Skinner’s Box Experiment (Behaviorism Study)
We receive rewards and punishments for many behaviors. More importantly, once we experience that reward or punishment, we are likely to perform (or not perform) that behavior again in anticipation of the result.
Psychologists in the late 1800s and early 1900s believed that rewards and punishments were crucial to shaping and encouraging voluntary behavior. But they needed a way to test it. And they needed a name for how rewards and punishments shaped voluntary behaviors. Along came Burrhus Frederic Skinner , the creator of Skinner's Box, and the rest is history.
What Is Skinner's Box?
The "Skinner box" is a setup used in animal experiments. An animal is isolated in a box equipped with levers or other devices in this environment. The animal learns that pressing a lever or displaying specific behaviors can lead to rewards or punishments.
This setup was crucial for behavioral psychologist B.F. Skinner developed his theories on operant conditioning. It also aided in understanding the concept of reinforcement schedules.
Here, "schedules" refer to the timing and frequency of rewards or punishments, which play a key role in shaping behavior. Skinner's research showed how different schedules impact how animals learn and respond to stimuli.
Who is B.F. Skinner?
Burrhus Frederic Skinner, also known as B.F. Skinner is considered the “father of Operant Conditioning.” His experiments, conducted in what is known as “Skinner’s box,” are some of the most well-known experiments in psychology. They helped shape the ideas of operant conditioning in behaviorism.
Law of Effect (Thorndike vs. Skinner)
At the time, classical conditioning was the top theory in behaviorism. However, Skinner knew that research showed that voluntary behaviors could be part of the conditioning process. In the late 1800s, a psychologist named Edward Thorndike wrote about “The Law of Effect.” He said, “Responses that produce a satisfying effect in a particular situation become more likely to occur again in that situation, and responses that produce a discomforting effect become less likely to occur again in that situation.”
Thorndike tested out The Law of Effect with a box of his own. The box contained a maze and a lever. He placed a cat inside the box and a fish outside the box. He then recorded how the cats got out of the box and ate the fish.
Thorndike noticed that the cats would explore the maze and eventually found the lever. The level would let them out of the box, leading them to the fish faster. Once discovering this, the cats were more likely to use the lever when they wanted to get fish.
Skinner took this idea and ran with it. We call the box where animal experiments are performed "Skinner's box."
Why Do We Call This Box the "Skinner Box?"
Edward Thorndike used a box to train animals to perform behaviors for rewards. Later, psychologists like Martin Seligman used this apparatus to observe "learned helplessness." So why is this setup called a "Skinner Box?" Skinner not only used Skinner box experiments to show the existence of operant conditioning, but he also showed schedules in which operant conditioning was more or less effective, depending on your goals. And that is why he is called The Father of Operant Conditioning.
How Skinner's Box Worked
Inspired by Thorndike, Skinner created a box to test his theory of Operant Conditioning. (This box is also known as an “operant conditioning chamber.”)
The box was typically very simple. Skinner would place the rats in a Skinner box with neutral stimulants (that produced neither reinforcement nor punishment) and a lever that would dispense food. As the rats started to explore the box, they would stumble upon the level, activate it, and get food. Skinner observed that they were likely to engage in this behavior again, anticipating food. In some boxes, punishments would also be administered. Martin Seligman's learned helplessness experiments are a great example of using punishments to observe or shape an animal's behavior. Skinner usually worked with animals like rats or pigeons. And he took his research beyond what Thorndike did. He looked at how reinforcements and schedules of reinforcement would influence behavior.
About Reinforcements
Reinforcements are the rewards that satisfy your needs. The fish that cats received outside of Thorndike’s box was positive reinforcement. In Skinner box experiments, pigeons or rats also received food. But positive reinforcements can be anything added after a behavior is performed: money, praise, candy, you name it. Operant conditioning certainly becomes more complicated when it comes to human reinforcements.
Positive vs. Negative Reinforcements
Skinner also looked at negative reinforcements. Whereas positive reinforcements are given to subjects, negative reinforcements are rewards in the form of things taken away from subjects. In some experiments in the Skinner box, he would send an electric current through the box that would shock the rats. If the rats pushed the lever, the shocks would stop. The removal of that terrible pain was a negative reinforcement. The rats still sought the reinforcement but were not gaining anything when the shocks ended. Skinner saw that the rats quickly learned to turn off the shocks by pushing the lever.
About Punishments
Skinner's Box also experimented with positive or negative punishments, in which harmful or unsatisfying things were taken away or given due to "bad behavior." For now, let's focus on the schedules of reinforcement.
Schedules of Reinforcement
We know that not every behavior has the same reinforcement every single time. Think about tipping as a rideshare driver or a barista at a coffee shop. You may have a string of customers who tip you generously after conversing with them. At this point, you’re likely to converse with your next customer. But what happens if they don’t tip you after you have a conversation with them? What happens if you stay silent for one ride and get a big tip?
Psychologists like Skinner wanted to know how quickly someone makes a behavior a habit after receiving reinforcement. Aka, how many trips will it take for you to converse with passengers every time? They also wanted to know how fast a subject would stop conversing with passengers if you stopped getting tips. If the rat pulls the lever and doesn't get food, will they stop pulling the lever altogether?
Skinner attempted to answer these questions by looking at different schedules of reinforcement. He would offer positive reinforcements on different schedules, like offering it every time the behavior was performed (continuous reinforcement) or at random (variable ratio reinforcement.) Based on his experiments, he would measure the following:
- Response rate (how quickly the behavior was performed)
- Extinction rate (how quickly the behavior would stop)
He found that there are multiple schedules of reinforcement, and they all yield different results. These schedules explain why your dog may not be responding to the treats you sometimes give him or why gambling can be so addictive. Not all of these schedules are possible, and that's okay, too.
Continuous Reinforcement
If you reinforce a behavior repeatedly, the response rate is medium, and the extinction rate is fast. The behavior will be performed only when reinforcement is needed. As soon as you stop reinforcing a behavior on this schedule, the behavior will not be performed.
Fixed-Ratio Reinforcement
Let’s say you reinforce the behavior every fourth or fifth time. The response rate is fast, and the extinction rate is medium. The behavior will be performed quickly to reach the reinforcement.
Fixed-Interval Reinforcement
In the above cases, the reinforcement was given immediately after the behavior was performed. But what if the reinforcement was given at a fixed interval, provided that the behavior was performed at some point? Skinner found that the response rate is medium, and the extinction rate is medium.
Variable-Ratio Reinforcement
Here's how gambling becomes so unpredictable and addictive. In gambling, you experience occasional wins, but you often face losses. This uncertainty keeps you hooked, not knowing when the next big win, or dopamine hit, will come. The behavior gets reinforced randomly. When gambling, your response is quick, but it takes a long time to stop wanting to gamble. This randomness is a key reason why gambling is highly addictive.
Variable-Interval Reinforcement
Last, the reinforcement is given out at random intervals, provided that the behavior is performed. Health inspectors or secret shoppers are commonly used examples of variable-interval reinforcement. The reinforcement could be administered five minutes after the behavior is performed or seven hours after the behavior is performed. Skinner found that the response rate for this schedule is fast, and the extinction rate is slow.
