He left the seminary to study chemistry and physiology at the University of St. Petersburg, where he became fascinated with the physiology of digestion.

Pavlov’s Experiments

Pavlov’s research methods were quite innovative for his time. He was the first physiologist in the Russian Empire to conduct repeated experiments, allowing him to observe his test subjects over a longer period. He performed surgical operations on animals to study their digestive systems, which, although controversial, provided valuable insights.

His most famous experiments involved dogs and their salivation in response to food. Pavlov noticed that the dogs would start salivating not just when they saw food, but also when they heard the footsteps of the person bringing the food. This observation led him to explore the idea of conditioned reflexes.

Pavlov introduced a neutral stimulus, like a metronome, alongside the food. Over time, the dogs began to salivate just at the sound of the metronome, even without the food. This demonstrated the principles of what we now call classical conditioning.

Pavlov’s Impact and Legacy

Pavlov’s work had a huge impact on psychology, especially in the development of behaviorism and the study of learned behaviors. He believed in the importance of understanding individual behavior and consciousness, even though later psychologists interpreted his work in different ways.

In recognition of his contributions to science, Pavlov received many awards, including the Nobel Prize in Physiology in 1904. His research laid the foundation for future studies in psychology and behaviorism.

Despite the political turmoil in Russia during his lifetime, Pavlov continued his research and maintained a strong reputation. He passed away on February 27, 1936, leaving behind a legacy that still influences psychology and physiology today.

Pavlov’s scientific methods and discoveries about conditioned reflexes have helped us understand human behavior and learning processes, making his work an essential part of psychological studies.

Lesson Vocabulary

Pavlov – A Russian physiologist known for his work in classical conditioning, particularly with dogs. – Pavlov’s experiments with dogs helped scientists understand how conditioning works in psychology.

Dogs – Animals often used in psychological experiments to study behavior and conditioning. – In Pavlov’s famous experiment, dogs were conditioned to salivate at the sound of a bell.

Conditioning – A learning process in which an association is made between a previously neutral stimulus and a stimulus that naturally evokes a response. – Through conditioning, the students learned to associate the sound of the bell with the end of the class.

Reflexes – Automatic, involuntary responses to stimuli. – The doctor tested the patient’s reflexes by tapping his knee with a small hammer.

Behaviorism – A theory of learning that focuses on observable behaviors and the ways they are learned or conditioned. – Behaviorism suggests that all behaviors are acquired through conditioning and interaction with the environment.

Psychology – The scientific study of the mind and behavior. – Psychology helps us understand how people think, feel, and act in different situations.

Experiments – Scientific procedures undertaken to test hypotheses and observe outcomes. – The students conducted experiments to see how different variables affected plant growth.

Learning – The process of acquiring new knowledge or skills through experience, study, or teaching. – Learning about the brain’s functions can help us understand human behavior better.

Science – The systematic study of the structure and behavior of the physical and natural world through observation and experiment. – Science provides us with evidence-based explanations for natural phenomena.

Physiology – The branch of biology that deals with the normal functions of living organisms and their parts. – Understanding human physiology is essential for medical professionals to diagnose and treat illnesses.

Discussion Questions

  • How did Ivan Pavlov’s early life and family influences shape his career path and interests in science?
  • What aspects of Pavlov’s research methods were innovative for his time, and how did they contribute to his discoveries?
  • In what ways did Pavlov’s experiments with dogs change the understanding of behavior and learning?
  • How do you think Pavlov’s background and personal experiences influenced his approach to scientific research?
  • What are some modern applications of Pavlov’s theory of classical conditioning in today’s world?
  • How did Pavlov’s work contribute to the development of behaviorism, and what are some key principles of this psychological approach?
  • Reflect on the ethical considerations of Pavlov’s experiments. How do you think these considerations would be addressed today?
  • What lessons can be learned from Pavlov’s persistence in his research despite the political challenges he faced in Russia?

Lesson Activities

Create a pavlov’s experiment simulation.

Recreate Pavlov’s famous experiment using a simple simulation. Use household items to represent the dog, food, and metronome. Document the process and results, and explain how the experiment demonstrates classical conditioning.

Role-Play a Day in the Life of Ivan Pavlov

Work in groups to role-play a day in the life of Ivan Pavlov during his experiments. Assign roles such as Pavlov, his assistants, and the dogs. Focus on the scientific process and the observations made during the experiments. Present your role-play to the class.

Design a Poster on Classical Conditioning

Create an informative poster that explains the concept of classical conditioning. Include key terms, such as unconditioned stimulus, conditioned stimulus, and conditioned response. Use visuals to illustrate Pavlov’s experiment and its significance in psychology.

Write a Diary Entry from Pavlov’s Perspective

Imagine you are Ivan Pavlov. Write a diary entry describing your thoughts and feelings after a successful day of experiments. Reflect on the challenges and breakthroughs you experienced, and how you believe your work will impact the future of science.

Explore Modern Applications of Pavlov’s Work

Research and present on how Pavlov’s principles of classical conditioning are applied in modern psychology and everyday life. Consider areas such as advertising, education, and therapy. Share your findings with the class in a short presentation.

Share This Lesson:

  • Categories: Biographics , Video Lessons , Grade 9 , Psychology , Science

Click Here to Chat About This Lesson

pavlov experiment conditioning

Zumbi – The Last King Of Palmares

pavlov experiment conditioning

Zoroastrianism | World History

pavlov experiment conditioning

Zora Neale Hurston’s “Their Eyes Were Watching God” – Tanya Boucicaut

pavlov experiment conditioning

Zoo Song Animal Cartoon & Kindergarten Nursery Rhyme by Loco Nuts

pavlov experiment conditioning

Zoo Adventure 🐼🦜🐅 | 15 MINUTE FULL EPISODE | Pandas and Parrots | Preschoolers

pavlov experiment conditioning

‘Zombies Don’t Eat Veggies!’ read by Jaime Camil

View all video lessons, privacy overview.

CookieDurationDescription
cookielawinfo-checkbox-analytics11 monthsThis cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Analytics".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-functional11 monthsThe cookie is set by GDPR cookie consent to record the user consent for the cookies in the category "Functional".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-necessary11 monthsThis cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookies is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Necessary".
cookielawinfo-checkbox-others11 monthsThis cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Other.
cookielawinfo-checkbox-performance11 monthsThis cookie is set by GDPR Cookie Consent plugin. The cookie is used to store the user consent for the cookies in the category "Performance".
viewed_cookie_policy11 monthsThe cookie is set by the GDPR Cookie Consent plugin and is used to store whether or not user has consented to the use of cookies. It does not store any personal data.
  • Learn the Alphabet
  • Video Lessons

AI CONVERSATIONS

  • AI Homework Helper
  • Interview a Historical Figure
  • AI Conversation Practice
  • AI Book Chat
  • AI Country Guide

AI TOOLS FOR STUDENTS

  • AI Dictionary
  • AI Thesaurus
  • AI Sentence Generator
  • AI Grammar Correction
  • AI Paraphraser
  • AI Summarizer
  • AI Lyrics Generator
  • AI Poem Generator
  • AI Ancient Text Translator
  • AI Children's Story Generator
  • Role-Play Game: Fantasy Quest
  • AI Figure of Speech Generators

AI TOOLS FOR TEACHERS

  • AI Rubric Generator
  • AI Essay Grader
  • AI Prompt Optimizer
  • AI Lesson Creator
  • AI Lesson Plan Creator
  • AI Multiple-Choice Quiz Creator
  • AI True-False Quiz Creator
  • AI Fill-in-the-Blanks Quiz Creator
  • AI Book Quiz Creator
  • AI Report Card Comments
  • AI Comments for English Teachers
  • IEP Generator
  • Children's Music

Pavlovian Conditioning: Ivan Pavlov’s Dogs Experiment

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849-1936) lived during a golden age of scientific discovery.

Born into the Russian Empire, and known within his family for being intellectually curious and unusually energetic from a young age, Pavlov won the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1904 for his work on the physiology of digestion, making him the first Russian Nobel laureate. Despite this, Pavlov’s most well-known contribution to science was through his dogs experiments, which became the basis for Pavlovian conditioning (also known as classical conditioning).