Skinner's Box and Pigeon Pilots in World War II
Yes, you read that right. Skinner's work with pigeons and other animals in Skinner's box had real-life effects. After some time training pigeons in his boxes, B.F. Skinner got an idea. Pigeons were easy to train. They can see very well as they fly through the sky. They're also quite calm creatures and don't panic in intense situations. Their skills could be applied to the war that was raging on around him.
B.F. Skinner decided to create a missile that pigeons would operate. That's right. The U.S. military was having trouble accurately targeting missiles, and B.F. Skinner believed pigeons could help. He believed he could train the pigeons to recognize a target and peck when they saw it. As the pigeons pecked, Skinner's specially designed cockpit would navigate appropriately. Pigeons could be pilots in World War II missions, fighting Nazi Germany.
When Skinner proposed this idea to the military, he was met with skepticism. Yet, he received $25,000 to start his work on "Project Pigeon." The device worked! Operant conditioning trained pigeons to navigate missiles appropriately and hit their targets. Unfortunately, there was one problem. The mission killed the pigeons once the missiles were dropped. It would require a lot of pigeons! The military eventually passed on the project, but cockpit prototypes are on display at the American History Museum. Pretty cool, huh?
Examples of Operant Conditioning in Everyday Life
Not every example of operant conditioning has to end in dropping missiles. Nor does it have to happen in a box in a laboratory! You might find that you have used operant conditioning on yourself, a pet, or a child whose behavior changes with rewards and punishments. These operant conditioning examples will look into what this process can do for behavior and personality.
Hot Stove: If you put your hand on a hot stove, you will get burned. More importantly, you are very unlikely to put your hand on that hot stove again. Even though no one has made that stove hot as a punishment, the process still works.
Tips: If you converse with a passenger while driving for Uber, you might get an extra tip at the end of your ride. That's certainly a great reward! You will likely keep conversing with passengers as you drive for Uber. The same type of behavior applies to any service worker who gets tips!
Training a Dog: If your dog sits when you say “sit,” you might treat him. More importantly, they are likely to sit when you say, “sit.” (This is a form of variable-ratio reinforcement. Likely, you only treat your dog 50-90% of the time they sit. If you gave a dog a treat every time they sat, they probably wouldn't have room for breakfast or dinner!)
Operant Conditioning Is Everywhere!
We see operant conditioning training us everywhere, intentionally or unintentionally! Game makers and app developers design their products based on the "rewards" our brains feel when seeing notifications or checking into the app. Schoolteachers use rewards to control their unruly classes. Dog training doesn't always look different from training your child to do chores. We know why this happens, thanks to experiments like the ones performed in Skinner's box.
Related posts:
- Operant Conditioning (Examples + Research)
- Edward Thorndike (Psychologist Biography)
- Schedules of Reinforcement (Examples)
- B.F. Skinner (Psychologist Biography)
- Fixed Ratio Reinforcement Schedule (Examples)
Reference this article:
About The Author
Free Personality Test
Free Memory Test
Free IQ Test
PracticalPie.com is a participant in the Amazon Associates Program. As an Amazon Associate we earn from qualifying purchases.
Follow Us On:
Youtube Facebook Instagram X/Twitter
Psychology Resources
Developmental
Personality
Relationships
Psychologists
Serial Killers
Psychology Tests
Personality Quiz
Memory Test
Depression test
Type A/B Personality Test
© PracticalPsychology. All rights reserved
Privacy Policy | Terms of Use
Learned Helplessness in Rodents
Series: Neuromethods > Book: Psychiatric Vulnerability, Mood, and Anxiety Disorders
Protocol | DOI: 10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_9
- Department of Clinical Medicine, Translational Neuropsychiatry Unit, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
- School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirao Preto, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
- Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus, Denmark
Learned helplessness (LH) is a well-known phenomenon described in animals upon their exposure to uncontrollable and unpredictable aversive stimuli, usually inescapable shocks. It is often characterized by impairment in learning to escape or avoid
Learned helplessness (LH) is a well-known phenomenon described in animals upon their exposure to uncontrollable and unpredictable aversive stimuli, usually inescapable shocks. It is often characterized by impairment in learning to escape or avoid controllable aversive stimuli in a subsequent behavioral test. In addition to that, previously stressed animals present a range of other behavioral and physiological changes, including cognitive deficits, decreased reward sensitivity (anhedonia), hypolocomotion, sleep and appetite changes, among others. Disturbances in different neurotransmitter systems in cortical and limbic brain regions as well as impaired neuroplasticity have been associated with the development of LH following exposure to an uncontrollable stressor. Since exposure to uncontrollable stress is a risk factor for developing psychiatric disorders and feelings of helplessness are part of the symptomatology in patients suffering from depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), the LH paradigm has been used in animals to study the pathophysiology of such conditions and new treatment options. This chapter summarizes the most relevant discussions about the effects induced by inescapable shock that can be associated with LH development and depression neurobiology. Moreover, we present herein a protocol that can be used to test the effect of different pharmacological interventions and how this can be relevant to studying depression neurobiology.
Figures ( 0 ) & Videos ( 0 )
Experimental specifications, other keywords.