In this article, I’m going to look into Pavlov’s dogs experiment, followed by a detailed look at Pavlovian conditioning and Pavlovian response, before moving on to a section on further reading for anyone interested in learning more about this field.

What is Pavlov’s Dogs Experiment?

Ivan Pavlov’s dogs experiment is an experiment that took place in the 1890s in which the Russian physiologist surgically implanted small tubes into the cheeks of dogs to measure the buildup of saliva that took place under a variety of conditions.

Pavlov’s dogs experiment came about as part of an accidental discovery. Pavlov had at the time been conducting research experiments into the dogs’ gastric systems. As part of this research, Pavlov and his assistants would enter the room where the dogs were housed with a variety of edible and non-edible items, with the intention of measuring the amount of saliva that each dog produced when each item was placed in front of them.

Pavlov prediction that the dogs would salivate when presented with edible items was soon proved correct. This represents an unconditioned response in the animals, in which the sight and smell of the food causes them to salivate. Pavlov couldn’t have predicted what happened next.

A Pavlovian Response

While conducting his gastric experiment, Pavlov began to notice something peculiar. He noticed that the dogs would begin salivating not when food was placed in front of them, but when they heard the footsteps of one of Pavlov’s assistants coming down the hall to bring the food to them.

Pavlov soon realized that he could teach his dogs to associate almost any sound, item, or event with the reward of food. To put this another way, it became clear that salivation was a learned response. The most famous item used in Pavlov’s dogs experiment was that of a bell—Pavlov or one of his assistants would ring a bell before feeding his dogs. Soon enough, the single act of ringing the bell would be enough for the dogs to associate this seemingly neutral act with the promise of food.

Pavlovian conditioning was born, and Pavlov’s dogs experiment became his life’s work.

Pavlovian Conditioning

With its genesis in Pavlov’s dogs experiment, Pavlovian conditioning is defined as a form of behavioral psychology (or behaviorism) in which an animal, or human, can be conditioned to respond in a certain way to a stimulus that, had it not been conditioned, should in no way be associated with the act in question.

Let me show you what I mean:

An Unconditioned Stimulus Causes an Unconditioned Response

Prior to Pavlov’s experiment and the discovery of Pavlovian conditioning, it was well-known in the scientific world that an unconditioned stimulus causes an unconditioned response.

An example of this in terms of Pavlov’s dogs experiment would be the food being placed directly in front of the dogs, causing them to salivate. The unconditioned stimulus in this example is the food, and the unconditioned response is the salivation. Pavlov’s dogs’ response (to salivate) was unconditioned because they didn’t need to be trained to respond to the food in this way—it simply happened naturally.

A Neutral Stimulus Causes No Response

In the same way that an unconditioned stimulus causes an unconditioned response, Pavlov confirmed the commonly agreed-upon theory that a neutral stimulus causes no response.

An example of this in terms of Pavlov’s dogs experiment would be the act of Pavlov or one of his assistants ringing a bell before feeding the dogs, before they had taken the time to condition the bell as a stimulus to the food. If they were to ring the bell while it was still a neutral stimulus, no response, conditioned or unconditioned, would have occurred. (Depending on how loud the bell was, the dogs may have been startled the first few times it rang, but this is superfluous to the experiment.)

A Conditioned Stimulus Causes a Conditioned Response

Finally, Pavlov discovered through the course of his experiment that a conditioned stimulus causes a conditioned response.

An example of this in terms of Pavlov’s dogs experiment would be the act of Pavlov or one of his assistants ringing a bell before feeding the dogs, after they have already conditioned the sound of the bell to the promise of food. In this case, the sound of the bell has graduated from being a neutral stimulus to a conditioned stimulus, therefore the dogs’ response (to salivate) became a conditioned response.

Further Reading

In this article I have introduced Pavlov’s dogs experiment and Pavlovian conditioning. The field of classical conditioning and behavioral psychology is vast, and if you found this article interesting I recommend you take a look at some of the following:

  • Behaviorism, Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy
  • A Short History of Behaviorism, Washington State University
  • Behavioral Principles: Classical Conditioning, St. Cloud University

Ivan Pavlov’s dogs experiment and the birth of Pavlovian conditioning was an instrumental scientific discovery at its time that deserves the acclaim and spirited conversation that it entails to this day.

If you’re interested in hearing more from me, be sure to subscribe to my free email newsletter , and if you enjoyed this article, please share it on social media, link to it from your website, or bookmark it so you can come back to it often. ∎

Benjamin Spall

Benjamin Spall

You might also like....

How Diffusion of Responsibility Alters Group Psychology

How Diffusion of Responsibility Alters Group Psychology

The Bystander Effect: The Psychology Behind a Social Phenomenon

The Bystander Effect: The Psychology Behind a Social Phenomenon

Consequentialism Ethics: A Brief Introduction

Consequentialism Ethics: A Brief Introduction

cropped Screenshot 2023 08 20 at 23.18.57

Ivan Pavlov’s Groundbreaking Contributions to Psychology: From Classical Conditioning to Modern Neuroscience

A simple bell, a hungry dog, and a visionary scientist—these elements combined in Ivan Pavlov’s groundbreaking experiments that would forever change our understanding of learning, behavior, and the intricate workings of the mind. Little did Pavlov know that his curiosity about canine digestion would lead to a revolutionary discovery that would ripple through the annals of psychology for generations to come.

Born in 1849 in Ryazan, Russia, Ivan Petrovich Pavlov was a man of many talents and interests. His early life was marked by a passion for learning and a keen interest in the natural world. As a young boy, he would spend hours observing the local flora and fauna, developing an insatiable curiosity that would serve him well in his future scientific endeavors.

Pavlov’s journey into the world of psychology was anything but straightforward. In fact, he initially trained as a physiologist, focusing on the intricate workings of the digestive system. It was during these studies that he stumbled upon the phenomenon that would make him a household name in the field of psychology.

The late 19th and early 20th centuries were a time of great upheaval and progress in the scientific world. As psychology pioneers were beginning to establish their field as a legitimate science, Pavlov’s work would provide a crucial bridge between the physiological and psychological realms. His emphasis on objective measurement and rigorous scientific methods would help lay the groundwork for the behaviorist movement that was to follow.

Classical Conditioning: Pavlov’s Claim to Fame

Imagine, if you will, a laboratory filled with the sounds of tinkling bells and the eager anticipation of hungry dogs. This was the scene of Pavlov’s most famous experiments, which would lead to the discovery of classical conditioning. It all began with a simple observation: Pavlov noticed that his dogs would begin to salivate not only when they were presented with food but also when they saw the lab assistants who typically fed them.

Intrigued by this phenomenon, Pavlov designed a series of experiments to explore it further. He began by ringing a bell just before presenting food to the dogs. After repeated pairings, he found that the dogs would start salivating at the sound of the bell alone, even when no food was present. This was the birth of classical conditioning, a concept that would revolutionize our understanding of learning and behavior.

The key components of classical conditioning are deceptively simple:

1. Unconditioned stimulus (US): A stimulus that naturally triggers a response (in this case, food). 2. Unconditioned response (UR): The natural response to the US (salivation). 3. Conditioned stimulus (CS): A neutral stimulus that becomes associated with the US (the bell). 4. Conditioned response (CR): The learned response to the CS (salivation in response to the bell).

While Pavlov’s experiments might seem quaint by today’s standards, their implications were far-reaching. Classical conditioning helps explain a wide range of human behaviors, from our emotional responses to certain songs or smells to the development of phobias and anxiety disorders. It’s no wonder that Pavlov’s work continues to be celebrated as one of the classics in the history of psychology .

The Physiological Perspective: Pavlov’s Unique Approach

Pavlov’s background in physiology profoundly influenced his approach to psychology. Unlike many of his contemporaries who focused on introspection and subjective experiences, Pavlov was determined to study behavior through objective, measurable means. This emphasis on scientific rigor would become a hallmark of the behaviorist movement that followed.

Central to Pavlov’s approach was the concept of reflexes. He believed that all behavior could be understood as a series of reflexes, both innate and learned. This idea was revolutionary at the time, as it suggested that even complex behaviors could be broken down into simpler, measurable components.