- Selye H (1950) Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. Br Med J 1:1383–1392
- Selye H (1936) A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 138:32–32
- McEwen BS (2008) Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 583:174–185
- McEwen BS (2006) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: central role of the brain. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8:367–381
- McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ (2010) Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1186:190–222
- Ehlert U (2013) Enduring psychobiological effects of childhood adversity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1850–1857
- Söderlund J, Lindskog M (2018) Relevance of rodent models of depression in clinical practice: can we overcome the obstacles in translational neuropsychiatry? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:668–676
- Anderzhanova E, Kirmeier T, Wotjak CT (2017) Animal models in psychiatric research: the RDoC system as a new framework for endophenotype-oriented translational neuroscience. Neurobiol Stress 7:47–56
- (2013) Diagnostics, MDPI AG
- McArthur R, Borsini F (2006) Animal models of depression in drug discovery: a historical perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 84:436–452
- Willner P (1986) Validation criteria for animal models of human mental disorders: learned helplessness as a paradigm case. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 10:677–690
- Gururajan A, Reif A, Cryan JF et al (2019) The future of rodent models in depression research. Nat Rev Neurosci 20:686–701
- Belzung C, Lemoine M (2011) Criteria of validity for animal models of psychiatric disorders: focus on anxiety disorders and depression. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 1:9
- Seligman ME, Maier SF (1967) Failure to escape traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol 74:1–9
- Overmier JB, Bruce Overmier J, Seligman ME (1967) Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024166
- Maier SF, Seligman ME (1976) Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol Gen 105:3
- Overmier JB (1968) Interference with avoidance behavior: failure to avoid traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol 78:340–343
- Maier SF, Seligman MEP (2016) Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychol Rev 123:349–367
- Maier SF (1970) Failure to escape traumatic electric shock: incompatible skeletal-motor responses or learned helplessness? Learn Motiv 1:157–169
- Weiss JM, Glazer HI (1975) Effects of acute exposure to stressors on subsequent avoidance-escape behavior. Psychosom Med 37:499–521
- Glazer HI, Weiss JM (1976) Long-term interference effect: an alternative to learned helplessness. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2:202
- Jackson RL, Alexander JH, Maier SF (1980) Learned helplessness, inactivity, and associative deficits: effects of inescapable shock on response choice escape learning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 6:1–20
- Minor TR, Jackson RL, Maier SF (1984) Effects of task-irrelevant cues and reinforcement delay on choice-escape learning following inescapable shock: evidence for a deficit in selective attention. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 10:543–556
- Rosellini RA, DeCola JP, Shapiro NR (1982) Cross-motivational effects of inescapable shock are associative in nature. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 8:376–388
- Maier SF, Minor TR (1993) Dissociation of interference with the speed and accuracy of escape produced by inescapable shock. Behav Neurosci 107:139–146
- Lee RK, Maier SF (1988) Inescapable shock and attention to internal versus external cues in a water discrimination escape task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 14:302–310
- Minor TR, Trauner MA, Lee CY et al (1990) Modeling signal features of escape response: effects of cessation conditioning in “learned helplessness” paradigm. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 16:123–136
- Amat J, Paul E, Zarza C et al (2006) Previous experience with behavioral control over stress blocks the behavioral and dorsal raphe nucleus activating effects of later uncontrollable stress: role of the ventral medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 26:13264–13272
- Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV et al (2006) Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8:397–406
- Abrahamsen GC, Stock HS, Caldarone BJ et al (1993) Learned helplessness inducing foot shock can exacerbate morphine responsiveness. Physiol Behav 54:289–294
- Sutton LC, Lea SE, Will MJ et al (1997) Inescapable shock-induced potentiation of morphine analgesia. Behav Neurosci 111:1105–1113
- Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Sigrist H et al (2012) Establishing a learned-helplessness effect paradigm in C57BL/6 mice: behavioural evidence for emotional, motivational and cognitive effects of aversive uncontrollability per se. Neuropharmacology 62:358–372
- Caldarone BJ, George TP, Zachariou V et al (2000) Gender differences in learned helplessness behavior are influenced by genetic background. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66:811–817
- Teixeira NA, Pereira DG, Hermini AH (1997) Effects of naltrexone and cross-tolerance to morphine in a learned helplessness paradigm. Braz J Med Biol Res 30:775–782
- Rojas-Corrales MO, Berrocoso E, Gibert-Rahola J et al (2002) Antidepressant-like effects of tramadol and other central analgesics with activity on monoamines reuptake, in helpless rats. Life Sci 72:143–152
- Vollmayr B, Gass P (2013) Learned helplessness: unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell Tissue Res 354:171–178
- Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Spinelli S et al (2011) Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharmacol Ther 132:242–267
- Maier SF (2001) Exposure to the stressor environment prevents the temporal dissipation of behavioral depression/learned helplessness. Biol Psychiatry 49:763–773
- Desan PH, Silbert LH, Maier SF (1988) Long-term effects of inescapable stress on daily running activity and antagonism by desipramine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 30:21–29
- Musty RE, Jordan MP, Lenox RH (1990) Criterion for learned helplessness in the rat: a redefinition. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 36:739–744
- Seligman ME, Beagley G (1975) Learned helplessness in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 88:534–541
- Saylor CF, Finch AJ Jr, Cassel SC et al (1984) Learned helplessness: effects of noncontingent reinforcement and response cost with emotionally disturbed children. J Psychol 117:189–196
- Abramson LY, Seligman ME, Teasdale JD (1978) Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol 87:49–74
- Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Sanchis-Segura C et al (2005) Learned helplessness: validity and reliability of depressive-like states in mice. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 16:70–78
- Vollmayr B, Henn FA (2001) Learned helplessness in the rat: improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 8:1–7
- Batsching S, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (2016) Inescapable stress changes walking behavior in flies – learned helplessness revisited. PLoS One 11:e0167066
- Demin KA, Lakstygal AM, Chernysh MV et al (2020) The zebrafish tail immobilization (ZTI) test as a new tool to assess stress-related behavior and a potential screen for drugs affecting despair-like states. J Neurosci Methods 337:108637
- Baumgarten HG, Göthert M (2012) Serotoninergic neurons and 5-HT receptors in the CNS. Springer
- Graeff FG, Guimarães FS, De Andrade TG et al (1996) Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 54:129–141
- Maier SF, Watkins LR (2005) Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:829–841
- Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E et al (2005) Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci 8:365–371
- Koike H, Iijima M, Chaki S (2011) Involvement of AMPA receptor in both the rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects of ketamine in animal models of depression. Behav Brain Res 224:107–111
- Marques DB, Ruggiero RN, Bueno-Junior LS et al (2021) Prediction of learned resistance or helplessness by hippocampal-prefrontal cortical network activity during stress. J Neurosci
- Hajszan T, Dow A, Warner-Schmidt JL et al (2009) Remodeling of hippocampal spine synapses in the rat learned helplessness model of depression. Biol Psychiatry 65:392–400