Pavlov’s work on reflexes led him to develop the concept of SR psychology , or stimulus-response psychology. This approach focused on understanding how specific stimuli in the environment could elicit particular responses in an organism. It was a stark departure from the introspective methods favored by many psychologists of the time, and it paved the way for more objective studies of behavior.

One can’t help but wonder how Pavlov’s meticulous approach might have influenced other great minds of his era. Would the father of behaviorism, John B. Watson , have developed his theories without Pavlov’s groundwork? It’s a tantalizing question that highlights the interconnectedness of scientific progress.

Beyond Bells and Dogs: Pavlov’s Other Contributions

While classical conditioning remains Pavlov’s most famous contribution, his work extended far beyond salivating dogs. His research on the physiology of digestion, for which he won the Nobel Prize in 1904, was groundbreaking in its own right. Pavlov’s studies on the nervous system and brain functioning laid the foundation for much of our current understanding of neuroscience.

One of Pavlov’s lesser-known but equally fascinating contributions was his work on “experimental neuroses.” He found that when dogs were subjected to conflicting or confusing stimuli, they would often develop behaviors reminiscent of human anxiety and neurosis. This research opened up new avenues for understanding mental health and laid the groundwork for future studies on stress and anxiety disorders.

Pavlov also developed the concept of “transmarginal inhibition,” which refers to a protective shutdown of the nervous system in response to overwhelming stress. This idea has found applications in understanding phenomena such as combat fatigue and post-traumatic stress disorder.

It’s worth noting that Pavlov’s work wasn’t conducted in isolation. He was part of a broader scientific community that was pushing the boundaries of knowledge in various fields. For instance, his contemporary, Lev Vygotsky , was making significant strides in developmental psychology. While their approaches differed, both men were instrumental in shaping our understanding of human behavior and cognition.

Pavlov’s Enduring Influence: From Behaviorism to Neuroscience

The ripples of Pavlov’s work can still be felt in modern psychology and neuroscience. His influence on behaviorism was profound, providing a scientific foundation for the study of learning and behavior. Pioneers of early behaviorism psychology built upon Pavlov’s work, expanding and refining his theories.

One of the most notable figures to build on Pavlov’s legacy was B.F. Skinner , whose work on operant conditioning complemented and extended Pavlov’s theories. While Pavlov focused on reflexive behaviors, Skinner explored how consequences shape voluntary behaviors. Together, their work provides a comprehensive framework for understanding learning and behavior.

In the realm of therapy, Pavlovian principles have found practical applications in cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT). Techniques such as systematic desensitization, used to treat phobias and anxiety disorders, are directly based on the principles of classical conditioning. By gradually exposing patients to feared stimuli in a controlled environment, therapists can help “recondition” their responses.

Modern neuroscience continues to build on Pavlov’s foundations. Advanced brain imaging techniques now allow us to observe the neural correlates of conditioning in real-time. These studies have revealed the complex interplay between different brain regions during learning and memory formation, providing a deeper understanding of the mechanisms underlying Pavlov’s observations.

A Critical Look: Strengths and Limitations of Pavlov’s Work

While Pavlov’s contributions to psychology are undeniable, it’s important to approach his work with a critical eye. His emphasis on objective measurement and controlled experiments set a high standard for psychological research. However, his focus on animal subjects raises questions about the generalizability of his findings to human behavior.

The ethical considerations surrounding Pavlov’s animal experiments also warrant discussion. While his methods were considered acceptable at the time, they would likely face scrutiny under modern ethical standards. This raises important questions about the balance between scientific progress and ethical considerations in research.

Despite these limitations, Pavlov’s work continues to inspire and inform contemporary psychological research. His emphasis on the relationship between the brain and behavior laid the groundwork for modern neuroscientific approaches. Researchers today continue to explore the neural mechanisms underlying learning and memory, building on the foundations laid by Pavlov over a century ago.

It’s fascinating to consider how Pavlov’s work might have influenced other groundbreaking studies in psychology. Would Stanley Milgram’s famous obedience experiments have been possible without the groundwork laid by Pavlov? How might Konrad Lorenz’s studies on imprinting have been different without Pavlov’s insights into learning and behavior? These connections highlight the interconnected nature of scientific progress and the lasting impact of pioneering work.

The Legacy Lives On: Pavlov’s Enduring Impact

As we reflect on Pavlov’s contributions to psychology, it’s clear that his influence extends far beyond the realm of salivating dogs and ringing bells. His work fundamentally changed our understanding of learning and behavior, providing a scientific framework for studying the mind that continues to shape research today.

From the bell psychology of his famous experiments to the complex neural networks explored by modern neuroscientists, Pavlov’s legacy is evident in countless areas of psychological research. His emphasis on objective measurement and scientific rigor set a standard that continues to guide psychological research today.

As we look to the future, it’s exciting to consider how Pavlov’s work might continue to inspire new discoveries. Perhaps advances in artificial intelligence will allow us to create more sophisticated models of learning based on Pavlovian principles. Or maybe new techniques in neuroscience will provide even deeper insights into the neural mechanisms underlying conditioning.

Whatever the future holds, one thing is certain: Ivan Pavlov’s contributions to psychology will continue to ring out, like a bell echoing through time, inspiring generations of researchers to come. His work reminds us of the power of curiosity, the importance of rigorous scientific methods, and the profound insights that can arise from careful observation of the world around us.

So the next time you find yourself unconsciously salivating at the smell of your favorite food, or feeling a twinge of anxiety at a familiar sound, take a moment to appreciate the complex learning processes at work. In doing so, you’ll be paying homage to a man whose simple experiments with dogs and bells opened up a whole new world of understanding about the intricate workings of the mind.

References:

1. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Oxford University Press.

2. Todes, D. P. (2014). Ivan Pavlov: A Russian Life in Science. Oxford University Press.

3. Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43(3), 151-160.

4. Windholz, G. (1997). Ivan P. Pavlov: An overview of his life and psychological work. American Psychologist, 52(9), 941-946.

5. Fearing, F. (1930). Reflex action: A study in the history of physiological psychology. Williams & Wilkins Co.

6. Boakes, R. A. (1984). From Darwin to behaviourism: Psychology and the minds of animals. Cambridge University Press.

7. Gray, J. A. (1979). Ivan Pavlov. Viking Press.

8. Yerkes, R. M., & Morgulis, S. (1909). The method of Pavlov in animal psychology. The Psychological Bulletin, 6(8), 257-273.

9. Gantt, W. H. (1944). Experimental basis for neurotic behavior: Origin and development of artificially produced disturbances of behavior in dogs. P. B. Hoeber.

10. Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. John Wiley & Sons Inc.

Similar Posts

Permissive Psychology: Definition, Impact, and Applications in Modern Society

Permissive Psychology: Definition, Impact, and Applications in Modern Society

From the nurturing embrace of a permissive parent to the self-guided exploration of a curious child, the far-reaching impact of permissive psychology weaves its way through the tapestry of modern society. It’s a concept that has sparked countless debates, reshaped parenting styles, and influenced educational approaches across the globe. But what exactly is permissive psychology,…

Gustav Fechner’s Contributions to Psychology: Pioneering Psychophysics and Beyond

Gustav Fechner’s Contributions to Psychology: Pioneering Psychophysics and Beyond

A 19th-century polymath’s groundbreaking work laid the foundation for modern psychology, forever changing how we understand the human mind and perceive the world around us. This visionary thinker was none other than Gustav Theodor Fechner, a German physicist, philosopher, and psychologist whose contributions to the field of psychology continue to shape our understanding of human…

Nolen-Hoeksema’s Contributions to Abnormal Psychology: Revolutionizing Mental Health Research

Nolen-Hoeksema’s Contributions to Abnormal Psychology: Revolutionizing Mental Health Research

Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, a trailblazing psychologist, forever changed the landscape of mental health research with her groundbreaking insights into the complex interplay between gender, emotion, and psychopathology. Her work not only revolutionized our understanding of depression and anxiety but also challenged long-held assumptions about mental health disparities between men and women. As we delve into the…

Paul Broca: Pioneering Contributions to Psychology and Neuroscience

Paul Broca: Pioneering Contributions to Psychology and Neuroscience

From a curious boy dissecting animal brains in his mother’s kitchen to a trailblazing scientist whose discoveries revolutionized our understanding of the human mind, Paul Broca’s extraordinary journey left an indelible mark on the fields of psychology and neuroscience. His insatiable curiosity and relentless pursuit of knowledge would lead him to unravel some of the…

Hippocrates’ Psychology: Ancient Foundations of Modern Mental Health

Hippocrates’ Psychology: Ancient Foundations of Modern Mental Health

Over two millennia ago, a revolutionary thinker named Hippocrates forever changed the course of psychology by planting the seeds of a holistic approach to mental health that continues to influence the field today. This ancient Greek physician, often hailed as the “Father of Medicine,” left an indelible mark on the world of healing and human…

Construct Definition in Psychology: Understanding Mental Representations

Construct Definition in Psychology: Understanding Mental Representations

Psychological constructs, the invisible architecture of the mind, shape our understanding of human behavior and cognition, guiding researchers through the complex landscape of mental phenomena. These abstract concepts serve as the building blocks of psychological theory and research, offering a framework for understanding the intricacies of human thought, emotion, and behavior. But what exactly are…

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Introduction to Pavlovian Conditioning: Part 1

An object touches an infant's lips and she begins sucking. A child opens the window of a car and the blast of wind causes him to blink. The cutting of onions produces tears in the eyes of a cook. In all of these examples, a stimulus reliably produces a change in behavior. Technically the stimulus is said to elicit the response, and the relation between the stimulus and response is called a reflex. More specifically, it is an unconditioned reflex because it does not depend upon any prior learning. The stimulus is called an unconditioned stimulus (US) and the response is called an unconditioned response (UR) . For more examples of unconditioned reflexes, see Table 1 . All of these "prewired" stimulus-response connections presumably evolved because of their survival value for the individual.

Pavlovian (or Respondent or Classical) Conditioning builds on the unconditioned reflex. Pavlov (1927/1960) studied unconditioned reflexes with dogs. He found that placing meat powder in a dog's mouth (US) elicits salivation (UR). The conditioning part of his experiment involved pairing various innocuous stimuli with the food, such that they preceded food delivery. The fascinating result was that after a number of these pairings, the mere presentation of the paired stimulus elicited salivation.

For example, the sound of a bell elicits very little to no responding; for this reason it is referred to as neutral stimulus (NS) . In Pavlov's experiment, a bell was paired with food presentation. The result was that the bell came to produce a reliable change in behavior, salivation. This new relation is called a conditioned reflex . It is "conditioned" in the sense that it depends on a prior relation between the bell and the food.

A previously neutral stimulus, the bell, became an eliciting stimulus. Technically, at this point it is referred to as a conditioned stimulus (CS) and the salivation resulting from it is said to be the conditioned response(CR) . Pavlov's experiment is illustrated in Figure 1 .

The bell will remain an eliciting stimulus as long as it continues to be paired with the food. If this pairing stops, the bell will return to its neutral status and no longer reliably produce salivation. This phenomenon is known as respondent extinction .

Pavlovian Conditioning is often involved in emotions. In a frequently cited experiment ( Watson & Rayner, 1920 ), an infant boy named Albert was exposed to the sound of striking a suspended steel bar with a hammer. This sound (US) elicited fear-like responses such as crying (UR). Presentations of a white rat reliably preceded this sound; eventually the white rat itself (CS) was sufficient to elicit crying (CR). Albert was not afraid of white rats prior to the experiment; his fear of white rats, or a white rat phobia, developed during the course of the experiment due Pavlovian Conditioning.

Table 1 . Examples of Unconditioned Reflexes (adapted from Grant & Evans, 1994 p. 379)

Unconditioned Stimulus (US) → Unconditioned Response (UR)
Puff of air to the eye → Eye blink
Light to the eye → Pupil contraction
Food in the mouth → Salivation
Cold temperature → Shivering
High Temperature → Perspiration
Spoiled foods → Sickness, vomiting
Onion vapors → Crying
Tap to the knee (patellar tendon) → Knee jerk
Foreign matter in nose → Sneezing
Foreign matter in throat → Coughing
Pressure to baby's lips → Sucking
Stimulating sex organs → Erection, glandular secretions
Startling noise → Heart rate increase, pupil dilation, tense muscles

Figure 1 . Illustration of Pavlov's conditioning experiment (adapted from Grant & Evans, 1994 ).

Biography of Ivan Pavlov, Father of Classical Conditioning

National Library of Medicine / Public Domain

  • Archaeology

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (September 14, 1849 - February 27, 1936) was a Nobel Prize-winning physiologist best known for his classical conditioning experiments with dogs. In his research, he discovered the conditioned reflex, which shaped the field of behaviorism in psychology.

Fast Facts: Ivan Pavlov

  • Occupation : Physiologist
  • Known For : Research on conditioned reflexes ("Pavlov's Dogs")
  • Born : September 14, 1849, in Ryazan, Russia
  • Died : February 27, 1936, in Leningrad (now St. Petersburg), Russia
  • Parents : Peter Dmitrievich Pavlov and Varvara Ivanovna Uspenskaya
  • Education : M.D., Imperial Medical Academy in St. Petersburg, Russia
  • Key Accomplishments : Nobel Prize for Physiology (1904)
  • Offbeat Fact : A lunar crater on the Moon was named after Pavlov.

Early Years and Education

Pavlov was born on September 14, 1849, in the small village of Ryazan, Russia. His father, Peter Dmitrievich Pavlov, was a priest who hoped that his son would follow in his footsteps and join the church. In Ivan's early years, it seemed that his father's dream would become a reality. Ivan was educated at a church school and a theological seminary. But when he read the works of scientists like Charles Darwin and I. M. Sechenov, Ivan decided to pursue scientific studies instead.

He left the seminary and began studying chemistry and physiology at the University of St. Petersburg. In 1875, he earned an M.D. from the Imperial Medical Academy before going on to study under Rudolf Heidenhain and Carl Ludwig, two renowned physiologists. 

Personal Life and Marriage

Ivan Pavlov married Seraphima Vasilievna Karchevskaya in 1881. Together, they had five children: Wirchik, Vladimir, Victor, Vsevolod, and Vera. In their early years, Pavlov and his wife lived in poverty. During the hard times, they stayed with friends, and at one point, rented a bug-infested attic space.

Pavlov's fortunes changed in 1890 when he took an appointment as the Professor of Pharmacology at the Military Medical Academy. That same year, he became the director of the Department of Physiology at the Institute of Experimental Medicine. With these well-funded academic positions, Pavlov had the opportunity to further pursue the  scientific studies  that interested him.

Research on Digestion

Pavlov's early research focused primarily on the physiology of digestion . He used surgical methods to study various processes of the digestive system. By exposing portions of a dog's intestinal canal during surgery, he was able to gain an understanding of gastric secretions and the role of the body and mind in the digestive process. Pavlov sometimes operated on live animals, which was an acceptable practice back then but would not occur today due to modern ethical standards.

In 1897, Pavlov published his findings in a book called “Lectures on the Work of the Digestive Glands.” His work on the physiology of digestion was also recognized with a Nobel Prize for Physiology in 1904. Some of Pavlov's other honors include an honorary doctorate from Cambridge University, which was awarded in 1912, and the Order of the Legion of Honor, which was given to him in 1915.

Discovery of Conditioned Reflexes

Although Pavlov has many notable accomplishments, he is most well known for defining the concept of conditioned reflexes. 

A conditioned reflex is considered a form of learning that can occur through exposure to stimuli. Pavlov studied this phenomenon in the lab through a series of experiments with dogs. Initially, Pavlov was studying the connection between salivation and feeding. He proved that dogs have an unconditioned response when they are fed — in other words, they are hard-wired to salivate at the prospect of eating.

However, when Pavlov noticed that the mere sight of a person in a lab coat was enough to cause the dogs to salivate, he realized that he had accidentally made an additional scientific discovery. The dogs had learned that a lab coat meant food, and in response, they salivated every time they saw a lab assistant. In other words, the dogs had been conditioned to respond a certain way. From this point on, Pavlov decided to devote himself to the study of conditioning.