- Ryan BK, Vollmayr B, Klyubin I et al (2010) Persistent inhibition of hippocampal long-term potentiation in vivo by learned helplessness stress. Hippocampus 20:758–767
- Shors TJ (2004) Learning during stressful times. Learn Mem 11:137–144
- Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK et al (2003) Central mechanisms of stress integration: hierarchical circuitry controlling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroendocrinol 24:151–180
- Huzian O, Baka J, Csakvari E et al (2021) Stress resilience is associated with hippocampal synaptoprotection in the female rat learned helplessness paradigm. Neuroscience 459:85–103
- Aznar S, Klein AB, Santini MA et al (2010) Aging and depression vulnerability interaction results in decreased serotonin innervation associated with reduced BDNF levels in hippocampus of rats bred for learned helplessness. Synapse 64:561–565
- Bland ST, Tamlyn JP, Barrientos RM et al (2007) Expression of fibroblast growth factor-2 and brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus after uncontrollable or controllable stress. Neuroscience 144:1219–1228
- Schulte-Herbrüggen O, Hellweg R, Chourbaji S et al (2007) Differential regulation of neurotrophins and serotonergic function in mice with genetically reduced glucocorticoid receptor expression. Exp Neurol 204:307–316
- Vollmayr B, Faust H, Lewicka S et al (2001) Brain-derived-neurotrophic-factor (BDNF) stress response in rats bred for learned helplessness. Mol Psychiatry 6(471–4):358
- Cheng Y, Pardo M, Armini R de S et al (2016) Stress-induced neuroinflammation is mediated by GSK3-dependent TLR4 signaling that promotes susceptibility to depression-like behavior. Brain Behav Immun 53:207–222
- Kubera M, Obuchowicz E, Goehler L et al (2011) In animal models, psychosocial stress-induced (neuro)inflammation, apoptosis and reduced neurogenesis are associated to the onset of depression. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35:744–759
- Woodburn SC, Bollinger JL, Wohleb ES (2021) The semantics of microglia activation: neuroinflammation, homeostasis, and stress. J Neuroinflammation 18:258
- McKinney WT Jr, Bunney WE Jr (1969) Animal model of depression. I. Review of evidence: implications for research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 21:240–248
- Willner P (1984) The validity of animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology 83:1–16
- Murua VS, Gomez RA, Andrea ME et al (1991) Shuttle-box deficits induced by chronic variable stress: reversal by imipramine administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 38:125–130
- Tennen H, Gillen R, Drum PE (1982) The debilitating effect of exposure to noncontingent escape: a test of the learned helplessness model. J Pers 50:409–425
- Lester D (1998) Helplessness, hopelessness, and haplessness and suicidality. Psychol Rep 82:946
- Leenaars L, Lester D (2007) Construct validity of the helplessness/hopelessness/haplessness scale: correlations with perfectionism and depression
- Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA (2006) Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in men. Am J Psychiatry 163:115–124
- Ho TC, King LS (2021) Mechanisms of neuroplasticity linking early adversity to depression: developmental considerations. Transl Psychiatry 11:517
- Zacharko RM, Bowers WJ, Kokkinidis L et al (1983) Region-specific reductions of intracranial self-stimulation after uncontrollable stress: possible effects on reward processes. Behav Brain Res 9:129–141
- Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R, Henn FA et al (2005) Reduced sensitivity to sucrose in rats bred for helplessness: a study using the matching law. Behav Pharmacol 16:267–270
- Park S-C, Kim Y-K (2018) Depression in DSM-5: changes, controversies, and future directions. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6577-4_1
- Seligman MEP, Weiss JM, Weinraub M et al (1980) Part I: introduction. Behav Res Ther 18:459–461
- Adrien J, Dugovic C, Martin P (1991) Sleep-wakefulness patterns in the helpless rat. Physiol Behav 49:257–262
- Haracz JL, Minor TR, Wilkins JN et al (1988) Learned helplessness: an experimental model of the DST in rats. Biol Psychiatry 23:388–396
- Maier SF, Nguyen KT, Deak T et al (1999) Stress, learned helplessness, and brain interleukin-1 beta. Adv Exp Med Biol 461:235–249
- Chen L, Wang X, Zhang Y et al (2021) Daidzein alleviates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal Axis hyperactivity, ameliorates depression-like behavior, and partly rectifies circulating cytokine imbalance in two rodent models of depression. Front Behav Neurosci 15:671864
- Sherman AD, Sacquitne JL, Petty F (1982) Specificity of the learned helplessness model of depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16:449–454
- Highland JN, Morris PJ, Zanos P et al (2019) Mouse, rat, and dog bioavailability and mouse oral antidepressant efficacy of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine. J Psychopharmacol 33:12–24
- Sartorius A, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2009) Deep brain stimulation of the lateral habenula to treat depression. Front Neurosci 3:272
- Azis IA, Hashioka S, Tsuchie K et al (2019) Electroconvulsive shock restores the decreased coverage of brain blood vessels by astrocytic endfeet and ameliorates depressive-like behavior. J Affect Disord 257:331–339
- Nijssen A, Schelvis PR (1987) Effect of an anti-anxiety drug in a learned helplessness experiment. Neuropsychobiology 18:195–198
- Morais M, Patrício P, Mateus-Pinheiro A et al (2017) The modulation of adult neuroplasticity is involved in the mood-improving actions of atypical antipsychotics in an animal model of depression. Transl Psychiatry 7:e1146
- Paula Soares V de, Vicente MA, Biojone C, et al (2011) Distinct behavioral consequences of stress models of depression in the elevated T-maze. Behav Brain Res 225:590–595
- Wieland S, Boren JL, Consroe PF et al (1986) Stock differences in the susceptibility of rats to learned helplessness training. Life Sci 39:937–944
- Takase LF, Nogueira MI, Bland ST et al (2005) Effect of number of tailshocks on learned helplessness and activation of serotonergic and noradrenergic neurons in the rat. Behav Brain Res 162:299–306
- Joca SRL, Padovan CM, Guimarães FS (2003) Activation of post-synaptic 5-HT(1A) receptors in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development. Brain Res 978:177–184
- Joca SRL, Zanelati T, Guimarães FS (2006) Post-stress facilitation of serotonergic, but not noradrenergic, neurotransmission in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development in rats. Brain Res 1087:67–74
- Stanquini LA, Biojone C, Guimarães FS et al (2018) Repeated treatment with nitric oxide synthase inhibitor attenuates learned helplessness development in rats and increases hippocampal BDNF expression. Acta Neuropsychiatr 30:127–136
- Sales AJ, Maciel IS, Suavinha ACDR et al (2021) Modulation of DNA methylation and gene expression in rodent cortical neuroplasticity pathways exerts rapid antidepressant-like effects. Mol Neurobiol 58:777–794
- Sousa Maciel I de, Sales AJ, Casarotto PC, et al (2020) Nitric Oxide Synthase inhibition counteracts the stress-induced DNA methyltransferase 3b expression in the hippocampus of rats. Eur J Neurosci
- Geoffroy M, Christensen AV (1993) Psychomotor stimulants versus antidepressants in the learned helplessness model of depression. Drug Dev Res 29:48–55
- Sales AJ, Fogaça MV, Sartim AG et al (2019) Cannabidiol induces rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects through increased BDNF signaling and synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex. Mol Neurobiol 56:1070–1081
- Sales AJ, Joca SRL (2018) Antidepressant administration modulates stress-induced DNA methylation and DNA methyltransferase expression in rat prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Behav Brain Res 343:8–15
- Martin P, Soubrie P, Simon P (1987) The effect of monoamine oxidase inhibitors compared with classical tricyclic antidepressants on learned helplessness paradigm. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 11:1–7
- Petty F, Sacquitne JL, Sherman AD (1982) Tricyclic antidepressant drug action correlates with its tissue levels in anterior neocortex. Neuropharmacology 21:475–477
- Ramaker MJ, Dulawa SC (2017) Identifying fast-onset antidepressants using rodent models. Mol Psychiatry 22:656–665
- Shirayama Y, Hashimoto K (2018) Lack of antidepressant effects of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine in a rat learned helplessness model: comparison with (R)-ketamine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:84–88
- Antila H, Ryazantseva M, Popova D et al (2017) Isoflurane produces antidepressant effects and induces TrkB signaling in rodents. Sci Rep 7:7811
- Hamani C, Diwan M, Macedo CE et al (2010) Antidepressant-like effects of medial prefrontal cortex deep brain stimulation in rats. Biol Psychiatry 67:117–124
- Creed MC, Hamani C, Nobrega JN (2013) Effects of repeated deep brain stimulation on depressive- and anxiety-like behavior in rats: comparing entopeduncular and subthalamic nuclei. Brain Stimul 6:506–514
- Han S, Yang SH, Kim JY et al (2017) Down-regulation of cholinergic signaling in the habenula induces anhedonia-like behavior. Sci Rep 7:900
- Urbanavicius J, Fabius S, Roncalho A et al (2019) Melanin-concentrating hormone in the Locus Coeruleus aggravates helpless behavior in stressed rats. Behav Brain Res 374:112120
- Nobrega JN, Hedayatmofidi PS, Lobo DS (2016) Strong interactions between learned helplessness and risky decision-making in a rat gambling model. Sci Rep 6:37304
- Dalla C, Edgecomb C, Whetstone AS et al (2008) Females do not express learned helplessness like males do. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:1559–1569
- Ribeiro DE, Casarotto PC, Staquini L et al (2019) Reduced P2X receptor levels are associated with antidepressant effect in the learned helplessness model. PeerJ 7:e7834
- Drugan RC, Basile AS, Ha JH et al (1997) Analysis of the importance of controllable versus uncontrollable stress on subsequent behavioral and physiological functioning. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 2:69–74
- Landgraf D, Long J, Der-Avakian A et al (2015) Dissociation of learned helplessness and fear conditioning in mice: a mouse model of depression. PLoS One 10:e0125892
- Baratta MV, Christianson JP, Gomez DM et al (2007) Controllable versus uncontrollable stressors bi-directionally modulate conditioned but not innate fear. Neuroscience 146:1495–1503
- Shumake J, Barrett D, Gonzalez-Lima F (2005) Behavioral characteristics of rats predisposed to learned helplessness: reduced reward sensitivity, increased novelty seeking, and persistent fear memories. Behav Brain Res 164:222–230
- Bougarel L, Guitton J, Zimmer L et al (2011) Behaviour of a genetic mouse model of depression in the learned helplessness paradigm. Psychopharmacology 215:595–605
- Cai H, Zhang P, Qi G et al (2021) Systematic input-output mapping reveals structural plasticity of VTA dopamine neurons-zona Incerta loop underlying the social buffering effects in learned helplessness. Mol Neurobiol
- Richter SH, Sartorius A, Gass P et al (2014) A matter of timing: harm reduction in learned helplessness. Behav Brain Funct 10:41
- Yang C, Shirayama Y, Zhang J-C, et al (2015) regional differences in brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and dendritic spine density confer resilience to inescapable stress. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 18:yu121
- Yang C, Shirayama Y, Zhang J-C et al (2015) Peripheral interleukin-6 promotes resilience versus susceptibility to inescapable electric stress. Acta Neuropsychiatr 27:312–316
- Perova Z, Delevich K, Li B (2015) Depression of excitatory synapses onto parvalbumin interneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex in susceptibility to stress. J Neurosci 35:3201–3206
- Freda JS, Klein SB (1976) Generality of the failure-to-escape (helplessness) phenomenon in rats. Anim Learn Behav 4:401–406
- Anisman H, Merali Z (2001) Rodent models of depression: learned helplessness induced in mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 8:Unit 8.10C
- Anisman H, Merali Z (2009) Learned helplessness in mice. In: Mood and anxiety related phenotypes in mice. Humana Press, Totowa, NJ, pp 177–196
Advertisement
Learned Helplessness in Rodents
- First Online: 01 October 2022
Cite this protocol
- Kennia Moura Silveira 3 , 4 &
- Sâmia Joca 3 , 5
Part of the book series: Neuromethods ((NM,volume 190))
1133 Accesses
3 Citations
Learned helplessness (LH) is a well-known phenomenon described in animals upon their exposure to uncontrollable and unpredictable aversive stimuli, usually inescapable shocks. It is often characterized by impairment in learning to escape or avoid controllable aversive stimuli in a subsequent behavioral test. In addition to that, previously stressed animals present a range of other behavioral and physiological changes, including cognitive deficits, decreased reward sensitivity (anhedonia), hypolocomotion, sleep and appetite changes, among others. Disturbances in different neurotransmitter systems in cortical and limbic brain regions as well as impaired neuroplasticity have been associated with the development of LH following exposure to an uncontrollable stressor. Since exposure to uncontrollable stress is a risk factor for developing psychiatric disorders and feelings of helplessness are part of the symptomatology in patients suffering from depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), the LH paradigm has been used in animals to study the pathophysiology of such conditions and new treatment options. This chapter summarizes the most relevant discussions about the effects induced by inescapable shock that can be associated with LH development and depression neurobiology. Moreover, we present herein a protocol that can be used to test the effect of different pharmacological interventions and how this can be relevant to studying depression neurobiology.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this chapter
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
- Available as PDF
- Read on any device
- Instant download
- Own it forever
- Available as EPUB and PDF
- Compact, lightweight edition
- Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
- Free shipping worldwide - see info
- Durable hardcover edition
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
Antidepressant activity of nociceptin/orphanin fq receptor antagonists in the mouse learned helplessness.
The Effect of Sericin on the Cognitive Impairment, Depression, and Anxiety Caused by Learned Helplessness in Male Mice
Long-Term Changes in Spontaneous Behavior and c-Fos Expression in the Brain in Mice in the Resting State in a Model of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
Selye H (1950) Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. Br Med J 1:1383–1392
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Selye H (1936) A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 138:32–32
Article Google Scholar
McEwen BS (2008) Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur J Pharmacol 583:174–185
McEwen BS (2006) Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: central role of the brain. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8:367–381
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ (2010) Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1186:190–222
Ehlert U (2013) Enduring psychobiological effects of childhood adversity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38:1850–1857
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Söderlund J, Lindskog M (2018) Relevance of rodent models of depression in clinical practice: can we overcome the obstacles in translational neuropsychiatry? Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:668–676
Anderzhanova E, Kirmeier T, Wotjak CT (2017) Animal models in psychiatric research: the RDoC system as a new framework for endophenotype-oriented translational neuroscience. Neurobiol Stress 7:47–56
(2013) Diagnostics, MDPI AG
Google Scholar
McArthur R, Borsini F (2006) Animal models of depression in drug discovery: a historical perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 84:436–452
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Willner P (1986) Validation criteria for animal models of human mental disorders: learned helplessness as a paradigm case. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 10:677–690
Article CAS Google Scholar
Gururajan A, Reif A, Cryan JF et al (2019) The future of rodent models in depression research. Nat Rev Neurosci 20:686–701
Belzung C, Lemoine M (2011) Criteria of validity for animal models of psychiatric disorders: focus on anxiety disorders and depression. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 1:9
Seligman ME, Maier SF (1967) Failure to escape traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol 74:1–9