Pavlov tested his theories in the lab using a variety of neural stimuli. For example, he used electric shocks, a buzzer that produced specific tones and the ticking of a metronome to make the dogs associate certain noises and stimuli with food. He found that not only could he cause a conditioned response (salivation), he could also break the association if he made these same noises but did not give the dogs food.

Even though he was not a psychologist, Pavlov suspected that his findings could be applied to humans as well. He believed that a conditioned response may be causing certain behaviors in people with psychological problems and that these responses could be unlearned. Other scientists, such as John B. Watson, proved this theory correct when they were able to replicate Pavlov's research with humans. 

Pavlov worked in the lab until his death at the age of 86. He died on February 27, 1936, in Leningrad (now St. Petersburg), Russia after contracting double pneumonia. His death was commemorated with a grand funeral and a monument that was erected in his home country in his honor. His laboratory was also turned into a museum.

Legacy and Impact

Pavlov was a physiologist, but his legacy is primarily recognized in psychology and educational theory. By proving the existence of conditioned and non-conditioned reflexes, Pavlov provided a foundation for the study of behaviorism. Many renowned psychologists, including John B. Watson and  B. F. Skinner , were inspired by his work and built on it to gain a better understanding of behavior and learning.

To this day, nearly every student of psychology studies Pavlov's experiments to gain a better understanding of the scientific method , experimental psychology, conditioning, and behavioral theory. Pavlov's legacy can also be seen in popular culture in books like Aldous Huxley's " Brave New World ", which contained elements of Pavlovian conditioning.

  • Cavendish, Richard. “Death of Ivan Pavlov.” History Today .
  • Gantt, W. Horsley. “ Ivan Petrovich Pavlov. ” Encyclopædia Britannica, Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc., 20 Feb. 2018.
  • McLeod, Saul. “Pavlov's Dogs.” Simply Psychology, 2013 .
  • Tallis, Raymond. “The Life of Ivan Pavlov.” The Wall Street Journal, 14 Nov. 2014 .
  • “Ivan Pavlov - Biographical.” Nobelprize.org .
  • “Ivan Pavlov.” PBS, Public Broadcasting Service .
  • What Is a Conditioned Response?
  • What Is an Unconditioned Response?
  • What Is Classical Conditioning?
  • What Is Behaviorism in Psychology?
  • What Is Operant Conditioning? Definition and Examples
  • What Is the Law of Effect in Psychology?
  • Implicit Bias: What It Means and How It Affects Behavior
  • What Is the Premack Principle? Definition and Examples
  • How Psychology Defines and Explains Deviant Behavior
  • What Is Positive Psychology?
  • Parasocial Relationships: Definition, Examples, and Key Studies
  • What's the Difference Between Eudaimonic and Hedonic Happiness?
  • What Makes the “Uncanny Valley” So Unsettling?
  • What Is Socioemotional Selectivity Theory?
  • 5 Psychology Studies That Will Make You Feel Good About Humanity
  • Cultivation Theory

Ivan Pavlov (Biography + Experiments)

practical psychology logo

When most people think of Ivan Pavlov two thoughts readily come to mind. The first is Pavlov was an amazing psychologist. The second is he worked with dogs. But although Pavlov did some incredible work with dogs and made major contributions to the field of psychology, the truth is he was not a psychologist at all. So, who exactly was he?

Ivan Pavlov

Who Is Ivan Pavlov? 

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov was a Russian physiologist who is best known for discovering the concept of classical conditioning. He was born on September 14, 1849, in Ryazan, Russia. Pavlov won the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in 1904. He died in Leningrad on February 27, 1936.

His Early Years

Ivan Pavlov was the eldest child of Varvara Ivanovna Uspenskaya and Peter Dmitrievich Pavlov. He had ten siblings. Pavlov’s mother was a homemaker and his father was a Russian Orthodox priest at the village church. His grandfather also worked at the church as a sexton.

Pavlov was a good reader by the time he was seven years old. However, he was seriously hurt when he fell from a high wall during his childhood. His injuries caused him to spend most of his early years at home and in his family garden. During this part of his life, Pavlov grew to love nature, gardening, and working with his hands.

As the years passed, Pavlov’s body slowly began to heal. He was eleven years old when he started classes at the Ryazan church school.

What did Ivan Pavlov Study?

After Pavlov completed his classes at the local church school, he enrolled at the seminary in Ryazan. He was immediately impressed by his teachers’ desire to share religious knowledge with him. But despite growing up in a religious household, Pavlov struggled to accept religion. He soon began to wonder if studying theology was right for him.

While at the seminary Pavlov became inspired by the ideas of Dmitry Pisarev—a radical Russian writer and social critic, and Ivan M. Sechenov—a prominent Russian physiologist. Their progressive ideas convinced Pavlov to drop his religious studies at the seminary and devote his life to science. Unsurprisingly, Pavlov's father was furious when he found out. However, Pavlov was determined to live his life the way he wanted.

University of St. Petersburg

In 1870, Pavlov was accepted at the University of St. Petersburg. He enrolled in the physics and mathematics department because he wanted to study natural science. Pavlov spent much of his time studying chemistry and physiology. His first-year chemistry professor was Dmitri Mendeleev, the man who invented the periodic table of elements.

During his first research course in natural science, Pavlov and another student named Afanasyev published a research paper on the physiology of pancreatic nerves. They received much praise and were awarded a gold medal for their work. Overall, Pavlov's grades at the University of St. Petersburg were excellent. He completed his degree in natural science in 1875.

Pavlov's passion for physiology motivated him to continue his studies at the Imperial Academy of Medical Surgery. While there, he worked as an assistant to his former teacher Elias von Cyon—a Russian-French physiologist. However, von Cyon was forced to relocate to Paris when students protested his political views. When von Cyon was replaced by another instructor, Pavlov quit the department.

Pavlov spent two years as an assistant at the physiological department of the Veterinary Institute. During that time he worked on his medical dissertation on the circulatory system. In 1878, Pavlov was offered the position of director of the Physiological Laboratory at Sergey Botkin’s clinic. Botkin was a famous clinician and therapist at the time and was later regarded as one of the pillars of modern medical science in Russia.

Pavlov graduated from the Academy in 1879. At his graduation, he was awarded another gold medal for his outstanding research. He also won a fellowship at the Academy. This fellowship and his role at the Botkin Clinic allowed him to continue his research until he completed his dissertation on The Centrifugal Nerves of the Heart in 1883.

Work with Carl Ludwig

In 1884, Pavlov went abroad to continue his studies. First, he worked under the supervision of Carl Ludwig—a well known cardiovascular physiologist—in Leipzig, Germany. He then went to Breslau, Poland to assist renowned physiologist Rudolf Heidenhain in his study of digestion in dogs. After his studies were complete, Pavlov returned to Russia in 1886.

Pavlov accepted the role of professor of Pharmacology at the Military Medical Academy (formerly called the Imperial Academy of Medical Surgery) in 1890. Less than one year later, he was also invited to serve as the head of the Physiology Department at the Institute of Experimental Medicine. Pavlov was appointed to the Chair of Physiology at the Military Medical Academy in 1895—a role he occupied for 30 years. However, most of his research on the physiology of digestion was conducted at the Institute of Experimental Medicine, where he worked for 45 years.

Pavlov's Dog Experiment

The bulk of Pavlov’s research was conducted from 1891 to the early 1900s. In 1902 he was researching how dogs salivated in response to being fed. To measure the amount of saliva produced, he surgically implanted a small tube into the cheek of each dog. His prediction was that salivation would begin only after the food was placed in front of the dogs.

However, Pavlov soon noticed something quite interesting. At first, the dogs salivated only if they were presented with food. But later in the experiment, the dogs began salivating when they heard Pavlov’s assistant coming with their food. Were the dogs producing more saliva because they could smell the food as it was brought closer? Apparently not, because the dogs still salivated even when Pavlov’s assistant came empty-handed.

Pavlov was fascinated by these results. It did not take him long to figure out that other objects or events would trigger the same salivation response if the dogs associated those objects or events with food. Pavlov immediately realized he had made an important scientific discovery. He spent the rest of his professional life studying this type of learning.