Overmier JB, Bruce Overmier J, Seligman ME (1967) Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024166
Maier SF, Seligman ME (1976) Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol Gen 105:3
Overmier JB (1968) Interference with avoidance behavior: failure to avoid traumatic shock. J Exp Psychol 78:340–343
Maier SF, Seligman MEP (2016) Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychol Rev 123:349–367
Maier SF (1970) Failure to escape traumatic electric shock: incompatible skeletal-motor responses or learned helplessness? Learn Motiv 1:157–169
Weiss JM, Glazer HI (1975) Effects of acute exposure to stressors on subsequent avoidance-escape behavior. Psychosom Med 37:499–521
Glazer HI, Weiss JM (1976) Long-term interference effect: an alternative to learned helplessness. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2:202
Jackson RL, Alexander JH, Maier SF (1980) Learned helplessness, inactivity, and associative deficits: effects of inescapable shock on response choice escape learning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 6:1–20
Minor TR, Jackson RL, Maier SF (1984) Effects of task-irrelevant cues and reinforcement delay on choice-escape learning following inescapable shock: evidence for a deficit in selective attention. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 10:543–556
Rosellini RA, DeCola JP, Shapiro NR (1982) Cross-motivational effects of inescapable shock are associative in nature. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 8:376–388
Maier SF, Minor TR (1993) Dissociation of interference with the speed and accuracy of escape produced by inescapable shock. Behav Neurosci 107:139–146
Lee RK, Maier SF (1988) Inescapable shock and attention to internal versus external cues in a water discrimination escape task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 14:302–310
Minor TR, Trauner MA, Lee CY et al (1990) Modeling signal features of escape response: effects of cessation conditioning in “learned helplessness” paradigm. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 16:123–136
Amat J, Paul E, Zarza C et al (2006) Previous experience with behavioral control over stress blocks the behavioral and dorsal raphe nucleus activating effects of later uncontrollable stress: role of the ventral medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 26:13264–13272
Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV et al (2006) Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8:397–406
Abrahamsen GC, Stock HS, Caldarone BJ et al (1993) Learned helplessness inducing foot shock can exacerbate morphine responsiveness. Physiol Behav 54:289–294
Sutton LC, Lea SE, Will MJ et al (1997) Inescapable shock-induced potentiation of morphine analgesia. Behav Neurosci 111:1105–1113
Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Sigrist H et al (2012) Establishing a learned-helplessness effect paradigm in C57BL/6 mice: behavioural evidence for emotional, motivational and cognitive effects of aversive uncontrollability per se. Neuropharmacology 62:358–372
Caldarone BJ, George TP, Zachariou V et al (2000) Gender differences in learned helplessness behavior are influenced by genetic background. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 66:811–817
Teixeira NA, Pereira DG, Hermini AH (1997) Effects of naltrexone and cross-tolerance to morphine in a learned helplessness paradigm. Braz J Med Biol Res 30:775–782
Rojas-Corrales MO, Berrocoso E, Gibert-Rahola J et al (2002) Antidepressant-like effects of tramadol and other central analgesics with activity on monoamines reuptake, in helpless rats. Life Sci 72:143–152
Vollmayr B, Gass P (2013) Learned helplessness: unique features and translational value of a cognitive depression model. Cell Tissue Res 354:171–178
Pryce CR, Azzinnari D, Spinelli S et al (2011) Helplessness: a systematic translational review of theory and evidence for its relevance to understanding and treating depression. Pharmacol Ther 132:242–267
Maier SF (2001) Exposure to the stressor environment prevents the temporal dissipation of behavioral depression/learned helplessness. Biol Psychiatry 49:763–773
Desan PH, Silbert LH, Maier SF (1988) Long-term effects of inescapable stress on daily running activity and antagonism by desipramine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 30:21–29
Musty RE, Jordan MP, Lenox RH (1990) Criterion for learned helplessness in the rat: a redefinition. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 36:739–744
Seligman ME, Beagley G (1975) Learned helplessness in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 88:534–541
Saylor CF, Finch AJ Jr, Cassel SC et al (1984) Learned helplessness: effects of noncontingent reinforcement and response cost with emotionally disturbed children. J Psychol 117:189–196
Abramson LY, Seligman ME, Teasdale JD (1978) Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol 87:49–74
Chourbaji S, Zacher C, Sanchis-Segura C et al (2005) Learned helplessness: validity and reliability of depressive-like states in mice. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 16:70–78
Vollmayr B, Henn FA (2001) Learned helplessness in the rat: improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 8:1–7
Batsching S, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (2016) Inescapable stress changes walking behavior in flies – learned helplessness revisited. PLoS One 11:e0167066
Demin KA, Lakstygal AM, Chernysh MV et al (2020) The zebrafish tail immobilization (ZTI) test as a new tool to assess stress-related behavior and a potential screen for drugs affecting despair-like states. J Neurosci Methods 337:108637
Baumgarten HG, Göthert M (2012) Serotoninergic neurons and 5-HT receptors in the CNS. Springer
Graeff FG, Guimarães FS, De Andrade TG et al (1996) Role of 5-HT in stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 54:129–141
Maier SF, Watkins LR (2005) Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: the roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:829–841
Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E et al (2005) Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci 8:365–371
Koike H, Iijima M, Chaki S (2011) Involvement of AMPA receptor in both the rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects of ketamine in animal models of depression. Behav Brain Res 224:107–111
Marques DB, Ruggiero RN, Bueno-Junior LS et al (2021) Prediction of learned resistance or helplessness by hippocampal-prefrontal cortical network activity during stress. J Neurosci
Hajszan T, Dow A, Warner-Schmidt JL et al (2009) Remodeling of hippocampal spine synapses in the rat learned helplessness model of depression. Biol Psychiatry 65:392–400
Ryan BK, Vollmayr B, Klyubin I et al (2010) Persistent inhibition of hippocampal long-term potentiation in vivo by learned helplessness stress. Hippocampus 20:758–767
Shors TJ (2004) Learning during stressful times. Learn Mem 11:137–144
Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK et al (2003) Central mechanisms of stress integration: hierarchical circuitry controlling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroendocrinol 24:151–180
Huzian O, Baka J, Csakvari E et al (2021) Stress resilience is associated with hippocampal synaptoprotection in the female rat learned helplessness paradigm. Neuroscience 459:85–103
Aznar S, Klein AB, Santini MA et al (2010) Aging and depression vulnerability interaction results in decreased serotonin innervation associated with reduced BDNF levels in hippocampus of rats bred for learned helplessness. Synapse 64:561–565
Bland ST, Tamlyn JP, Barrientos RM et al (2007) Expression of fibroblast growth factor-2 and brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus after uncontrollable or controllable stress. Neuroscience 144:1219–1228
Schulte-Herbrüggen O, Hellweg R, Chourbaji S et al (2007) Differential regulation of neurotrophins and serotonergic function in mice with genetically reduced glucocorticoid receptor expression. Exp Neurol 204:307–316
Vollmayr B, Faust H, Lewicka S et al (2001) Brain-derived-neurotrophic-factor (BDNF) stress response in rats bred for learned helplessness. Mol Psychiatry 6(471–4):358
Cheng Y, Pardo M, Armini R de S et al (2016) Stress-induced neuroinflammation is mediated by GSK3-dependent TLR4 signaling that promotes susceptibility to depression-like behavior. Brain Behav Immun 53:207–222
Kubera M, Obuchowicz E, Goehler L et al (2011) In animal models, psychosocial stress-induced (neuro)inflammation, apoptosis and reduced neurogenesis are associated to the onset of depression. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35:744–759
Woodburn SC, Bollinger JL, Wohleb ES (2021) The semantics of microglia activation: neuroinflammation, homeostasis, and stress. J Neuroinflammation 18:258
McKinney WT Jr, Bunney WE Jr (1969) Animal model of depression. I. Review of evidence: implications for research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 21:240–248
Willner P (1984) The validity of animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology 83:1–16
Murua VS, Gomez RA, Andrea ME et al (1991) Shuttle-box deficits induced by chronic variable stress: reversal by imipramine administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 38:125–130
Tennen H, Gillen R, Drum PE (1982) The debilitating effect of exposure to noncontingent escape: a test of the learned helplessness model. J Pers 50:409–425
Lester D (1998) Helplessness, hopelessness, and haplessness and suicidality. Psychol Rep 82:946
Leenaars L, Lester D (2007) Construct validity of the helplessness/hopelessness/haplessness scale: correlations with perfectionism and depression
Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA (2006) Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major depression in men. Am J Psychiatry 163:115–124
Ho TC, King LS (2021) Mechanisms of neuroplasticity linking early adversity to depression: developmental considerations. Transl Psychiatry 11:517
Zacharko RM, Bowers WJ, Kokkinidis L et al (1983) Region-specific reductions of intracranial self-stimulation after uncontrollable stress: possible effects on reward processes. Behav Brain Res 9:129–141
Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R, Henn FA et al (2005) Reduced sensitivity to sucrose in rats bred for helplessness: a study using the matching law. Behav Pharmacol 16:267–270
Park S-C, Kim Y-K (2018) Depression in DSM-5: changes, controversies, and future directions. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6577-4_1
Seligman MEP, Weiss JM, Weinraub M et al (1980) Part I: introduction. Behav Res Ther 18:459–461
Adrien J, Dugovic C, Martin P (1991) Sleep-wakefulness patterns in the helpless rat. Physiol Behav 49:257–262
Haracz JL, Minor TR, Wilkins JN et al (1988) Learned helplessness: an experimental model of the DST in rats. Biol Psychiatry 23:388–396
Maier SF, Nguyen KT, Deak T et al (1999) Stress, learned helplessness, and brain interleukin-1 beta. Adv Exp Med Biol 461:235–249
Chen L, Wang X, Zhang Y et al (2021) Daidzein alleviates hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal Axis hyperactivity, ameliorates depression-like behavior, and partly rectifies circulating cytokine imbalance in two rodent models of depression. Front Behav Neurosci 15:671864
Sherman AD, Sacquitne JL, Petty F (1982) Specificity of the learned helplessness model of depression. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16:449–454
Highland JN, Morris PJ, Zanos P et al (2019) Mouse, rat, and dog bioavailability and mouse oral antidepressant efficacy of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine. J Psychopharmacol 33:12–24
Sartorius A, Meyer-Lindenberg A (2009) Deep brain stimulation of the lateral habenula to treat depression. Front Neurosci 3:272
Azis IA, Hashioka S, Tsuchie K et al (2019) Electroconvulsive shock restores the decreased coverage of brain blood vessels by astrocytic endfeet and ameliorates depressive-like behavior. J Affect Disord 257:331–339
Nijssen A, Schelvis PR (1987) Effect of an anti-anxiety drug in a learned helplessness experiment. Neuropsychobiology 18:195–198
Morais M, Patrício P, Mateus-Pinheiro A et al (2017) The modulation of adult neuroplasticity is involved in the mood-improving actions of atypical antipsychotics in an animal model of depression. Transl Psychiatry 7:e1146
Paula Soares V de, Vicente MA, Biojone C, et al (2011) Distinct behavioral consequences of stress models of depression in the elevated T-maze. Behav Brain Res 225:590–595
Wieland S, Boren JL, Consroe PF et al (1986) Stock differences in the susceptibility of rats to learned helplessness training. Life Sci 39:937–944
Takase LF, Nogueira MI, Bland ST et al (2005) Effect of number of tailshocks on learned helplessness and activation of serotonergic and noradrenergic neurons in the rat. Behav Brain Res 162:299–306
Joca SRL, Padovan CM, Guimarães FS (2003) Activation of post-synaptic 5-HT(1A) receptors in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development. Brain Res 978:177–184
Joca SRL, Zanelati T, Guimarães FS (2006) Post-stress facilitation of serotonergic, but not noradrenergic, neurotransmission in the dorsal hippocampus prevents learned helplessness development in rats. Brain Res 1087:67–74
Stanquini LA, Biojone C, Guimarães FS et al (2018) Repeated treatment with nitric oxide synthase inhibitor attenuates learned helplessness development in rats and increases hippocampal BDNF expression. Acta Neuropsychiatr 30:127–136
Sales AJ, Maciel IS, Suavinha ACDR et al (2021) Modulation of DNA methylation and gene expression in rodent cortical neuroplasticity pathways exerts rapid antidepressant-like effects. Mol Neurobiol 58:777–794
Sousa Maciel I de, Sales AJ, Casarotto PC, et al (2020) Nitric Oxide Synthase inhibition counteracts the stress-induced DNA methyltransferase 3b expression in the hippocampus of rats. Eur J Neurosci
Geoffroy M, Christensen AV (1993) Psychomotor stimulants versus antidepressants in the learned helplessness model of depression. Drug Dev Res 29:48–55
Sales AJ, Fogaça MV, Sartim AG et al (2019) Cannabidiol induces rapid and sustained antidepressant-like effects through increased BDNF signaling and synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex. Mol Neurobiol 56:1070–1081
Sales AJ, Joca SRL (2018) Antidepressant administration modulates stress-induced DNA methylation and DNA methyltransferase expression in rat prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Behav Brain Res 343:8–15
Martin P, Soubrie P, Simon P (1987) The effect of monoamine oxidase inhibitors compared with classical tricyclic antidepressants on learned helplessness paradigm. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 11:1–7
Petty F, Sacquitne JL, Sherman AD (1982) Tricyclic antidepressant drug action correlates with its tissue levels in anterior neocortex. Neuropharmacology 21:475–477
Ramaker MJ, Dulawa SC (2017) Identifying fast-onset antidepressants using rodent models. Mol Psychiatry 22:656–665
Shirayama Y, Hashimoto K (2018) Lack of antidepressant effects of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine in a rat learned helplessness model: comparison with (R)-ketamine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:84–88
Antila H, Ryazantseva M, Popova D et al (2017) Isoflurane produces antidepressant effects and induces TrkB signaling in rodents. Sci Rep 7:7811
Hamani C, Diwan M, Macedo CE et al (2010) Antidepressant-like effects of medial prefrontal cortex deep brain stimulation in rats. Biol Psychiatry 67:117–124
Creed MC, Hamani C, Nobrega JN (2013) Effects of repeated deep brain stimulation on depressive- and anxiety-like behavior in rats: comparing entopeduncular and subthalamic nuclei. Brain Stimul 6:506–514
Han S, Yang SH, Kim JY et al (2017) Down-regulation of cholinergic signaling in the habenula induces anhedonia-like behavior. Sci Rep 7:900
Urbanavicius J, Fabius S, Roncalho A et al (2019) Melanin-concentrating hormone in the Locus Coeruleus aggravates helpless behavior in stressed rats. Behav Brain Res 374:112120
Nobrega JN, Hedayatmofidi PS, Lobo DS (2016) Strong interactions between learned helplessness and risky decision-making in a rat gambling model. Sci Rep 6:37304
Dalla C, Edgecomb C, Whetstone AS et al (2008) Females do not express learned helplessness like males do. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:1559–1569
Ribeiro DE, Casarotto PC, Staquini L et al (2019) Reduced P2X receptor levels are associated with antidepressant effect in the learned helplessness model. PeerJ 7:e7834
Drugan RC, Basile AS, Ha JH et al (1997) Analysis of the importance of controllable versus uncontrollable stress on subsequent behavioral and physiological functioning. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 2:69–74
Landgraf D, Long J, Der-Avakian A et al (2015) Dissociation of learned helplessness and fear conditioning in mice: a mouse model of depression. PLoS One 10:e0125892
Baratta MV, Christianson JP, Gomez DM et al (2007) Controllable versus uncontrollable stressors bi-directionally modulate conditioned but not innate fear. Neuroscience 146:1495–1503