Discovering Pavlovian Conditioning

Pavlovian conditioning (also called classical conditioning ) refers to the process of learning through association. It was first documented by Ivan Pavlov in 1902 when he was researching digestion in dogs. Although he was a brilliant man, Pavlov made this discovery quite by accident. Nevertheless, classical conditioning went on to have a major influence in the field of psychology.

Pavlovian conditioning assumes there are some behaviors that humans and animals do not need to learn. Instead, the response or reflex occurs naturally whenever it is triggered. In Pavlov’s case, the dogs salivated (unconditioned reflex) when they were presented with food (unconditioned stimulus). In this case, the stimulus and reflex are described as “unconditioned” because the reaction is hard-wired into the dogs and required no learning.

However, Pavlov knew that the dogs did learn new things as the experiment went on. He came to this conclusion because initially, the dogs only salivated when they were given food. At the start of the experiment they did not salivate when they heard the footsteps of his assistant. The fact that the dogs later started to salivate when they heard the footsteps shows they had learned to associate Pavlov’s assistant with the food they desired.

Stimuli in Classical Conditioning

Pavlov's lab assistant can be thought of as a “neutral stimulus” at the beginning of the experiment. This is because his presence caused no response from the dogs. As the experiment went on, the dogs linked the lab assistant (neutral stimulus) with food (unconditioned stimulus). After the association was formed, the dogs began salivating whenever they heard the assistant’s footsteps.

If the dogs could learn to associate his assistant with food, Pavlov believed they could learn to associate other things with food. To test if his belief was correct, he decided to use a metronome as his neutral stimulus. A metronome is a device that produces a click or tone at regular intervals.

Under normal circumstances, dogs do not salivate when they hear a tone. But if the tone was successfully linked with food, Pavlov believed the dogs would salivate each time they heard it.

classical conditioning

How Did Pavlov's Dog Experiments Work? 

So Pavlov started to play the tone before he fed his dogs. He repeated the process for days. After some time had passed, he played the tone without presenting any food to the dogs. As he expected, his dogs showed an increase in salivation whenever they heard the tone.

Although the dogs had no response to the tone at the start of the experiment, they had learned a new response by the end of it. And as this response needed to be learned, Pavlov called it a “conditional reflex.” Pavlov also recognized that the tone was no longer a neutral stimulus. By linking it with food (unconditioned stimulus), the tone had become a “conditioned stimulus.”

There are many reports that Pavlov used a bell for the experiments he conducted with his dogs. And he may have used one on occasion. However, Pavlov wanted to control the intensity, quality, and duration of the stimuli. So he relied heavily on a metronome, harmonium, buzzer, and even electric shocks for most of his experiments.

There was one more thing Pavlov discovered during his experiment. He realized that the tone (initially a neutral stimulus) and the food (unconditioned stimulus) needed to be presented close together in time for the link to be made. He referred to this requirement as the law of temporal contiguity. If there is too much time between the playing of the tone and the presentation of the food, the dogs would not learn to salivate when they heard the tone.

Behaviorism Theory

Behaviorism is a theory that suggests human and animal psychology can be understood by studying observable actions. While many forms of psychology emphasize thoughts and feelings, behaviorists believe the “inner world” is not important because it cannot be seen or accurately measured. Behaviorists believe all human behavior is learned by interacting with the environment. Consequently, any person can be trained to become an expert in any task, regardless of his or her personality, culture, or genetic traits.

John B. Watson

Behaviorism was developed by American psychologist John B. Watson in 1913. He was greatly influenced by the work and observations of Ivan Pavlov. Watson believed all facets of human psychology could be explained by Pavlovian conditioning. He denied the existence of the mind, believed all humans begin as a blank slate, and claimed speech, emotional reactions, and other complex behaviors were nothing more than learned responses to environmental stimuli.

two types of conditioning

B.F. Skinner

Another prominent behaviorist who was heavily influenced by Pavlov is Burrhus Frederic Skinner . While Watson expanded on methodological behaviorism, Skinner pioneered a different approach called radical behaviorism. Skinner is widely considered to be the father of operant conditioning—a learning process that is different from classical conditioning. Skinner actually had plans to major in English and become a novelist before he was introduced to Pavlov’s work in 1927.

Although many people think Pavlov did not care about studying things that could not be measured, he never made those claims himself. In fact, he viewed the human mind as a great mystery. If scientists want to understand the human mind, the process has to begin somewhere. Pavlov believed the best approach was to begin with observation and hard data.

Pavlov's Impact on Psychology and Education

Classical conditioning has had a big impact on modern-day learning strategies. Although Pavlov worked with animals, he always believed the principles of classical conditioning can be applied to humans. A number of Pavlov’s basic ideas have been implemented in classrooms and other learning environments. Just as Pavlov used different stimuli to increase or decrease specific behaviors in his dogs, many teachers change their tools, instructions, or environment to influence the behavior of their students and increase learning.

If a teacher is faced with an ongoing problem behavior from a student, the teacher may try to eliminate or change the behavior. One way to do this is by changing something in the learning environment that triggers that specific behavior. So the teacher may move the student to a different seat, change the lights in the classroom, or close an open window if they trigger the bad behavior. The teacher may also try to change her content or modify the way it is presented in order to boost learning.

These strategies are particularly effective for teaching people with behavior problems or learning disabilities. They have been implemented in many schools, homes, and health centers around the world.

Ivan Pavlov's Accomplishments and Awards

Pavlov published many research papers and lectures throughout his long professional career. Some of his more notable works have been compiled into a few books such as The Work of the Digestive Glands (1897), Conditioned Reflexes (1926), and Psychopathology and Psychiatry (1961). His biography, Pavlov: A Biography was written by Boris Babkin and published in 1949. A more recent biography, Ivan Pavlov: A Russian Life in Science, was written by Daniel P. Todes and published in 2014.

Nobel Prize

Pavlov was nominated for the Nobel Prize from 1901 to 1904. However, he did not win the prize for the first three years because his nominations were tied to a variety of findings rather than a specific discovery. When he was first nominated in 1901, he was already well known among physiologists, especially those who studied digestion. However, Pavlov's research on conditioned reflexes was not published until 1902 and it may have taken a while for this work to penetrate the field of psychology.

In 1904, Pavlov was finally awarded the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine. He received the award for his outstanding research on digestion in animals. This research involved removing a dog's esophagus and creating a fistula or tunnel in a dog’s throat so that if the dog ate, the food would not enter its stomach. Pavlov would then collect and test the different secretions along the dog’s digestive system.

Although Pavlov’s methods may seem extreme by today's standards, he always did his best to keep his dogs fed and healthy. He viewed them as very valuable for his work. When his dogs eventually died, he found effective ways to get more. He would take in strays or even pay thieves to steal dogs from other people.

After Pavlov won the Nobel Prize, he drew the attention of many other scientists from around the world. American psychologists, in particular, became more aware of his work and were more willing to test his findings on conditional reflex.

Personal Life and Death

Throughout his life, Pavlov was never easy to get along with. In his childhood days, he often felt uncomfortable around his parents. He was also known to be a volatile and difficult student. When he opened his lab as an adult, his staff knew to avoid him if he was having one of his many bad days.

Ivan Pavlov Children and Wife

Ivan Pavlov met Seraphima Vasilievna Karchevskaya (also known as Sara) in 1878 or 1879. At the time, Sara was a student at the Pedagogical Institute. It did not take long for the young couple to fall in love. They were married on May 1, 1881.

When Sara became pregnant for the first time, she had a miscarriage. The couple was very careful the second time Sara conceived, and she gave birth to a healthy baby boy named Mirchik. However, Mirchik died suddenly in childhood and this made Sara very depressed. Eventually, the couple had four more children. Their names were Vladimir, Victor, Vsevolod, and Vera.

Ivan and Sara Pavlov spent their first nine years as husband and wife in poverty. Due to their financial troubles, they were often forced to live in different homes so they could benefit from the hospitality of other people. Pavlov even grew potatoes and other crops outside his lab to help make ends meet. Once their finances became stable, Ivan and Sara were able to live together in the same house.

Pavlov was eventually able to earn money from health products he made in his lab. He sold the gastric juice he collected from his dogs as an effective treatment for indigestion. Of course, winning the Nobel Prize in 1904 brought monetary rewards. However, the ever-changing political scene in Russia made life difficult for him, his family, and his fellow scientists.

Cause of Death

On February 27, 1936, Ivan Pavlov passed away in Leningrad, Russia. He was 86 years old. He died from lung issues caused by pneumonia. Ever the researcher, Pavlov asked one of his students to sit beside his bed as he died so that the experience could be properly documented.

Related posts:

  • The Little Albert Experiment
  • Classical Conditioning (Memory Guide + Examples)
  • Philip Zimbardo (Biography + Experiments)
  • Albert Bandura (Biography + Experiments)
  • Stimulus Response Theory (Thorndike's Research + Examples)

Reference this article:

About The Author

Photo of author

Famous Psychologists:

Abraham Maslow

Albert Bandura

Albert Ellis

Alfred Adler

Beth Thomas

Carl Rogers

Carol Dweck

Daniel Kahneman

David Dunning

David Mcclelland

Edward Thorndike

Elizabeth Loftus

Erik Erikson

G. Stanley Hall

George Kelly

Gordon Allport

Howard Gardner

Hugo Munsterberg

Ivan Pavlov

Jerome Bruner

John B Watson

John Bowlby

Konrad Lorenz

Lawrence Kohlberg

Leon Festinger

Lev Vygotsky

Martin Seligman

Mary Ainsworth

Philip Zimbardo

Rensis Likert

Robert Cialdini

Robert Hare

Sigmund Freud

Solomon Asch

Stanley Milgram

Ulric Neisser

Urie Bronfenbrenner

Wilhelm Wundt

William Glasser

pavlov experiment conditioning

PracticalPie.com is a participant in the Amazon Associates Program. As an Amazon Associate we earn from qualifying purchases.

Follow Us On:

Youtube Facebook Instagram X/Twitter

Psychology Resources

Developmental

Personality

Relationships

Psychologists

Serial Killers

Psychology Tests

Personality Quiz

Memory Test

Depression test

Type A/B Personality Test

© PracticalPsychology. All rights reserved

Privacy Policy | Terms of Use

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Ivan Pavlov and His Discovery of Classical Conditioning

  • Classical Conditioning
  • Contributions

Select Publications

Ivan Pavlov was a Russian physiologist best known in psychology for his discovery of classical conditioning. During his studies on the digestive systems of dogs, Pavlov noted that the animals salivated naturally upon the presentation of food.

However, he also noted that the animals began to salivate whenever they saw the white lab coat of an experimental assistant. It was through this observation that Pavlov discovered that by associating the presentation of food with the lab assistant, a conditioned response occurred. Pavlov was also able to demonstrate classical conditioning in his subjects by associating food with sound of a tone.

Learn more about Ivan Pavlov and his contributions to the field of psychology.

Pavlov discovered classical conditioning in the 1890s and published his results in 1897. The discovery had a reverberating influence on psychology. Pavlov's discovery had a major influence on other thinkers including John B. Watson and contributed significantly to the development of the school of thought known as behaviorism.

Take a closer look at Ivan Pavlov's life and career in this brief biography.

Ivan Pavlov is best known for:

  • Classical conditioning
  • Research on physiology and digestion
  • 1904 Nobel Prize in Physiology

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov was born on September 14, 1849, in the village of Ryazan, Russia, where his father was the village priest. His earliest studies were focused on theology, but reading Charles Darwin's On the Origin of the Species had a powerful influence on his future interests.

He soon abandoned his religious studies and devoted himself to the study of science. In 1870, he began studying the natural sciences at St. Petersburg University.

Pavlov's primary interests were the study of physiology and natural sciences. He was a founder of the Russian Physiological Society and also served as its first president, a position he held for 19 years.  

"Science demands from a man all his life. If you had two lives that would not be enough for you. Be passionate in your work and in your searching, "  Pavlov once suggested.

So, how did his work in physiology lead to his discovery of classical conditioning?

Ivan Pavlov's Discovery of Classical Conditioning

While researching the digestive function of dogs, he noted his subjects would salivate when they saw the person who was delivering food. In a series of well-known experiments , he presented a variety of stimuli before the presentation of food, eventually finding that, after repeated association, a dog would salivate to the presence of a stimulus other than food.

Pavlov termed this response a conditional reflex . Pavlov also discovered that these reflexes originate in the cerebral cortex of the brain.

Pavlov received considerable acclaim for his work, including a 1901 appointment to the Russian Academy of Sciences and the 1904 Nobel Prize in Physiology. The Soviet government also offered substantial support for Pavlov's work, and the Soviet Union soon became a leading center of physiology research.

He died on February 27, 1936.

Ivan Pavlov's Contributions to Psychology

Many outside of psychology may be surprised to learn that Pavlov was not a psychologist at all. Not only was he not a psychologist; he reportedly was skeptical of the emerging field of psychology altogether.

However, his work had a major influence on the field, particularly on the development of behaviorism . His discovery and research on reflexes influenced the growing behaviorist movement, and his work was often cited in John B. Watson's writings.

Other researchers utilized Pavlov's work in the study of conditioning as a form of learning. His research also demonstrated techniques of studying reactions to the environment in an objective scientific method.

One of Pavlov's earliest publications was his 1897 text The Work of the Digestive Glands , which centered on his physiology research.

Later works that focused on his discovery of classical conditioning include his 1927 book Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex and Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes: Twenty-five Years of Objective Study of the High Nervous Activity (Behavior) of Animals which was published one year later.

A Word From Verywell

Ivan Pavlov may not have set out to change the face of psychology, but his work had a profound and lasting influence on the science of the mind and behavior. His discovery of classical conditioning helped establish the school of thought known as behaviorism.

Thanks to the work of behavioral thinkers such as Watson and Skinner, behaviorism rose to be a dominant force within psychology during the first half of the twentieth century. 

Brown RE, Molnár Z, Filaretova L, Ostrovsky M, Piccolino M, Lorusso L. The 100th anniversary of the Russian Pavlov Physiological Society. Physiology (Bethesda) . 2017;32(6):402-407. doi:10.1152/physiol.00023.2017

Eelen P. Classical conditioning: Classical yet modern . Psychol Belg . 2018;58(1):196-211. doi:10.5334/pb.451

McCabe B. Hopkins researcher discovers everything we know about Pavlov is wrong . Johns Hopkins Magazine . 2014.

Nobel. The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1904: Ivan Pavlov - Biographical

Santana LH. Comparing Watson's behaviorism and Meyer's objectivism: Reassessing traditional assumptions in psychology . 2023.

Pavlov I. The work of the digestive glands . In: Scientific and Medical Knowledge Production, 1796-1918. Routledge. 2023:157-173

Schultz, D. P., & Schultz, S. E (Eds.). (2012). A History of Modern Psychology . Australia Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.

Todes, DP. Ivan Pavlov: A Russian Life in Science . New York: Oxford; 2014.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Pardon Our Interruption

As you were browsing something about your browser made us think you were a bot. There are a few reasons this might happen:

  • You've disabled JavaScript in your web browser.
  • You're a power user moving through this website with super-human speed.
  • You've disabled cookies in your web browser.
  • A third-party browser plugin, such as Ghostery or NoScript, is preventing JavaScript from running. Additional information is available in this support article .

To regain access, please make sure that cookies and JavaScript are enabled before reloading the page.

IMAGES

  1. Premium Vector

    pavlov experiment conditioning

  2. Understanding Psychological Perspectives and the Motives Behind Our

    pavlov experiment conditioning

  3. Conditioning Vector Illustration. Labeled Pavlovian Respondent Learn

    pavlov experiment conditioning

  4. Classical Conditioning

    pavlov experiment conditioning

  5. Classical Conditioning

    pavlov experiment conditioning

  6. Classical Conditioning: How It Works and How It Can Be Applied

    pavlov experiment conditioning

VIDEO

  1. Pavlov's Conditioning: Creating Associations 🔔 #PsychologyBasics #ClassicalConditioning #shorts

  2. PAVLOV CLASSICAL CONDITIONING THEORY

  3. J.B. Watson Ivan Pavlov Classic Conditioning

  4. LECTURE ON CONDITIONED REFLEX By Dr Kavita S Gupta

  5. Classical Conditioning Theory of Learning ( Pavlov ) B.Ed

  6. E2/P3: Ethics: Attitude Formation, Behavioral Change, Milgram Experiment, IEC

COMMENTS

  1. Pavlov's Dogs Experiment & Pavlovian Conditioning Response

    Pavlov's Dogs Experiment and Pavlovian Conditioning Response. Like many great scientific advances, Pavlovian conditioning (aka classical conditioning) was discovered accidentally. Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (1849-1936) was a physiologist, not a psychologist. During the 1890s, Pavlov researched salivation in dogs in response to being fed.