Shumake J, Barrett D, Gonzalez-Lima F (2005) Behavioral characteristics of rats predisposed to learned helplessness: reduced reward sensitivity, increased novelty seeking, and persistent fear memories. Behav Brain Res 164:222–230
Bougarel L, Guitton J, Zimmer L et al (2011) Behaviour of a genetic mouse model of depression in the learned helplessness paradigm. Psychopharmacology 215:595–605
Cai H, Zhang P, Qi G et al (2021) Systematic input-output mapping reveals structural plasticity of VTA dopamine neurons-zona Incerta loop underlying the social buffering effects in learned helplessness. Mol Neurobiol
Richter SH, Sartorius A, Gass P et al (2014) A matter of timing: harm reduction in learned helplessness. Behav Brain Funct 10:41
Yang C, Shirayama Y, Zhang J-C, et al (2015) regional differences in brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and dendritic spine density confer resilience to inescapable stress. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 18:yu121
Yang C, Shirayama Y, Zhang J-C et al (2015) Peripheral interleukin-6 promotes resilience versus susceptibility to inescapable electric stress. Acta Neuropsychiatr 27:312–316
Perova Z, Delevich K, Li B (2015) Depression of excitatory synapses onto parvalbumin interneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex in susceptibility to stress. J Neurosci 35:3201–3206
Freda JS, Klein SB (1976) Generality of the failure-to-escape (helplessness) phenomenon in rats. Anim Learn Behav 4:401–406
Anisman H, Merali Z (2001) Rodent models of depression: learned helplessness induced in mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 8:Unit 8.10C
Anisman H, Merali Z (2009) Learned helplessness in mice. In: Mood and anxiety related phenotypes in mice. Humana Press, Totowa, NJ, pp 177–196
Chapter Google Scholar
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
School of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirao Preto, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Kennia Moura Silveira & Sâmia Joca
Department of Clinical Medicine, Translational Neuropsychiatry Unit, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Kennia Moura Silveira
Department of Biomedicine, Aarhus, Denmark
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Sâmia Joca .
Editor information
Editors and affiliations.
Chair of Neuropsychopharmacology, Institute of Chemistry, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia
Jaanus Harro
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
Copyright information
© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature
About this protocol
Silveira, K.M., Joca, S. (2023). Learned Helplessness in Rodents. In: Harro, J. (eds) Psychiatric Vulnerability, Mood, and Anxiety Disorders. Neuromethods, vol 190. Humana, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_9
Download citation
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-0716-2748-8_9
Published : 01 October 2022
Publisher Name : Humana, New York, NY
Print ISBN : 978-1-0716-2747-1
Online ISBN : 978-1-0716-2748-8
eBook Packages : Springer Protocols
- Publish with us
Policies and ethics
- Find a journal
- Track your research
Learned helplessness in the rat: effect of response topography in a within-subject design
Affiliation.
- 1 Universidad de Guadalajara, Mexico. [email protected]
- PMID: 21129457
- DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.11.005
Three experiments investigated learned helplessness in rats manipulating response topography within-subject and different intervals between treatment and tests among groups. In Experiment 1, rats previously exposed to inescapable shocks were tested under an escape contingency where either jumping or nose poking was required to terminate shocks; tests were run either 1, 14 or 28 days after treatment. Most rats failed to jump, as expected, but learned to nose poke, regardless of the interval between treatment and tests and order of testing. The same results were observed in male and female rats from a different laboratory (Experiment 2) and despite increased exposure to the escape contingencies using a within-subject design (Experiment 3). Furthermore, no evidence of helplessness reversal was observed, since animals failed to jump even after having learned to nose-poke in a previous test session. These results are not consistent with a learned helplessness hypothesis, which claims that shock (un)controllability is the key variable responsible for the effect. They are nonetheless consistent with the view that inescapable shocks enhance control by irrelevant features of the relationship between the environment and behavior.
Copyright © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Publication types
- Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
- Behavior, Animal / physiology*
- Conditioning, Operant
- Data Interpretation, Statistical
- Electroshock
- Escape Reaction / physiology
- Helplessness, Learned*
- Motor Activity / physiology
- Rats, Wistar
- Reinforcement Schedule
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Skinner observed that they were likely to engage in this behavior again, anticipating food. In some boxes, punishments would also be administered. Martin Seligman's learned helplessness experiments are a great example of using punishments to observe or shape an animal's behavior. Skinner usually worked with animals like rats or pigeons.
The first variable is gender: Yano and Hunziker's rats were female, while ours were male, and there is evidence that, in tests of learned helplessness with the jumping response, gender may interact with some variables (like drugs or shock unpredictability - Gouveia, 2001) in determining the learned helplessness effect. Second, their rats and ...
Four experiments attempted to produce behavior in the rat parallel to the behavior characteristic of learned helplessness in the dog. When rats received escapable, inescapable, or no shock and were later tested in jump-up escape, both inescapable and no-shock controls failed to escape. When bar pres …
4 experiments, using a total of 159 male albino Sprague-Dawley rats, attempted to produce behavior in the rat parallel to the behavior characteristic of learned helplessness in the dog. When Ss received escapable, inescapable, or no shock and were later tested in jump-up escape, both inescapable and no-shock controls failed to escape. When barpressing, rather than jumping up, was used as the ...
Shirayama Y, Hashimoto K (2018) Lack of antidepressant effects of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine in a rat learned helplessness model: comparison with (R)-ketamine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 21:84-88 ... Maier SF, Seligman ME (1976) Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. J Exp Psychol Gen 105:3 Overmier JB (1968) Interference with avoidance ...
Three experiments investigated learned helplessness in rats manipulating response requirements, shock duration, and intervals between treatment and testing. In Experiment 1, rats previously exposed to uncontrollable or no shocks were tested under one of four different contingencies of negative reinf …
parallel phenomena in cats, fish, rats, and man. Of particular interest is the discussion of learned helplessness in rats and man. Rats are of interest because learned helplessness has been difficult to demonstrate in rats. However, we show that inescapably shocked rats do fail to learn to escape if the escape task is rea-sonably difficult.
The phenomenon of learned helplessness also occurs in many species, such as dogs, rats, and mice [15, 44, 45], and has been translated even to non-mammals, such as zebra fish and flies [46, 47]. Most of the work has been developed in rats, although more recently mice have been widely used as well, with the advantage of testing transgenic ...
The previous experiments showed that learned helplessness can be obtained with rats under FR 1 on a task that isolated the operant nature of the response. This experiment was designed to assess the effects of the passage of time on learned helplessness, using a clearly operant task.
Three experiments investigated learned helplessness in rats manipulating response topography within-subject and different intervals between treatment and tests among groups. In Experiment 1, rats previously exposed to inescapable shocks were tested under an escape contingency where either jumping or nose poking was required to terminate shocks ...