  2. Pavlov's Dog: Pavlov's Theory of Classical Conditioning

    Impact. Pavlov's dog experiments played a critical role in the discovery of one of the most important concepts in psychology: Classical conditioning. While it happened quite by accident, Pavlov's famous experiments had a major impact on our understanding of how learning takes place as well as the development of the school of behavioral psychology.

  3. Classical Conditioning: How It Works With Examples

    Classical Conditioning Examples Pavlov's Dogs. The most famous example of classical conditioning was Ivan Pavlov's experiment with dogs, who salivated in response to a bell tone. Pavlov showed that when a bell was sounded each time the dog was fed, the dog learned to associate the sound with the presentation of the food.

  4. Classical conditioning

    Classical conditioning (also respondent conditioning and Pavlovian conditioning) is a behavioral procedure in which a biologically potent stimulus (e.g. food, a puff of air on the eye, a potential rival) is paired with a neutral stimulus (e.g. the sound of a musical triangle).The term classical conditioning refers to the process of an automatic, conditioned response that is paired with a ...

  5. Pavlov's Dog: The Psychology Experiment That Changed Everything

    October 28, 2023 by Leo. Pavlov's Dog is a well-known experiment in psychology that has been taught for decades. Ivan Pavlov, a Russian physiologist, discovered classical conditioning through his experiments with dogs. He found that dogs could be trained to associate a sound with food, causing them to salivate at the sound alone.

  6. Classical Conditioning: Exploring Pavlov's Famous Experiment

    Classical conditioning taught Pavlov's dogs what to expect after they heard the bell: food. Your dog also learns to positively associate actions like picking up a leash with going for a walk or ...

  7. Pavlov's Dogs and Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov's Dog Experiments. Pavlov came across classical conditioning unintentionally during his research into animals' gastric systems. Whilst measuring the salivation rates of dogs, he found that they would produce saliva when they heard or smelt food in anticipation of feeding. This is a normal reflex response which we would expect to happen ...

  8. Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov (1849-1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning (Figure 1). As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

  9. Classical Conditioning: Examples and How It Works

    In simple terms, classical conditioning involves placing a neutral stimulus before a naturally occurring reflex. One of the best-known examples of classical conditioning is Pavlov's classic experiments with dogs. In these experiments, the neutral signal was the sound of a tone and the naturally occurring reflex was salivating in response to food.

  10. 6.2 Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov (1849-1936), a Russian scientist, performed extensive research on dogs and is best known for his experiments in classical conditioning . As we discussed briefly in the previous section, classical conditioning is a process by which we learn to associate stimuli and, consequently, to anticipate events.

  11. 7.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov Demonstrates Conditioning in Dogs. In the early part of the 20th century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936) was studying the digestive system of dogs when he noticed an interesting behavioral phenomenon: The dogs began to salivate when the lab technicians who normally fed them entered the room, even though the dogs had not yet received any food.

  12. Ivan Pavlov and the Theory of Classical Conditioning

    Ivan Pavlov's experiments with dogs are very well-known in the history of psychology. People built a psychological learning theory from his small accidental discovery. Pavlov's studies have helped us understand associative learning through classical conditioning.. Classical conditioning consists of associating an initially neutral stimulus with a meaningful stimulus.

  13. Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov's Experiment. Classical conditioning was stumbled upon by accident. Pavlov was conducting research on the digestion of dogs when he noticed that the dogs' physical reactions to food subtly changed over time. At first, the dogs would only salivate when the food was placed in front of them. However, later they salivated slightly before ...

  14. 8.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning

    Pavlov Demonstrates Conditioning in Dogs. In the early part of the 20th century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936), shown in Figure 8.2, was studying the digestive system of dogs when he noticed an interesting behavioural phenomenon: the dogs began to salivate when the lab technicians who normally fed them entered the room, even though the dogs had not yet received any food.

  15. Pavlov: Theory, Experiments, & Dog

    Pavlov's experiments lead to his developing the theory of classical conditioning (Pavlov, 1927). This theory states that we can learn to expect certain things to occur one after the other. Oftentimes, one of those things generates a natural response in us, but the other does not necessarily generate any particular response.

  16. Ivan Pavlov

    Ivan Pavlov (born September 14 [September 26, New Style], 1849, Ryazan, Russia—died February 27, 1936, Leningrad [now St. Petersburg]) was a Russian physiologist known chiefly for his development of the concept of the conditioned reflex. In a now-classic experiment, he trained a hungry dog to salivate at the sound of a metronome or buzzer ...

  17. Classical Conditioning (Pavlov)

    There are two forms of associative learning: classical conditioning (made famous by Ivan Pavlov's experiments with dogs) and operant conditioning. Pavlov's Dogs. In the early twentieth century, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov did Nobel prize-winning work on digestion [2]. While studying the role of saliva in dogs' digestive processes, he ...

  18. Ivan Pavlov: His Dogs and Conditioning Theory

    Pavlov - A Russian physiologist known for his work in classical conditioning, particularly with dogs. - Pavlov's experiments with dogs helped scientists understand how conditioning works in psychology. Dogs - Animals often used in psychological experiments to study behavior and conditioning. - In Pavlov's famous experiment, dogs were conditioned to salivate at the sound of a bell.

  19. Pavlovian Conditioning: Ivan Pavlov's Dogs Experiment

    Pavlovian Conditioning. With its genesis in Pavlov's dogs experiment, Pavlovian conditioning is defined as a form of behavioral psychology (or behaviorism) in which an animal, or human, can be conditioned to respond in a certain way to a stimulus that, had it not been conditioned, should in no way be associated with the act in question.

  20. Ivan Pavlov: Pioneering Psychologist's Enduring Legacy

    Classical Conditioning: Pavlov's Claim to Fame. Imagine, if you will, a laboratory filled with the sounds of tinkling bells and the eager anticipation of hungry dogs. This was the scene of Pavlov's most famous experiments, which would lead to the discovery of classical conditioning.

  21. Ivan Pavlov and Pavlovian Conditioning

    Pavlov (1927/1960) studied unconditioned reflexes with dogs. He found that placing meat powder in a dog's mouth (US) elicits salivation (UR). The conditioning part of his experiment involved pairing various innocuous stimuli with the food, such that they preceded food delivery. The fascinating result was that after a number of these pairings ...

  22. Ivan Pavlov: Life, Research, Classical Conditioning

    Ivan Petrovich Pavlov (September 14, 1849 - February 27, 1936) was a Nobel Prize-winning physiologist best known for his classical conditioning experiments with dogs. In his research, he discovered the conditioned reflex, which shaped the field of behaviorism in psychology.

  23. Ivan Pavlov (Biography + Experiments)

    Ivan Petrovich Pavlov was a Russian physiologist who is best known for discovering the concept of classical conditioning. He was born on September 14, 1849, in Ryazan, Russia. Pavlov won the Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine in 1904. He died in Leningrad on February 27, 1936.

  24. Ivan Pavlov's Influence on Psychology

    Ivan Pavlov was a Russian physiologist best known in psychology for his discovery of classical conditioning. During his studies on the digestive systems of dogs, Pavlov noted that the animals salivated naturally upon the presentation of food. However, he also noted that the animals began to salivate whenever they saw the white lab coat of an ...

  25. Comparing Pavlov and Thorndike: Learning Theories Explained

    Pavlov's work was a major influence on the development of behaviorism, particularly Watson's and later Skinner's theories. Thorndike's ideas were more directly influential in educational psychology and were a precursor to Skinner's operant conditioning. Comparison and Contrast of Pavlov's and Thorndike's Contributions While Pavlov and Thorndike made distinct contributions to learning theory ...